Skip to main content

Case Presentation

A 60-year-old right-handed man, who was a retired electrical engineer, presented to our clinic after 2 years of insidious-onset progressive word-finding difficulties, as well as an observed decline in verbal output (1). He described his symptoms as “trouble capturing thoughts in my mind” and “losing words in my head.” He reported problems with complex decision making and denied difficulties with language comprehension, memory, and tasks involving visuospatial reasoning. He and his wife noted mild social withdrawal, increased frustration, and “trying less” in most activities over the previous 2 years. There were no personality changes, signs of current major depression, or significant apathy. He was successfully driving, playing golf, and managing household finances and otherwise independent in basic and instrumental activities of daily living (ADLs).
The patient’s past medical history included hypertension and major depressive disorder. His medications included olmesartan and escitalopram. Social and developmental history were negative for dyslexia and other learning disabilities. He completed college with a degree in electrical engineering and worked as a systems consultant, retiring 3 years prior to symptom onset at age 55, with no cognitive difficulties at work. There was no history of substance use. His family history was notable for Parkinson’s disease in his brother and coronary artery disease.
Question: What are diagnostic considerations based on history alone?
The history of insidious onset of cognitive (language) symptoms, which worsened progressively over 2 years in our patient, is most suggestive of a neurodegenerative disease. Alzheimer’s disease and the frontotemporal dementias are the most common diseases when symptom onset occurs before age 65. Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease is more likely than late-onset Alzheimer’s disease to present with primary cognitive dysfunction in nonmemory domains, such as language or executive function (2). Neuroimaging will rule out unlikely causes like an unusual, slow-growing focal lesion that could cause highly circumscribed verbal deficits.
Neurocognitive assessment of affected domains of cognition and behavior allows us to characterize symptoms according to known cognitive syndromes, initially in a “neuropathology blind” way. Identifying a known cognitive syndrome allows the initial generation of a neuropathologic differential diagnosis based on already identified clinical-pathologic correlates.
Assessing the degree to which symptoms affect daily functioning can help differentiate between prodromal and later stages of neurodegenerative disease. Our patient presented with intact functioning in his instrumental and basic ADLs and with objective impairments on cognitive testing, placing him at the level of mild cognitive impairment, synonymous with mild neurocognitive disorder. If he had no objective deficits, his syndrome would have been considered subjective cognitive decline, which in some instances is a risk factor for developing mild cognitive impairment and later impairments in daily functioning (3).
Our patient’s primary reported complaints were in the domain of language, with a history that suggests relative preservation of memory, attention, executive function, and visuospatial abilities. Generally, history of initial decline in one or more domains of language production is concerning for primary progressive aphasia (PPA), a group of degenerative language syndromes for which clinical-radiologic-pathologic comparative studies have identified three variants with formalized diagnostic criteria and difficult-to-classify cases variously called “atypical” or “mixed” (46). Variants differ in aspects of language affected and in most common underlying neuropathology.
The three canonical variants include nonfluent-agrammatic variant PPA (nfvPPA), semantic variant PPA (svPPA), and logopenic variant PPA (lvPPA) (46). In nfvPPA, defining linguistic deficits include agrammatism, effortful and halting speech (with phrase length typically less than six words) with or without apraxia of speech, and neuroanatomic correlates of atrophy and/or hypometabolism in the left inferior frontal lobe, often in the posterior fronto-insular cortex. In svPPA, defining linguistic deficits include impaired single-word comprehension and naming, with typical neuroanatomic correlates of bilateral and left-greater-than-right atrophy and/or hypometabolism in the anterior temporal lobes. In lvPPA, defining linguistic deficits include impaired word retrieval and phrase or sentence repetition, with left posterior perisylvian/parietal atrophy and/or hypometabolism (69). There is heterogeneity within subtypes and ongoing debate regarding core defining criteria (9, 10). Figure 1 depicts a simplified, diagnostic decision tree (8).
FIGURE 1. A clinical “roadmap” for diagnosis of canonical primary progressive aphasia syndromes, synthesizing key features and indicating major decision pointsa
alvPPA=logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia; nfvPPA=nonfluent-agrammatic variant primary progressive aphasia; PPA=primary progressive aphasia; svPPA=semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Figure reproduced with permission from Marshall et al. (8); PMID: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29392464/. Reprinted without change, under terms of Creative Commons Attribution, 4.0, International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0).
Up to 40% of cases may not clearly meet consensus criteria for the three canonical variants, and symptoms may be “mixed” across variants or are “atypical,” involving specific language dysfunction not represented in criteria for canonical variants (1113).
Question: How does the neurocognitive and physical examination contribute to our understanding of diagnostic considerations?
