Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: 1 February 2005

Characterizing the Emotions That Trigger Cataplexy

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Cataplexy is an intriguing example of how emotions can trigger muscle weakness by activating neural pathways. When associated with excessive daytime sleepiness, cataplexy is considered pathognomonic of narcolepsy. A questionnaire was administered to 55 patients with narcolepsy-cataplexy and 47 comparison subjects with obstructive sleep apnea. The area under the receiver-operating curve was 0.94 for the combination of muscle weakness with laughter and ability to hear during the episode. A 51-item questionnaire succeeds in identifying cataplexy in narcolepsy-cataplexy patients measured up against a comparison group. In the future, an abbreviated survey with these two questions should identify cataplexy with high sensitivity and specificity. These selected questions could subsequently be included into screening tools for use with different patient populations.
Cataplexy is one of the most intriguing examples of how thought content can alter neurologic functioning. In a state of cataplexy, an intense emotional state triggers objective transient muscle weakness verified by areflexia.1 The clinical manifestations are varied, ranging from involuntary eye closure and neck weakness to a subtle buckling of the knees to generalized muscle weakness that causes the patient to collapse.2 Consciousness and awareness of the environment are preserved throughout the episode. Few clinical phenomena better illustrate the ability of an emotional experience to cause neurochemical alterations that result in observable behavioral signs.
Known primarily as a symptom of the sleep disorder narcolepsy, cataplexy, in extremely rare cases, has been associated with other disorders, which include Nieman-Pick disease type C, Norrie’s disease, mid-brain tumors, and familial isolated cataplexy.3 When associated with excessive daytime sleepiness, with or without any other narcolepsy symptoms, cataplexy is considered pathognomonic of narcolepsy. Because of this close association, the pathologic features of narcolepsy provide valuable information about the factors conferring vulnerability to cataplexy. Recently, narcolepsy with cataplexy was associated with a deficiency of the neuropeptide hypocretin 1 (also known as orexin A), which is produced by a small number of cell bodies located only in the lateral hypothalamus.4 The hypocretin neurons project widely throughout the central nervous system, including the spinal cord. Hypocretin 1 deficiency appears to play a permissive role, allowing certain emotional states to cause rapid shifts in downstream neurotransmitters, which results in cataplexy.
The decreased muscle tone observed in cataplexy is similar to the absent electromyographic tone documented during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Therefore, cataplexy is believed to represent dissociated REM phenomena intruding into wakefulness after an emotional trigger. The proposed mechanism implicates postsynaptic inhibition of spinal alpha motoneurons by activated cells, including cholinoceptive ones in the midbrain, pontine, and medullary regions.5,6
The specific aim of this study was to investigate which emotional experiences triggered cataplexy, observe the manifestations of the episode and determine whether a brief survey that would aid in identifying cataplexy could be developed. The negative and positive predictive value of the abbreviated survey was also measured.

METHOD

Subjects

Fifty-five patients with narcolepsy and cataplexy were selected based on a clear history of cataplexy. All patients fulfilled the Mayo Narcolepsy Research criteria for definite narcolepsy.7 These diagnostic criteria require specific electrophysiologic data or physician-witnessed cataplexy with confirmed areflexia. This narcolepsy classification has been previously shown to have an interrater reliability of 0.98. In all cases, the presence of cataplexy was confirmed based on a review of the medical record by experienced sleep specialists (L.E.K., M.H.S.). In seven cases, the physician verified cataplexy by observing one episode. The narcoleptic patients remained on their customary therapy. The comparison group consisted of 47 subjects with obstructive sleep apnea diagnosed based on polysomnographic studies using International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD) criteria3{American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM), pp 52–58}. One comparison subject was excluded because of more persistent muscle weakness due to postpolio syndrome. No other narcoleptic subjects or comparison subjects were excluded due to any coexisting medical or psychiatric disorder.
The survey represented a modified version of the published 51-item cataplexy questionnaire validated by Anic-Labat8 that involved 74 patients with narcolepsy-cataplexy and 909 comparison subjects. The positive and negative predictive value of this tool was not reported. The original questions were formatted differently, and 15 items were revised to aid comprehension (e.g., “quick verbal response” was substituted for “repartee”).9 Patients were asked to answer the questions about triggering events based on their lifetime experience of cataplexy.
The questionnaires were mailed to narcolepsy-cataplexy subjects who returned them in a postage-paid envelope. Several of the participants completed the survey at the time of their scheduled follow-up office visits. The survey was distributed in an ongoing fashion as part of a narcolepsy registry, and a sample of the first 55 consecutive responses was used for this study. The comparison subjects were a convenience sample of obstructive sleep apnea patients returning for continuing care. They were given the survey at the time of their appointment by a registered nurse (W.M.). No remuneration was offered to participants. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Mayo Clinic Rochester. Patients indicated their voluntary consent to participate by returning a completed questionnaire that included a cover letter explaining the nature of the study.

