Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: 1 August 2005

Symptom-Oriented Versus Syndrome Approaches to Resolving Heterogeneity of Neuropsychological Functioning in Schizophrenia

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Schizophrenia is a heterogeneous disorder. The syndrome and the symptom-oriented approaches are two methods that have been used to examine differences in psychopathological processes across different patients with schizophrenia. The authors indirectly compared these two approaches in their examination of associations between positive symptoms and neuropsychological performance. Positive syndrome and delusion and hallucination severity scores were compared in respective associations to functioning on 12 neuropsychological variables in 73 stable outpatients with schizophrenia. The individual symptoms of the positive syndrome were associated with relatively distinct patterns of neuropsychological performance, suggesting that the symptom-oriented approach was more sensitive.
The heterogeneity problem in schizophrenia research has long been considered a significant obstacle to attempts to identify neuropsychological impairments that meaningfully characterize the disorder. Simply put, there is considerable heterogeneity across patients with respect to most aspects of the disease process. In order to address this problem, a number of methods for identifying and defining more homogenous groups have been advanced. With respect to the study of neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia, these approaches have been useful for addressing the question, “Which impairments characterize which patients?” However, the ideal method of classifying patients or their pathological processes into meaningful groups for study has yet to be established. In examining associations between positive symptoms and neuropsychological functioning, this study compared two of these methods: the syndrome approach and the symptom-oriented approach.
The syndrome approach involves grouping co-occurring symptoms into clusters or syndromes. Factor analyses of symptom ratings have typically identified at least three distinct clusters of schizophrenia symptoms. The three syndromes most often identified are referred to as the positive, negative, and disorganization syndromes.16 Many but not all studies that have examined the neuropsychological concomitants of these syndromes have found that the negative and disorganization syndromes are related to impairment in relatively diverse sets of neuropsychological abilities, while the positive syndrome is not highly related to neuropsychological deficits (e.g., 79 but see 1012). However, it is important to note that there has been some inconsistency with respect to neuropsychological associates of the positive syndrome. For example, impairment on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST), which is regarded as a measure of frontal lobe functioning, has been positively associated with severity of positive syndrome scores in some studies,11,13 unrelated in others,9,10,14,15 and inversely associated in others.7,8,16
In contrast to the syndrome approach, the symptom-oriented approach involves investigating individual symptoms. Advocates of the symptom-oriented approach have suggested that it is more useful for understanding psychopathology because there is controversy over which symptoms comprise a syndrome.17,18 Moreover, the individual symptoms of a syndrome sometimes may differ in their underlying psychopathological processes.17,19,20 There is a growing body of literature that has utilized symptom-oriented approaches in the investigations of neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia.16,2124 However, we are aware of only one published study that has directly compared the individual symptoms of a syndrome in their patterns of neuropsychological functioning. Bentall, Baker, and Kaney22 found that patients with hallucinations but no delusions performed worse on a self-monitoring task when compared to patients with delusions but no hallucinations, supporting the notion that there are differences in the neuropsychological processes that underlie delusions and hallucinations.
The purpose of this study was to compare the syndrome and the symptom-oriented approaches in examining associations between positive symptoms and neuropsychological performance. This was accomplished by examining the neuropsychological associations of the positive syndrome and those of the two main individual symptoms that comprise the positive syndrome: delusions and hallucinations. It was our expectation, based on the results of Bentall, Baker, and Kaney that delusions and hallucinations would be associated with different patterns of neuropsychological functioning. If true, this would suggest that the individual symptoms of the positive syndrome have appreciable differences in their underlying neuropsychological processes. Moreover, this would support the notion that a symptom-oriented approach is more sensitive for examining neuropsychological correlates of pathological behavior in schizophrenia.