Neurocognitive testing within the language domain begins with observation of spontaneous language output to assess fluency, grammar, word-finding pauses, and speech errors. Formal assessment of speech praxis, expressive language output, verbal fluency (letter- and category-based), motor speech, naming, repetition, and comprehension follows. At initial evaluation, 2 years into the disease course, our patient’s language impairment was notable for variability in word-finding pauses and overall sparse speech output, especially with spontaneous narrative generation. His speech was grammatical, with no apraxia of speech or dysarthria. Verbal fluency was impaired, with phonemic fluency weaker than category-based fluency (for further details, see video S1 in the online supplement). Design fluency was relatively intact, suggesting that verbal fluency deficits are more related to language impairment than to executive dysfunction. Confrontation naming was also relatively intact, despite word-finding pauses. Repetition, partially dependent on auditory verbal working memory, and basic comprehension were intact. The patient displayed intact decoding, reading comprehension, and written language abilities. Additionally, there were limitations in his attention and executive functioning (see Tables S1 and S2 in the online supplement).
Elemental neurologic examination had notable absence of involuntary movements, dystonia, Parkinsonism, and changes in extraocular motility. If present, these findings increase suspicion for underlying frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD), especially progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) and corticobasal (CBD) degeneration.
The patient’s examination thus far revealed a salient language output deficit and a normal neurologic examination. Given worsening of language impairment over time, with relative preservation of other cognitive domains, his syndrome was most suggestive of a PPA. Considering the canonical PPA variants, his pattern of fluent grammatical speech with intact motor aspects of speech was not suggestive of nfvPPA. Intact confrontation naming and sentence repetition made lvPPA less appropriate, even when applying an expanded conception to allow for normal repetition and prominent word retrieval and naming deficits (14). Furthermore, his intact naming and single-word comprehension (as shown through his ability to name pictures, a task that indexes semantic memory) were incompatible with svPPA. Symptoms did not meet consensus criteria, nor did they meet expanded conceptions for the three canonical PPA variants, “mixed” PPA, or “anomic” PPA (4, 6, 15). We could conceptualize this case as an “atypical” or “unclassifiable” PPA (8, 16, 17). However, this specific syndrome has been previously described in the context of neurodegeneration and of lesions to the left prefrontal cortex from neoplasm and infarction and termed “dynamic” or transcortical motor aphasia (1, 18).
Question: Can we more specifically localize the deficits? What additional tests or studies are indicated? How do these tests guide prediction of clinical-pathologic correlation?
Considering the examination findings of a relatively circumscribed language output deficit, we hypothesized a disruption in the region of the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and its connections, which are involved in recruitment of attention and executive control networks necessary for initiation of language (19). The SFG includes the supplementary motor area, which—when on the language dominant hemisphere—is thought to subserve initiation/activation, planning, and coordination of language via a fiber tract to the inferior frontal gyrus (20). Given findings of preserved grammar, motor aspects of speech, and auditory verbal working memory, we might expect sparing of the inferior frontal gyrus, insula, temporal lobes, and white matter tracts connecting these regions (7, 8).
Neuroimaging can be helpful to interrogate hypothesis-driven anatomical localization that explains observed patterns of impairment. Brain MRI allows for visualization of focal or segmental patterns of atrophy. The clinical read of our patient’s initial brain MRI suggested minimal global volume loss, with slight predominance in the left frontal and parietal cortices (see Figure S1 in the online supplement). Our initial follow-up FreeSurfer-calculated cortical thickness analysis demonstrated reduced volume in left-greater-than-right midcaudal superior frontal and caudal middle frontal gyri, compared with 20 healthy, age-matched control subjects (1). Additional analyses concluded that volume loss was isolated to the left SFG.
Positron emission tomography (PET) imaging using intravenous injection of [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG-PET) may provide further information about functional abnormalities, which are often observable prior to the development of brain atrophy (21). Distinct hypometabolic patterns have been observed for the three canonical PPA variants and may also advance neuropathologic differential diagnosis (6). Brain single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) also could be considered. While some studies demonstrate higher sensitivities and specificities for FDG-PET than SPECT, others have shown equivalence, and SPECT may be a more accessible test to obtain in some centers (22, 23).
Our patient’s FDG-PET scan demonstrates hypometabolism in the left SFG, with preserved metabolism elsewhere (Figure 2), consistent with the early atrophy pattern seen on MRI and with the patient’s syndrome. While this is not a typical pattern for a canonical PPA syndrome, isolated frontal lobe hypometabolism is most suggestive of FTLD pathology.
FIGURE 2. Brain [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography 2 years after symptom onset (at age 60) showing circumscribed left anterior frontal hypometabolisma
aPanel A shows an axial view of the patient’s brain (black/white). Panel B shows a color version of the axial view (green/yellow is reduced uptake). Panel C shows a sagittal view of the brain, emphasizing superior aspect of left frontal hypometabolism. Purple circles indicate anterior frontal hypometabolism. L=left; R=right.