Statistical Methods

The question responses were dichotomized into yes (always, sometimes, or rarely) versus no (never, unknown, or missing). Sensitivity, specificity, and area under the receiver-operating characteristic curve (AUC) were computed for each question individually. Confidence intervals for sensitivity and specificity were computed using the exact binomial distribution. For AUC, confidence intervals were computed using the bootstrap.10 Using only the 15 questions with AUC at least 0.75, recursive partitioning11 was employed to develop a decision tree. The results of this tree were summarized using the same diagnostic measures described above. Assuming cataplexy prevalence is 1 in 50 in a sleep disorders center population, positive predictive value and negative predictive value were estimated for each question as well as the decision rule suggested by the results of recursive partitioning.

RESULTS

Table 1 provides demographic and clinical data concerning the narcolepsy-cataplexy subjects and comparison subjects. Few of the narcolepsy-cataplexy subjects had current or past psychiatric disorders. In both groups, the most common coexisting psychiatric disorder was depression. The most common comorbid medical disorder was hypertension in four (7.2%) of the narcolepsy-cataplexy subjects and 14 (30%) of the comparison subjects. Eleven (20%) of the narcolepsy-cataplexy patients had obstructive sleep apnea. The majority of patients with narcolepsy-cataplexy were receiving treatment from one or more pharmacological classes at the time of the study: psychostimulants, 35 (64%); modafinil, seven (13%); antidepressants to suppress cataplexy, 21 (38%); and/or hypnotics to improve nocturnal sleep, five (9%).
Table 2 shows diagnostic results for the 15 questions with AUC greater than 0.75. Question 1, regarding laughing results, has the highest AUC, at 0.91 (95% CI: 0.85, 0.96). Note that the AUC confidence interval for Question 1 overlaps with the confidence intervals for 13 other questions. Recursive partitioning results suggested splitting first, based on Question 1, and splitting second, based on Question 35 (ability to hear during the spell), providing that the answer to question 1 was yes. The resulting decision rule is to classify patients as having cataplexy if the answers to both questions 1 and 35 are yes and to classify patients as not having cataplexy if the answer to either Question 1 or 35 is no.
Table 3 shows diagnostic results for this decision rule. Sensitivity (0.95) and specificity (0.94) are nearly equal, and the AUC (0.94) is higher than for any single question. Additionally, the AUC confidence interval overlaps with the confidence intervals for seven of the single questions.
Table 4 shows positive and negative predictive value for the 15 questions with AUC greater than 0.75 and the decision rule suggested by recursive partitioning. Negative predictive value is near 1.00 for all presented questions. The positive predictive value is higher for questions 32 or 31 than for the decision rule suggested by recursive partitioning. Moreover, the positive predictive value for Question 32 is equal to 1.00 due to lack of false positives.