METHOD

Subjects

This study was part of a multifaceted research project on cognition and symptoms in schizophrenia.25 The subject group consisted of 73 stable outpatients who met criteria for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition (DSM–IV)26 schizophrenia. Diagnoses were made by a clinical psychologist with diagnostic expertise (N.M. Docherty) and were based on information obtained during a Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Version (SADS-L)27 interview. Subjects with a primary language other than English who met criteria for current DSM–IV substance abuse or dependence or who presented histories of organic impairment were excluded. Subjects who had Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF)26 scores below 30 were also eliminated.
The subject group consisted of 45 men and 28 women. Fifty-three of the subjects were Caucasian, and 20 were African American. Subjects ranged in age from 18 to 67 years (mean = 36, SD = 10 years) and had an average education of 12.4, SD = 1.5 years and a mean GAF score of 50, SD = 12. Of the 73 subjects, 18 were being prescribed typical antipsychotic medication; 42 were being prescribed atypical antipsychotic medication; six were being prescribed both; and six were not being prescribed any antipsychotic medications. Twenty-seven of the subjects were being prescribed anticholinergic medications. Medication data were missing for one subject.

Procedure

Symptom Rating Scales.

The expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS)28 and the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS)29 were used to measure patients’ symptoms. How the positive syndrome is defined has varied across studies. For example, disorganization symptoms are included in some positive syndrome definitions30 but not others.31 For this reason, two different positive syndrome definitions were used in this study. The first, which reflects a broadly defined positive syndrome identified in a recent factor analysis study,30 was calculated by summing the bizarre behavior, unusual thought content, disorientation, hallucinations, and suspiciousness symptom ratings from the BPRS. The second, a more conservatively defined positive syndrome, was calculated by summing the individual global hallucinations and delusion symptom ratings from the SAPS. A negative syndrome score, also identified by Ventura et al.30 was used. The score was calculated by summing the blunted affect, motor retardation, emotional withdrawal, and self-neglect symptom ratings from the BPRS. Severity of formal thought disorder was measured using the SAPS global formal thought disorder score. The BPRS was employed because it measures a wide range of psychopathology and is commonly used to measure the schizophrenic syndromes. The SAPS was employed as a measure of individual symptoms because the individual SAPS items tend to measure a more narrowly defined scope of psychopathology than individual BPRS items. Symptom ratings were made by graduate student level researchers who had attained acceptable levels of interrater reliability (e.g., Intraclass Correlation Coefficient [ICC] values for SAPS positive syndrome ratings, ICC = 0.84; SAPS global formal thought disorder ratings, ICC = 0.82).

Neuropsychological Tests.

The neuropsychological battery included: the Shipley Institute of Living Scales,32 as a measure of general cognitive ability based on separate vocabulary and abstract concept formation ability subtests; a computerized Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST CV-2), as a measure of executive functions; the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) digit span subtest,33 which includes measures of verbal immediate memory (digit span forward) and working memory (digit span backward); the WAIS-R similarities subtest,33 which measures the ability to identify abstract relationships; the FAS Verbal Fluency Test;34 and a visual Dot Test,35 which measures nonverbal visuospatial working memory functions. The Shipley estimated intelligence quotient (IQ) scores are in standard score format,36 while the Shipley vocabulary and abstraction subtest scores are in T-score format. Perseverative and nonperseverative error scores from the WCST are represented by the age-normed T-score equivalents. Digit span total and similarities scores from the WAIS-R are represented by their age-normed scaled score equivalents. These tests were selected for use in the present study because their data were in the preexisting dataset. Due to subject attrition and protocol noncompliance, not all 73 subjects completed each individual test.

Analyses.

The analyses were conducted in two parts. First, bivariate correlations were calculated between the BPRS- and SAPS-defined positive syndromes and the 12 neuropsychological variables. Second, bivariate correlations were calculated separately between the SAPS global hallucinations score and the SAPS global delusions score and the 12 neuropsychological variables. These correlations were then compared using a Fisher r-to-z test.

RESULTS

Subject Demographic, Clinical Symptoms, and Neuropsychological Test Performance.