Other currently available tests that could be considered are amyloid- and tau-based molecular PET scans and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers, which are used primarily in clinical settings to detect Alzheimer’s disease pathology. We obtained Alzheimer’s disease CSF biomarkers to guide more predictive clinical-pathologic correlation given the atypical PPA variant, early age at onset, and absence of other examinations or neuroimaging results.
CSF analysis by enzyme-linked immunoassay for beta-amyloid 1-42 (Aβ42), total tau (t-tau), and phosphorylated tau181 (p-tau181), which are associated with Alzheimer’s disease pathology, revealed Aβ42 of 980 pg/mL, t-tau of 201.2 pg/mL, and p-tau181 of 45.8 pg/mL. The levels of each protein were within normal limits (24). We still lack robust CSF biomarkers to identify forms of FTLD, the most common subtypes of which involve pathological inclusions of the protein tau (i.e., FTLD-tau) or the protein transactive response DNA-binding protein 43 kDa (TDP-43) (i.e., FTLD-TDP). Primary tauopathies may or may not show elevations in total CSF tau.
Comparing findings to those of the described PPA syndromes and their common underlying pathologies can help narrow the neuropathological differential diagnosis in an atypical case. Any PPA case can be caused by single or multiple pathologic subtypes (9, 25). The most common neuropathologies that underly PPA are FTLD (associated with nfvPPA and svPPA) and Alzheimer’s disease (associated with lvPPA). Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and prion disease are rare causes (26, 27). In our patient, Alzheimer’s disease pathology was unlikely given the distribution of atrophy and hypometabolism, as well as the results of CSF Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers. There were no clinical or radiologic features suggestive of DLB or prion disease; FTLD was the most likely pathology.
Cases of dynamic aphasia have been associated with the classical clinical syndrome of PSP (Richardson syndrome), as well as with neuropathologic findings of FTLD-PSP, a pathologic subtype of FTLD-tau (2830). However, as noted above, our patient lacked secondary motor symptoms and signs suggestive of FTLD-PSP. A strongly asymmetric pattern of frontal hypometabolism with FDG-PET has been associated with FTLD-tau subtype Pick’s disease (FTLD-PiD), though many cases involve both frontal and temporal cortices (31, 32).
Question: Considering these additional data, what would be an appropriate diagnostic formulation?
Here, we have a case of insidious, slowly progressive language impairment, with salient deficits in verbal output, consistent with prior descriptions of “dynamic” aphasia, an “atypical” PPA syndrome, at the severity level of mild cognitive impairment.
Symptoms and examination findings broadly suggest disruption of the left dorsal prefrontal cortex, and imaging and biomarkers more precisely support this localization, revealing focal MRI atrophy and circumscribed FDG-PET hypometabolism in the left SFG. Given overlap in the anatomic distribution of “dynamic aphasia” and nfvPPA (which is most often caused by FTLD-tau), we predict underlying FTLD-tau neuropathology, either FTLD-PSP or FTLD-PiD subtypes.
Question: How does longitudinal course of the illness inform predictive clinical-pathologic correlation of underlying neuropathological processes?
The disease ran its course over 12 years. Three years after symptom onset, the patient’s speech became more sparse and executive functioning and attention deficits more profound. This suggests pathologic spread—likely anteriorly and inferiorly—to areas further subserving speech output and language and executive control networks (19, 20, 33, 34). Concurrently, the patient started to experience frustration and anger more easily and was noted to be more socially withdrawn. Serial MRI reflected progression of atrophy to involve bilateral frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes, with some predominance in left-greater-than-right frontal and temporal lobes (Figure 3).
FIGURE 3. Brain MRI at years 2, 4, and 8 after symptom onseta
aThe images are coronal T1-weighted sequences illustrating severe bifrontal (left-dominant) atrophy and “knife-like” gyri by year 8.
By year 6, his symptoms progressed to include apraxia of speech and other features seen in nfvPPA. His speech was more effortful, more dysfluent with sound errors, and developed a “groping” quality. He started to have grammatical errors, first in writing and then in speech. Comprehension of syntactically complex material, naming, and repetition also declined. Behaviorally, he developed symptoms that overlap with behavioral variant FTD (bvFTD) syndrome, including apathy, hyperorality, and disinhibition (31). He became dependent in instrumental ADLs.
MRI in year 8 demonstrated profound frontal lobar atrophy, mildly asymmetric (left greater than right), with “knife-edge” configuration of gyri (Figure 3C). In year 9, the patient could not repeat his name and was dependent in basic ADLs. Two years later, he developed extrapyramidal symptoms and was moved to a nursing home. By year 12, he was mute and exhibited no ability for language comprehension. During that year, he contracted COVID-19 and died of complications related to this illness.
Considering the evolving features of nfvPPA and bvFTD our patient developed, it is common in FTDs to see syndromic overlap, especially with disease progression. Common neuropathological associations with nfvPPA include different subtypes of FTLD-tau (CBD, PSP, and PiD) and FTLD-TDP type A. That he did not develop other neurologic signs or symptoms commonly seen in PSP or CBD reduces the likelihood of underlying FTLD-PSP or FTLD-CBD. While not exclusive to one pathology, the aforementioned pattern of atrophy with asymmetrical bilateral frontal, temporal, and parietal atrophy and gross “knife-edge” appearance is most consistent with the evolution of atrophy seen in FTLD-PiD (32, 35). Taken all together, these syndromic and imaging changes, over time, are most suggestive of the underlying FTLD-tau subtype FTLD-PiD.