DISCUSSION

This study confirmed the results of Anic-Labat,8 which suggest that laughter (Question 1) stands out as the emotional trigger most likely to provoke cataplexy. Patients with narcolepsy-cataplexy identified laughter as a more consistent cause of cataplexy than other closely related positive emotional states such as hearing a joke (Question 19), feeling excited (Question 3), feeling elated (Question 13), remembering a happy moment (Question 5), or experiencing an (unspecified) emotional event (Question 6). Laughter, a behavior unique to primates, is understudied. Studies on laughter have found that it consists of a process with repeated fluctuations of muscle activity and vocalizations spaced on average 210 ms apart.12 A recent report published in abstract form, revealed that the frontal lobe regions are activated on functional magnetic resonance imaging scans when subjects are amused but instructed not to laugh.13 The frontal lobes also determine social behavior and emotional judgment. The effect of laughter on neuronal firing in normal humans or narcoleptic subjects is unknown. Evidently, the hypocretin deficiency renders the subject vulnerable to laughter, causing abrupt transitions in neurotransmitters, putatively with increases in cholinergic activity, and reductions of norepinephrine and serotonin functioning.14 The mechanism by which laughter could result in the activation of cells in the midbrain, pons and medulla and cause inhibition of spinal motoneurons is currently unknown. The factors that determine the cessation of a cataplectic episode are also unknown but are suspected to be related to increases in norepinephrine and serotonin relative to acetylcholine. Antidepressant medications, particularly those that increase norepinephrine levels and suppress REM sleep, reduce the frequency of cataplectic episodes.15
The analysis of the questionnaire data examined the emotional triggers and the features of a cataplexy episode. Table 2 lists two characteristics of the episodes that are important for clinicians to recognize: slurred speech (Question 31) and being able to hear (Question 35). Subjects with narcolepsy-cataplexy also described having enough warning to break their fall (Question 43) and feeling paralyzed (Question 32). The differential diagnosis of spells is extensive. Slurred speech (related to muscle weakness), in the context of being able to hear auditory stimuli, helps distinguish narcolepsy-cataplexy from syncope, seizures, and other spells involving loss of consciousness. These features may also help distinguish narcolepsy-cataplexy from pseudocataplexy related to psychologic issues.16 Many subjects reported that others had witnessed a cataplexy episode (Question 50), a condition that raises suspicion for pseudoseizures that typically occur in public rather than solitary settings. However, the social nature of laughter would explain such an observation for narcolepsy-cataplexy.
This study has several possible limitations, including a relatively small sample size. Subjects may have had difficulty recalling events that may have occurred in the past, and no test-retest data are available. Another concern is that there is the remote chance that subjects in the obstructive sleep apnea comparison group had co-existing narcolepsy-cataplexy. The dramatically clear findings, when the responses of the two groups are compared, make this unlikely, however. Nonetheless, narcoleptic-cataplexy patients have a higher rate of obstructive sleep apnea than the general population. The procedure for diagnosing narcolepsy in patients with obstructive sleep apnea involves performing additional diagnostic testing if sleepiness persists after treatment. The comparison group in this study is more clearly defined than the one used by Anic-Labat8, which consisted of an undifferentiated sample of patients with varied sleep complaints, excluding clear-cut narcolepsy-cataplexy. Future studies could utilize a comparison group consisting of insomnia patients or the general population, although the utility of this questionnaire to distinguish patients with narcolepsy-cataplexy is expected to prevail.
Another potential limitation to this study is that participants may not have correctly understood the items on the questionnaire. Querying subjects about “muscle weakness” following a strong emotion is difficult, particularly for comparison subjects. Cataplexy is a condition so unusual that respondents may attempt to identify an incident that matches the description. The comparison subjects may have endorsed the experience inappropriately if they confuse narcolepsy-cataplexy with a syncopal spell, seizure, or other paroxysmal event. In our opinion, patients with narcolepsy-cataplexy are likely to recognize immediately the unique state they have experienced and correctly describe the events. The resulting bias is that the questionnaire may have resulted in false-positives in noncataplectic patients rather than false-positives or -negatives for the patients with first-hand experience of narcolepsy-cataplexy. Again, the robust findings reported here mitigate this situation.
In the absence of a reliable human cataplexy test, survey research about cataplexy takes on a special importance. This study confirms the findings of Anic-Labat that a questionnaire succeeds in identifying cataplexy in narcolepsy-cataplexy patients compared with a comparison group. This tool becomes particularly valuable if narcolepsy with cataplexy represents a disease with a different pathogenesis than the less common and more controversial condition of monosymptomatic narcolepsy (excessive daytime sleepiness without cataplexy). The hypocretin 1 data support this new segregation of narcolepsy into two distinct disorders since hypocretin deficiency has been strongly associated only with narcolepsy-cataplexy.17,18 If testing of cerebrospinal fluid levels of hypocretin 1 becomes a widely accepted diagnostic test for narcolepsy, then it would be prudent to verify the presence of narcolepsy-cataplexy with a valid and reliable survey before exposing the patient to the risks of a lumbar puncture.
The receiver-operating curve results indicate that this lengthy survey may be distilled from 51 questions to 2 questions (experience muscle weakness when laughing and ability to hear during the spell), with high sensitivity and specificity. In the future, an abbreviated version should be carefully evaluated with narcolepsy-cataplexy patients and comparison subjects. These selected questions could then be included into brief screening tools for use with sleep, community, or psychiatric patients. Once narcalepsy-cataplexy is recognized as a possibility, physicians can refer patients for appropriate laboratory testing. Narcolepsy-cataplexy remains an underdiagnosed disease worldwide, largely because of a lack of convenient and reliable screening. Comprehensive surveys remain useful for research purposes for patients with identified narcolepsy-cataplexy since collecting data about the disease facilitates research into this fascinating mind-body interface. Optimistically, functional neuroimaging studies that provide additional information about the specific neural pathways and neurochemical interactions involved in the disease will be performed in the future.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank Ashwin Gowda, Megan Reinalda, and Julie Stamschror for their contributions to this study.
TABLE 1. Characteristics of Subjects With Narcolepsy-Cataplexy and Subjects With Obstructive Sleep Apnea
TABLE 2. Diagnostic Results, Sorted by Area Under the Receiver-Operating Characteristic Curve (AUC), for the 15 Questions With AUC Greater Than 0.75
TABLE 3. Diagnostic Results for the Decision Rule Suggested by Recursive Partitioning
TABLE 4. Positive and Negative Predictive Values, Sorted by AUC, for the 15 Questions With AUC Greater Than 0.75 and the Decision Rule Suggested by Recursive Partitioning