Means and standard deviations were computed for the broadly defined positive syndrome (mean = 10.8, SD = 4.8), SAPS narrowly defined positive syndrome (mean = 3.9, SD = 3.1), SAPS global hallucination (mean = 2.0, SD = 2.0), SAPS global delusion (mean = 1.9, SD = 1.6), BPRS negative syndrome (mean = 7.0, SD = 3.6), and SAPS global formal thought disorder (mean = 0.8, SD = 1.0) scores. Means and standard deviations were computed for the vocabulary (N=73; mean = 41.5, SD = 11.0) and abstraction (N=73; mean = 45.0, SD = 10.1) subsections from the Shipley scores (full scale estimated IQ N=73; mean = 86.2, SD = 14.5); the perseverative errors (N=65; mean = 36.6, SD = 12.8), nonperseverative errors (N=65; mean = 36.5, SD = 12.6), and total trials to completion (N=65; mean = 37.1, SD = 40.3) scores from the WCST; the forward (N=76; mean = 6.6, SD = 2.1) and backward (N=76; mean = 5.4, SD = 2.1) scores from the digit span (total score N=76; mean = 7.9, SD = 2.6); and the similarities (N=67; mean = 9.1, SD = 2.5), the FAS verbal fluency (N=64; mean = 32.6, SD = 12), and Dot Test (N=50; mean = 8.6, SD = 12.2) scores.
The distributions of the original neuropsychological test scores—that is, scores that were not converted to standard or T-score format—each approached normality, with all skew and kurtosis values less than 1.5. The WCST total trials and Dot Test scores were multiplied by a factor of −1 so that increasing scores for all of the neuropsychological tests would represent better performance. Table 1 contains a correlation matrix of the neuropsychological test scores.
There were no significant differences between men and women or Caucasians and African Americans on the BPRS- or SAPS-defined positive syndromes or the SAPS hallucinations or delusions severity scores. Similarly, age, education, or BPRS negative syndrome scores were significantly correlated with any positive syndrome or symptom severity score. However, SAPS global formal thought disorder severity scores were significantly related to BPRS (r = 0.26, df = 73, p<0.05) and SAPS (r = 0.31, df = 73, p<0.01) defined positive syndromes and SAPS hallucinations (r = 0.28, df = 73, p<0.05) and SAPS delusions (r = 0.27, df = 73, p<0.05) scores. These results suggest that the positive syndromes, hallucinations, or delusions severity scores were not significantly related to the demographic score or to negative syndrome scores.
A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), with post-hoc Scheffe tests, was conducted to compare four medication groups: typical antipsychotic, atypical antipsychotic, typical and atypical, and no antipsychotic medication. These four groups were compared on positive syndrome and symptom severity scores and neuropsychological performance scores. There were no significant differences among the groups on BPRS- or SAPS-defined positive syndromes, SAPS hallucinations or delusions, SAPS formal thought disorder, or BPRS negative syndrome severity scores. Likewise, there were no significant group differences regarding neuropsychological performance, although there was a trend among subjects who were being prescribed typical antipsychotic medications to have poorer performance on the Shipley verbal, WAIS-R digit span forward, and similarities tests, compared to subjects being prescribed atypical antipsychotics. Subjects who were prescribed anticholinergic medication performed significantly poorer on WAIS-R digit span forward scores (t = 2.24, df = 67, p<0.05), and at a trend level on the digit span total scores, compared to subjects not being prescribed anticholinergic medication. Generally speaking, differences in medications accounted for little difference in symptoms among these stable outpatients and accounted for only a few relatively small differences in neuropsychological performance.

Positive Syndrome Severity Scores and Neuropsychological Performance.

Bivariate correlations were calculated between the BPRS-defined positive syndrome, the SAPS-defined positive syndrome, and the 12 neuropsychological test scores. These results are presented in Table 2. The BPRS-defined positive syndrome scores were significantly and positively associated with Dot Test scores and inversely associated with similarities test scores at a trend level. Likewise, the SAPS-defined positive syndrome scores were significantly and positively associated with Dot Test scores and inversely associated with similarities test scores. The BPRS-defined positive syndrome and the SAPS-defined positive syndrome scores did not differ in their respective correlations to any of the 12 neuropsychological performance scores. They were not associated with any other measures. These results suggest that severity of both positive syndromes was associated at or above the trend level, with better performance on the visual working memory task and poorer performance on the similarities test. Not surprisingly, the BPRS-defined positive syndrome and SAPS-defined positive syndrome scores were highly correlated with each other (r = 0.86, df = 73, p<0.001).