Neuropathology

Postmortem examination of the patient’s brain showed a weight of 975 grams, well below the normal range for an adult male (1,200–1,500 grams). On gross examination, there was marked cortical atrophy involving the frontal and anterior temporal lobes, with sharp demarcation from less-involved cortices (see Figure S2 in the online supplement). Coronal sections of the cerebrum revealed marked cortical atrophy of the frontal and temporal lobes, with yellow discoloration of cortices and underlying markedly shrunken white matter. There was atrophy of the basal nuclei involving the caudate, putamen, and globus pallidus. The thalamus and hippocampus were moderately atrophic. The substantia nigra was moderately depigmented, and the locus coeruleus was not visible grossly. The deep nuclei of the cerebellum appeared mildly shrunken and discolored, as did the inferior olives.
Histologic examination showed severe neuronal depletion, spongiosis, and gliosis of atrophic cortical areas (see Figure S3 in the online supplement). Classic intraneuronal round eosinophilic Pick bodies, along with ballooned neurons with abundant glassy cytoplasm, were present in the frontal, temporal, and insular cortices; amygdala; and entorhinal cortex. The hippocampus showed moderate neuronal depletion and Pick bodies, particularly in the dentate granule cells. In addition, granulovacuolar degeneration and Hirano bodies were present. Immunohistochemistry for total tau highlighted the frequent Pick bodies. Total tau protein includes two distinct isoforms, three-repeat (3R) and four-repeat (4R), which are derived from differential splicing of transcripts encoded by the MAPT gene. Tau 3R/4R isoform determination is helpful to distinguish certain neurodegenerative tauopathies, including FTLD-PiD (a 3R tauopathy) and PSP (a 4R tauopathy). We performed immunohistochemistry for 3R tau and 4R tau, which confirmed the presence of 3R-tau inclusions in the Pick bodies, while 4R tau was absent (Figure 4). The ballooned neurons showed dense neurofilament accumulation. There were also frequent astrocytic tau inclusions in severely involved cortices. The underlying white matter showed marked axonal loss and gliosis. The parietal and occipital cortices were relatively to completely spared of diagnostic pathology (neuronal loss or tau-immunoreactive inclusions). No amyloid plaques or Lewy pathology was present, while mild to focally moderate cerebral amyloid angiopathy was present in the leptomeninges of the parietal and occipital lobes.
FIGURE 4. Tau immunohistochemistry for three-repeat (3R) tau and four-repeat (4R) taua
aPanel A illustrates staining for total tau, showing dense spherical brown intracytoplasmic neuronal immunolabeling of Pick bodies (arrows). Panel B illustrates 3R tau immunostaining (RD3 antibody), showing dense brown intracytoplasmic neuronal immunolabeling of Pick bodies (arrow) in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus, while panel C shows that 4R tau immunostaining (RD4 antibody) is negative.
This case was designated by the neuropathology division as FTLD-tau, FTLD-PiD. FTLD pathology can be classified into broad histopathological molecular subtypes of FTLD-tau and FTLD-TDP, each comprising 40%–50% of cases (36). The FTLD-tau group can be further subclassified according to the number of repeats in the predominant isoform of accumulated tau, 3R or 4R. FTLD-PiD is the major 3R tauopathy (37). The 4R tauopathies include CBD, PSP, and argyrophilic grain disease. FTLD caused by mutations in the gene that encodes tau, MAPT, manifests pathologically as tauopathy with a range of 3R/4R status, with the isoform abundance affected by the location of the genetic variant in MAPT (38). Alzheimer’s disease and primary age-related tauopathy, previously known as tangle-only dementia, are also 3R/4R tauopathies.