References

1.
Krahn L, Black J, Silber M: Narcolepsy: new understanding of irresistible sleep. Mayo Clin Proc 2001; 76:185–194
2.
Parkes JD, Chen SY, Clift SJ, et al: The clinical diagnosis of the narcoleptic syndrome. J Sleep Res 1998; 7:41–52
3.
American Association of Sleep Medicine (AASM): The International Classification of Sleep Disorders—Revised: Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Rochester, Minn, AASM, 1997
4.
Nishino S, Ripley B, Overeem S, et al: Hypocretin (orexin) deficiency in human narcolepsy (letter). Lancet 2000; 355:39–40
5.
Lai Y, Siegel J: Medullary regions mediating atonia. J Neurosci 1988; 8:4790–4796
6.
Lai Y, Siegel J: Muscle tone suppression and stepping produced by stimulation of midbrain and rostral pontine reticular formation. J Neurosci 1990; 10:2727–2734
7.
Silber M, Krahn L, Olson E: Diagnosing narcolepsy: validity and reliability of new diagnostic criteria. Sleep Med 2002; 3:109–113
8.
Anic-Labat S, Guilleminault C, Kraemer HC, et al: Validation of a cataplexy questionnaire in 983 sleep-disorders patients. Sleep 1999; 22:77–87
9.
Gowda A, Krahn LE, Slocumb N, et al: Experiences that trigger cataplexy (abstract). Sleep 2001; 24(suppl S):A327
10.
Efron B, Tibshirani R: An Introduction to the Bootstrap. New York, Chapman & Hall, 1993
11.
Breiman L, Friedman JH, Olshen R, et al: Classification and Regression Trees. Belmont, Calif, Wadsworth, 1983
12.
Provine R: Laughter: A Scientific Investigation. New York, Viking, 2000
13.
Shibata DK, Zhong J, Kwok E, et al: Neuroimaging studies of laughter (abstract), in Proceedings of the 86th Annual Meeting of the Radiological Society of North America. Oak Brook, Ill, RSNA, 2000
14.
Guilleminault C, Gelb M: Clinical aspects and features of cataplexy, in Negative Motor Phenomena: Advances in Neurology. Edited by Fahn S, Hallett M, Luders H, Marsden C. Philadelphia, Lippincott-Raven, 1995, pp 65–77
15.
Nishino S, Mignot E: Pharmacological aspects of human and canine narcolepsy. Prog Neurobiol 1997; 52:27–78
16.
Krahn L, Hansen M, Shepard J: Pseudocataplexy. Psychosomatics 2001; 42:356–358
17.
Krahn LE, Pankratz VS, Oliver L, et al: CSF hypocretin (Orexin) levels in cerebrospinal fluid of patients with narcolepsy: relationship to cataplexy and HLA DQB1*0602 status. Sleep 2002; 25:733–736
18.
Mignot E, Lammers GJ, Ripley B, et al: The role of cerebrospinal fluid hypocretin measurement in the diagnosis of narcolepsy and other hypersomnias. Arch Neurol 2002; 59:1533–1562

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 45 - 50
PubMed: 15746482

History

Published online: 1 February 2005
Published in print: February 2005

Authors

Details

Lois E. Krahn, M.D.
Received October 29, 2002; revised May 19, 2003; accepted June 5, 2003. From the Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona; Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; Division of Biostatistics, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; and the Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota. Address correspondence to Dr. Krahn, Mayo Clinic Scottsdale, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259.
James F. Lymp, Ph.D.
Received October 29, 2002; revised May 19, 2003; accepted June 5, 2003. From the Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona; Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; Division of Biostatistics, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; and the Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota. Address correspondence to Dr. Krahn, Mayo Clinic Scottsdale, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259.
Wendy R. Moore, R.N.
Received October 29, 2002; revised May 19, 2003; accepted June 5, 2003. From the Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona; Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; Division of Biostatistics, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; and the Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota. Address correspondence to Dr. Krahn, Mayo Clinic Scottsdale, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259.
Nancy Slocumb
Received October 29, 2002; revised May 19, 2003; accepted June 5, 2003. From the Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona; Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; Division of Biostatistics, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; and the Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota. Address correspondence to Dr. Krahn, Mayo Clinic Scottsdale, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259.
Michael H. Silber, M.D.
Received October 29, 2002; revised May 19, 2003; accepted June 5, 2003. From the Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona; Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; Division of Biostatistics, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota; and the Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota. Address correspondence to Dr. Krahn, Mayo Clinic Scottsdale, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share