Delusions, Hallucinations, and Neuropsychological Test Performance.

Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms hallucination and delusion severity scores were significantly associated with each other (r = 0.57, df = 73, p<0.001). Bivariate correlations were calculated between these symptom severity scores and the 12 neuropsychological performance scores (Table 2). SAPS hallucination scores were significantly and inversely related to Shipley abstraction scores and WCST total trials-to-completion scores and were related to similarities scores at the trend level. SAPS delusion scores were significantly positively associated with digit span backward and dot test scores. These results indicate that severity of hallucinations was associated with worse performance on two tests of abstraction ability, as measured by the similarities and Shipley abstraction tests, and poorer overall performance on the WCST test. Severity of delusions was associated with better performance on the two working memory tasks, the digit span backward, and the visual dot test.
In order to examine whether SAPS-defined delusions and hallucinations were associated with different patterns of neuropsychological performance, their respective correlations to the 12 neuropsychological performance scores were compared using the Fisher r-to-z test (Table 2). SAPS delusion and hallucination severity scores differed significantly in their relationships to the Shipley estimated IQ, Shipley vocabulary test, and WCST nonperseverative errors scores, and, at a trend level, in their relationships to the Shipley abstraction scores, WCST total trials, and the visual dot test scores. In sum, the SAPS defined hallucination and delusion scores differed in their respective associations to six of the 12 neuropsychological performance variables at or above the trend level.

DISCUSSION

Consonant with many previous studies of neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia, positive syndrome severity scores were significantly associated with functioning on only a few of the neuropsychological tests. Likewise, when hallucination and delusion severity scores were examined separately, they were related to performance on only a few of the 12 neuropsychological variables. However, when hallucination and delusion severity scores were compared in their respective associations to neuropsychological functioning, they differed at or above the trend level on one-half of the neuropsychological variables. There were no substantive effects of demographic, medication type, or negative or disorganization syndrome severity variables in these results, suggesting that delusions and hallucinations are associated with relatively different patterns of neuropsychological functioning and presumably have at least some important differences in their respective underlying neuropsychological processes.
The notion that the symptom-oriented approach, as compared to the syndrome approach, may be a more useful method for investigating how neuropsychological functioning differs across the different manifestations of schizophrenia was supported by two findings from this study. First, severity scores of the two main individual symptoms that comprise the positive syndrome, hallucinations, and delusions showed appreciable differences in their respective neuropsychological associates. Second, the conservatively and liberally defined positive syndromes did not differ much from each other, even though the liberally defined syndrome included ratings of suspiciousness, bizarre behavior, and disorientation symptoms. Thus, the positive syndromes were associated with similar neuropsychological profiles despite significant differences in the neuropsychological correlates of the individual symptoms that constitute these syndromes.
It would be premature, based solely on the results of this study, to attempt to identify specific neuropsychological substrata that differentially underlie hallucinations versus delusions. The general trend was for hallucination severity ratings to be associated with poorer neuropsychological performance and for severity of delusion ratings to be associated with better neuropsychological performance, particularly on tests of working memory. It is surprising that severity of delusions was associated with better performance on any test used in this study. One possible explanation for this is that the patients who presented with more severe delusions tended to be of a paranoid subtype. This would be consistent with evidence suggesting that the cognitive profiles of paranoid patients more closely approximate normality when compared to other patients with schizophrenia.37 For this to be true, however, paranoid subtype patients in our sample would have had to have lower hallucinations ratings compared to the other patients. With respect to the literature on severity of hallucinations in paranoid patients, there is mixed support for this idea. On one hand, paranoid and hallucinatory symptoms have loaded on different clusters in a factor analysis of items in a symptom ratings scale administered to a group of outpatients with schizophrenia.38 However, severity of hallucinations has not differed between paranoid subtype patients versus undifferentiated subtype patients in one study,39 versus hebephrenic patients in another study40 and have loaded on similar factors in other studies.3 This study did not include subtype diagnoses for patients, which would be a pertinent analysis in future research.
Several limitations to the study apply. First, it was exploratory in design, and no formal attempt to control for type II errors was included. Second, most of the subjects in the present study were being prescribed psychotropic medication. While there were no apparent substantive effects of medication type on hallucination or delusion severity scores, analyses such as these are not conclusive since medications are not prescribed randomly. Third, although there were a number of significant differences between delusions and hallucinations in their respective neuropsychological associates, it is important to note that the correlations between the individual positive symptoms and the neuropsychological functioning scores were moderately significant at best. However, there appear to be different patterns of associations for the symptoms. Finally, the sample size used was composed of stable, yet relatively chronic outpatients. It is unclear whether the results of this study would generalize to other patient groups.
In summary, this study replicated findings that the positive syndrome has few clinically significant neuropsychological associates. There were no substantive differences between a conservatively versus liberally defined positive syndrome in terms of neuropsychological correlates. However, when analyzed independently, hallucination and delusion severity scores were associated with different patterns of neuropsychological functioning, suggesting that a symptom-oriented approach to investigating neuropsychological functioning across heterogeneous manifestations of schizophrenia may be more sensitive and informative than the syndrome approach.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This study was supported by NIMH grants MH-58783 and MH-57151 to Dr. Docherty and was presented at the 2003 International Congress on Schizophrenia Research in Colorado Springs, CO.
TABLE 1. Correlation Matrix of Neuropsychological Test Performance Scores
TABLE 2. Bivariate Correlations Between the BPRS Positive Syndrome, and SAPS Positive Syndrome, Global Hallucinations (Hall.) and Delusions (Del.) Severity Ratings and Neuropsychological Test Performance Scores, and p Value of Fisher r-to-z Comparison of the SAPS Hallucination vs. Delusions Scores in Their Respective Associations to Neuropsychological Performance