Discussion

Dynamic aphasia has been described as an impairment along the “thought-verbal interface,” where there is difficulty in translating one’s thoughts into verbal output (18, 33, 39). In our patient, difficulty with word retrieval and intact naming performance suggests dysfunction in executive components of language, such as organizing thoughts into expression. This case validates previously identified structure-function relationships linked to dynamic aphasia and provides insight into initial stages of language generation as being related to broader executive-functioning systems (19). Specifically, in certain models of executive function and working memory, speech output requires the “motivating” of thoughts into speech, which involves some organization (executive control) of the output of language to accurately represent those thoughts (1).
Over time, the clinical features in this case evolved to include symptoms also seen in nfvPPA and bvFTD, reflecting spread to widely distributed components of language and frontal executive-mediated functions. Symptom progression correlates with progression of atrophy seen on serial brain MRIs, starting in the left SFG and moving inferiorly, posteriorly, and bilaterally. In contrast to cases that present initially with nfvPPA, the initial locus of neurodegeneration in our patient was in the left SFG rather than the inferior frontal lobe, as is typical in nfvPPA. Over time, it is likely that pathology spread from the superior lobe to the inferior frontal lobe, which accounts for the development of nfvPPA-like symptoms. This pattern of pathologic spread can vary by subtype of PPA and underlying FTLD (40, 41). As neurodegenerative diseases progress, pathology spreads, and therefore it is useful for clinicians to continue to update their assessments and syndromic classification.
To our knowledge, this is the first report of a primary dynamic aphasia syndrome due to underlying FTLD-PiD. At least two other cases with similar syndromes have been reported to have underlying FTLD-PSP pathology, a 4R tauopathy (29, 30). PiD is a 3R tauopathy with Pick bodies (characteristic argyrophilic neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions), classified under the pathologic “umbrella” of FTLD-tau. Among the canonical PPA clinical syndromes, FTLD-PiD is less common as the dominant underlying pathology. However, in nfvPPA, the frequency of underlying FTLD-PiD is comparable to that of other pathologies, including CBD, PSP, and FTLD-TDP type A (9, 4244).
There is heterogeneity in the clinical syndromes that have a pathologic diagnosis of PiD. These most commonly include bvFTD, followed by nfvPPA and svPPA, and rarely CBD syndrome and amnestic syndrome (4547). In several series, about 60% of FTD-PiD cases identified as postmortem were diagnosed with bvFTD, while around 40% were diagnosed with PPA (47, 48). The better we can become at accurate clinicopathologic correlations during life, the better we are positioned to study and, ultimately, treat patients, perhaps even with disease-modifying therapy aimed to ameliorate specific neuropathology.
However, disease biomarkers themselves will not teach us about brain-behavior relationships. Being able to carefully trace the changing distribution of neuropathology and link it to changing clinical symptomatology will add to our understanding of how the brain works. Identifying the initial focality of neurodegenerative diseases and following its spreading distribution over time are worthy activities for neuropsychiatrists and behavioral neurologists.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the clinical and research teams at Brigham and Women’s Hospital (Division of Neuropathology and Center for Brain/Mind Medicine in the Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology) and Massachusetts General Hospital (Frontotemporal Disorders Unit), who cared for the patient described in this report, as well as involved him and his wife in clinical research over many years.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.neuropsych.22010013.ds001.pdf)
File (appi.neuropsych.22010013.ds002.mp4)

References

1.
Perez DL, Dickerson BC, McGinnis SM, et al: You don’t say: dynamic aphasia, another variant of primary progressive aphasia? J Alzheimers Dis 2013; 34:139–144
2.
Kuruppu DK, Matthews BR: Young-onset dementia. Semin Neurol 2013; 33:365–385
3.
Ismail Z, McGirr A, Gill S, et al: Mild behavioral impairment and subjective cognitive decline predict cognitive and functional decline. J Alzheimers Dis 2021; 80:459–469
4.
Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, et al: Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology 2011; 76:1006–1014
5.
Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, et al: Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 2004; 55:335–346
6.
Vandenberghe R: Classification of the primary progressive aphasias: principles and review of progress since 2011. Alzheimers Res Ther 2016; 8:16
7.
Devenney EM, Ahmed RM, Hodges JR: Frontotemporal dementia; in Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Amsterdam, Elsevier, 2019, pp 279–299
8.
Marshall CR, Hardy CJD, Volkmer A, et al: Primary progressive aphasia: a clinical approach. J Neurol 2018; 265:1474–1490
9.
Mesulam M-M, Weintraub S, Rogalski EJ, et al: Asymmetry and heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal pathology in primary progressive aphasia. Brain 2014; 137:1176–1192
10.
Giannini LAA, Irwin DJ, McMillan CT, et al: Clinical marker for Alzheimer disease pathology in logopenic primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 2017; 88:2276–2284
11.
Mazzeo S, Polito C, Padiglioni S, et al: Linguistic profiles, brain metabolic patterns and rates of amyloid-β biomarker positivity in patients with mixed primary progressive aphasia. Neurobiol Aging 2020; 96:155–164
12.
Schaeverbeke J, Gabel S, Meersmans K, et al: Single-word comprehension deficits in the nonfluent variant of primary progressive aphasia. Alzheimers Res Ther 2018; 10:68
13.
Tippett DC: Classification of primary progressive aphasia: challenges and complexities. F1000Res 2020; 9:64
14.
Mesulam MM, Weintraub S: Is it time to revisit the classification guidelines for primary progressive aphasia? Neurology 2014; 82:1108–1109
15.
Mesulam M: Primary progressive aphasia: a dementia of the language network. Dement Neuropsychol 2013; 7:2–9
16.
Montembeault M, Brambati SM, Gorno-Tempini ML, et al: Clinical, anatomical, and pathological features in the three variants of primary progressive aphasia: a review. Front Neurol 2018; 9:692
17.
Utianski RL, Botha H, Martin PR, et al: Clinical and neuroimaging characteristics of clinically unclassifiable primary progressive aphasia. Brain Lang 2019; 197:104676
18.
Chandregowda A, Clark HM, Duffy JR, et al: Dynamic aphasia as a variant of frontotemporal dementia. Cogn Behav Neurol 2021; 34:303–318
19.
Alexander MP: Impairments of procedures for implementing complex language are due to disruption of frontal attention processes. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006; 12:236–247
20.
Dick AS, Garic D, Graziano P, et al: The frontal aslant tract (FAT) and its role in speech, language and executive function. Cortex 2019; 111:148–163
21.
Bouwman F, Orini S, Gandolfo F, et al: Diagnostic utility of FDG-PET in the differential diagnosis between different forms of primary progressive aphasia. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2018; 45:1526–1533
22.
Ferrando R, Damian A: Brain SPECT as a biomarker of neurodegeneration in dementia in the era of molecular imaging: still a valid option? Front Neurol 2021; 12:629442
23.
Laforce R, Soucy JP, Sellami L, et al: Molecular imaging in dementia: past, present, and future. Alzheimers Dement 2018; 14:1522–1552
24.
Ibach B, Binder H, Dragon M, et al: Cerebrospinal fluid tau and β-amyloid in Alzheimer patients, disease controls and an age-matched random sample. Neurobiol Aging 2006; 27:1202–1211