References

1.
Liddle PF, Barnes TR, Morris D, et al: Three syndromes in chronic schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1989; 119–122
2.
Arndt S, Alliger RJ, Andreasen NC: The distinction of positive and negative symptoms: the failure of a two-dimensional model. Br J Psychiatry 1991; 158:317–322
3.
Grube BS, Bilder RM, Goldman RS: Meta-analysis of symptom factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1998; 31:113–120
4.
Toomey R, Faraone SV, Simpson JC, et al: Negative, positive, and disorganized symptom dimensions in schizophrenia, major depression, and bipolar disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:470–476
5.
Loeffler W, Haefner H: Replication of the 3-factor model by liddle to a population-related first-episode sample of schizophrenic patients. Nervenarzt 1999; 70:416–429
6.
John JP, Khanna S, Thennarasu K, et al: Exploration of dimensions of psychopathology in neuroleptic-naive patients with recent-onset schizophrenia/schizophreniform disorder. Psychiatry Res 2003; 121:11–20
7.
Schroder J, Tittle A, Stockert A, et al: Memory deficits in subsyndromes of chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1996; 21:19–26
8.
Basso MR, Nasrallah HA, Olson SC, et al: Neuropsychological correlates of negative, disorganized and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1998; 3:99–111
9.
Mahurin RK, Velligan DI, Miller AL: Executive-frontal lobe cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia: A symptom subtype analysis. Psychiatry Res 1998; 79:139–149
10.
Berman I, Viegner B, Merson A, et al: Differential relationships between positive and negative symptoms and neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1997; 25:1–10
11.
Zaksanis KK: Neuropsychological correlates of positive vs. negative schizophrenic symptomatology. Schizophr Res 1998; 29:227–233
12.
Shean G, Burnett T, Eckman FS: Symptoms of schizophrenia and neurocognitive test performance. J Clin Psychol 2004; 58:723–731
13.
Daban C, Amado I, Bayle F, et al:Correlation between clinical syndromes and neuropsychological tasks in unmedicated patients with recent onset schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2003; 113:83–92
14.
Himelhoch S, Taylor SF, Goldman RS, et al: Frontal lobe tasks, antipsychotic medication, and schizophrenic syndromes. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39:227–229
15.
Norman RMG, Mala AK, Morrison-Stewart SL, et al: Neuropsychology and schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 170:134–139
16.
Hammer MA, Katsanis J, Iacono W: The relationship between negative symptoms and neuropsychological performance. Biol Psychiatry 1995; 37:828–830
17.
Strauss ME: Relations of symptoms to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1993; 19:215–231
18.
Earnst KS, Kring AM: Construct validity of negative symptoms: An empirical and conceptual review. Clin Psychol Rev 1997; 17:167–190
19.
Bentall R. The Syndromes and Symptoms of Psychosis: Reconstructing Schizophrenia. Routledge, 1990
20.
Costello CG: Research on symptoms versus research on syndromes: arguments in favour of allocating more research time to the study of symptoms. Br J Psychiatry 1992; 160:304–308
21.
Bentall R, Kaney S: Content specific information processing and persecutory delusions: an investigation using the emotional Stroop test. Br J Bed Psychol 1989; 62:355–364
22.
Bentall RP, Baker GA, Havers S: Reality monitoring and psychotic hallucinations. British J Clin Psychol 1991; 30:213–222
23.