25.
Santos-Santos MA, Rabinovici GD, Iaccarino L, et al: Rates of amyloid imaging positivity in patients with primary progressive aphasia. JAMA Neurol 2018; 75:342–352
26.
Johnson DY, Dunkelberger DL, Henry M, et al: Sporadic Jakob-Creutzfeldt disease presenting as primary progressive aphasia. JAMA Neurol 2013; 70:254–257
27.
Watanabe H, Ikeda M, Mori E: Primary progressive aphasia as a prodromal state of dementia with Lewy bodies: a case report. Front Neurol 2020; 11:49
28.
Coughlin DG, Litvan I: Progressive supranuclear palsy: advances in diagnosis and management. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2020; 73:105–116
29.
Esmonde T, Giles E, Xuereb J, et al: Progressive supranuclear palsy presenting with dynamic aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1996; 60:403–410
30.
Robinson G, Shallice T, Cipolotti L: Dynamic aphasia in progressive supranuclear palsy: a deficit in generating a fluent sequence of novel thought. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44:1344–1360
31.
Price BH, Perez DL, Rapalino O, et al: Case 41-2020: a 62-year-old man with memory loss and odd behavior. N Engl J Med 2020; 383:2666–2675
32.
Rohrer JD, Lashley T, Schott JM, et al: Clinical and neuroanatomical signatures of tissue pathology in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Brain 2011; 134:2565–2581
33.
Barker MS, Nelson NL, Robinson GA: Idea formulation for spoken language production: the interface of cognition and language. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2020; 26:226–240
34.
Hickok G, Poeppel D: The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007; 8:393–402
35.
Perry DC, Brown JA, Possin KL, et al: Clinicopathological correlations in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 2017; 140:3329–3345
36.
Hernandez I, Fernandez M-V, Tarraga L, et al: Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD): review and update for clinical neurologists. Curr Alzheimer Res 2018; 15:511–530
37.
Irwin DJ, Cairns NJ, Grossman M, et al: Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: defining phenotypic diversity through personalized medicine. Acta Neuropathol 2015; 129:469–491
38.
Sieben A, van Langenhove T, Engelborghs S, et al: The genetics and neuropathology of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Acta Neuropathol 2012; 124:353–372
39.
Luria AR, Tsvetkova LS: Towards the mechanisms of “dynamic aphasia”. Acta Neurol Psychiatr Belg 1967; 67:1045–1057
40.
Rogalski EJ, Mesulam MM: Clinical trajectories and biological features of primary progressive aphasia (PPA). Curr Alzheimer Res 2009; 6:331–336
41.
Ulugut H, Stek S, Wagemans L, et al: The natural history of primary progressive aphasia: beyond aphasia. J Neurol 2022; 269:1375–1385
42.
Deramecourt V, Lebert F, Debachy B, et al: Prediction of pathology in primary progressive language and speech disorders. Neurology 2010; 74:42–49
43.
Harris JM, Gall C, Thompson JC, et al: Classification and pathology of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 2013; 81:1832–1839
44.
Spinelli EG, Mandelli ML, Miller ZA, et al: Typical and atypical pathology in primary progressive aphasia variants. Ann Neurol 2017; 81:430–443
45.
Boeve BF, Maraganore DM, Parisi JE, et al: Pathologic heterogeneity in clinically diagnosed corticobasal degeneration. Neurology 1999; 53:795–800
46.
Irwin DJ, Brettschneider J, McMillan CT, et al: Deep clinical and neuropathological phenotyping of Pick disease. Ann Neurol 2016; 79:272–287
47.
Piguet O, Halliday GM, Reid WGJ, et al: Clinical phenotypes in autopsy-confirmed Pick disease. Neurology 2011; 76:253–259
48.
Choudhury P, Scharf EL, Paolini MA, et al: Pick’s disease: clinicopathologic characterization of 21 cases. J Neurol 2020; 267:2697–2704

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 196 - 203
PubMed: 35921620

History

Received: 17 January 2022
Revision received: 31 March 2022
Accepted: 17 May 2022
Published in print: Summer 2022
Published online: 3 August 2022

Keywords

  1. Primary Progressive Aphasia
  2. Dynamic Aphasia
  3. Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration
  4. FTLD-tau
  5. Pick’s Disease
  6. Clinicopathologic Correlation
  7. Neuropathology
  8. Aphasias and Related Disorders

Authors

Details

Kristina Thurin, M.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Viharkumar Patel, M.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
David L. Perez, M.D., M.M.Sc.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Bradford C. Dickerson, M.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Daisy Hochberg, M.S., C.C.C.-S.L.P.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Megan Quimby, M.S., C.C.C.-S.L.P.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Michael B. Miller, M.D., Ph.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Mel Feany, M.D., Ph.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
David Silbersweig, M.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Scott M. McGinnis, M.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Kirk R. Daffner, M.D.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).
Seth A. Gale, M.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurology, Center for Cognitive and Memory Disorders, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, Ohio (Thurin); Departments of Psychiatry (Thurin, Silbersweig) and Neurology (Thurin, McGinnis, Daffner, Gale), Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Department of Pathology, Division of Neuropathology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Patel, Miller, Feany); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Divisions of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Perez); Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Dickerson, Hochberg, Quimby).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Gale ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

Dr. Perez has received research funding from NIH and the Sidney R. Baer, Jr. Foundation; he serves on the editorial boards of Epilepsy & Behavior and Brain and Behavior; he has received honoraria for continuing education lectures; and he receives royalties from Springer for a functional movement disorder textbook. Dr. Quimby receives an annual salary from Massachusetts General Hospital. Dr. Silbersweig is co-founder and chair of Ceretype Neuromedicine; he is a shareholder with Buoyant; and he serves as a Harvard Medical School CME course director. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share