Stolar N, Berenbaum H, Banich MT, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of alogia and affective flattening in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1994; 35:164–172
24.
Docherty NM, Hall MJ, Gordinier SW, et al: Conceptual sequencing and disordered speech in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:723–735
25.
Docherty NM, Cohen AS, Nienow TN, et al: Stability of formal thought disorder and referential communication disturbances in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 2003; 112:469–475
26.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association, 1994
27.
Endicott J, Spitzer RL: A diagnostic interview: the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35:837–844
28.
Lukoff D, Nuechterlein K, Ventura J: Manual for the expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Schizophr Bull 1986; 12:594–602
29.
Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. Iowa City, University of Iowa Press, 1982
30.
Ventura J, Nuechterlein KH, Subotnik KL, et al: Symptom dimensions in recent-onset schizophrenia and mania: a principal components analysis of the 24-item brief psychiatric rating scale. Psychiatry Res 2000; 97:129–135
31.
Liddle PF, Barnes TR: Syndromes of chronic schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 157:558–561
32.
Shipley WC: A self-administering scale for measuring intellectual impairment and detioration. J Psychol 1940; 9:371–377
33.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York, Psychological Corperation, 1981
34.
Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995
35.
Keefe RSE, Lees-Roitman SE, Dupre RL: Performance of patients with schizophrenia on a pen and paper visuospatial working memory task with short delay. Schizophr Res 1997; 26:9–14
36.
Zachary R, Paulson M, Gorsuch R: Estimating WAIS IQ from the shipley Institute of Living Scale using continuously adjusted age norms. J Clin Psychol 1985; 41:820–831
37.
Hill SK, Ragland JD, Gur RC, et al: Neuropsychological differences among empirically derived clinical subtypes of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 2001; 15:492–501
38.
Silver H, David D, Kaplan M, et al: Factor analysis of schizophrenic symptoms and comparison of different rating scales. Schizophr Res 10; 1993; 1:67–75
39.
Marengo JT, Harrow M, Westermeyer JF: Early longitudinal course of `chronic and paranoid' undifferentiated schizophrenia subtypes and schizophreniform disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2003; 100:600–603
40.
Winokur G, Morrison J, Clancy J, et al: Iowa 500: The clinical and genetic distinction of hebephrenic and paranoid schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 1974; 159:12–19

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 384 - 390
PubMed: 16179662

History

Published online: 1 August 2005
Published in print: August 2005

Authors

Affiliations

Alex S. Cohen, Ph.D.
Received August 26, 2003; revised June 8, 2004; accepted June 11, 2004. From the Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland. Address correspondence to Alex Cohen, Ph.D., University of Maryland, Department of Psychology, 1129 Biology/Psychology Bldg., College Park, MD 20742; [email protected] (E-mail).
Nancy M. Docherty, Ph.D.
Received August 26, 2003; revised June 8, 2004; accepted June 11, 2004. From the Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland. Address correspondence to Alex Cohen, Ph.D., University of Maryland, Department of Psychology, 1129 Biology/Psychology Bldg., College Park, MD 20742; [email protected] (E-mail).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share