Skip to main content
Full access
Clinical and Research Reports
Published Online: 1 August 2005

Risk-Taking on Tests Sensitive to Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Dysfunction Predicts Early Relapse in Alcohol Dependency: A Pilot Study

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Twenty one patients in a residential rehabilitation program fulfilling International Classification of Diseases-10 (ICD) criteria for alcohol dependence syndrome were recruited. On neuropsychological tests, alcohol dependent patients relapsed early if they made choices governed by immediate gain irrespective of later outcome, which is consistent with dysfunctional ventromedial-prefrontal cortex mediating the inability to resist the impulse to drink despite ultimately deleterious effects. The authors suggest that the use of neuropsychological tasks of decision making may have several advantages over more conventional strategies for studying alcoholism.
The prefrontal cortex is particularly vulnerable to damage following long-term alcohol abuse.1 Studies have found executive dysfunction, which is believed to be associated with prefrontal cortex impairment, in abstinent alcohol-dependent subjects in association with reduced frontal metabolism.2 The consequences of executive dysfunction include the failure to plan ahead or to think adequately about the consequences of actions. In alcohol dependence, such deficits may cause early relapse because individuals are unable to develop and use strategies to avoid alcohol or because they are unable to withhold the immediate desire to drink for long-term benefits. We tested these hypotheses using tests of executive function in recently detoxified alcohol-dependent subjects and, during a 3-month period, compared those who relapsed with those who remained abstinent. We used a test of planning sensitive to dorsolateral-prefrontal cortex function3 and tests of decision making or gambling believed to reflect the integrity of ventromedial-prefrontal cortex.4,5 We also measured memory and personality traits, which can be abnormal in alcoholism and may contribute to early relapse.

METHOD

Subjects

Twenty-one patients in a residential rehabilitation program fulfilling International Classification of Diseases-10 (ICD) criteria for alcohol dependence syndrome participated. Exclusion criteria included polysubstance dependency, organic brain disease, learning difficulties, and comorbid mental illness. Wandsworth Local Research Ethics Committee approved the study, and subjects gave written informed consent. Assessments were performed after 21 days of detoxification. Twenty normal volunteers served as comparison subjects for the neuropsychological tests and personality assessments.

Assessments

Three computerized executive tests were administered. 1) A computerized version of the Tower of London Planning Task,6 which requires subjects to plan and execute a series of moves in order to change the position of balls hanging in billiard pockets to match a goal arrangement, was administered. The number of required moves on each trial varies between 3 and 5. The measure is the number of solutions (out of 12) solved in the minimum number of moves (perfect solutions). 2) A decision making task7 in which subjects decided whether a token was hidden in a blue or red box was also administered. Subjects were given “odds” information (9:1, 8:2, 7:3, 6:4) to guide them. On each of 72 trials, they could also gain or lose points by betting according to their certainty of making a correct choice. The measures were the percent trials in which they chose the correct box (quality) and the percent accumulated points bet at each odd (risk). 3) A gambling task4 in which subjects selected a card from one of four decks on each of 100 trials was administered. Each card turn was rewarded with points, but some cards subsequently demanded a penalty forfeit of points. The decks were arranged so that the cards in two of the decks resulted in higher points than cards in other decks, but they extracted higher penalties and were disadvantageous compared to the “good” decks in the long term. Memory was assessed using a computerized paired associates test.8 Subjects needed to learn the position of six or eight patterns presented on the screen one at a time. They were reminded of their mistakes and had up to 10 attempts to correctly recall the position of each pattern. Measures were total number of errors (learning) and number of pattern location recalled correctly at the first attempt (memory). All subjects completed the computerized tasks except the decision making task, which was completed by all comparison subjects and 16 patients. Intelligence quotient (IQ) was estimated using the National Adult Reading Test,9 which was completed by 18 comparison and 16 patients. Other measures were: the Structured Clinical Interview for DSMIII-R, antisocial and borderline subscales; the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS); the Addiction Severity Index (ASI), which was completed by all patients; the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) (34-item),10 (see Table) which was completed by 18 patients and 19 comparison subjects; and the Dimensional Assessment of Personality–Basic Questionnaire (DAPP-BQ),11 completed by 20 patients and all comparison subjects.
Because of small numbers, group differences were analyzed using the Mann-Whitney U Test, except the decision making task, which was analyzed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) due to repeated measures. All probabilities are two-tailed.

RESULTS

Although the patients had a higher mean IQ (mean (SD), comparison group: 112.44 (8.49), patients: 119.44 (10.19); U= 62.5, p=0.005) and were slightly older (comparison group: 36.05 (10.97), patients: 40.95 (9.47), N=140.00, p=0.068), there were no differences in paired associates (memory: N=208, p=0.97; learning: U=194, p=0.68) and planning (perfect solutions: U=165, p=0.24). There were trends for the alcoholics to risk more points than controls across all odds on the decision making task (N=2.74, df=1,34, p=0.11) and to select more cards from the “bad” decks on the gambling task (N=142, p=0.08). Patients were more impulsive on the BIS (U=55, p<0.001) and had higher scores on emotional dysregulation (U=76.5, p=0.001), dissocial behavior (U=84.5, p=0.002) and compulsive (U=108.5, p=0.01) DAPP-BQ personality factors.
The patients were divided into those who had relapsed (N=6) or remained abstinent (N=15) at 3 months. The table shows that the relapsed patients were significantly younger, were more impulsive on the BIS, sampled significantly more cards from the bad decks on the gambling task and consistently risked more points across all odds on the decision making task. There were no differences between the groups on IQ, planning, quality of decision making, learning and memory. Six patients reached DSMIII-R criteria for borderline or dissocial personality disorder, with equal numbers in each group (Chi-Square =0.6, df=1, nsec). There were no differences between the groups with respect to DAPP-BQ personality factors (range of U values: 24–34), years drinking to intoxication (N=35, p=0.43) and psychopathology (BPRS, N=32.5, p=0.33).

DISCUSSION

Recently detoxified alcohol dependent patients were more likely to relapse within 3 months if they made more choices in which the immediate reward was large but the long term consequences were disadvantageous on a gambling task, and if they staked more points on their decisions being correct on a decision making task. It is unlikely that these findings reflect alcohol-induced brain damage because they were unimpaired on a memory test sensitive to the early stages of dementia.8 Long-term alcoholism particularly affects dorsolateral-prefrontal cortex1 and our patients were also unimpaired on a test of planning, sensitive to damage of this area.3,6 This specific pattern of performance is reminiscent of patients with ventromedial-prefrontal cortex lesions, who perform normally on most neuropsychological tests yet are impaired on decision making tasks where performance requires the weighing-up of risks and benefits.4 On such tasks they behave as if they are insensitive to the future consequences of their decisions and make choices based on immediate effects.4 Thus, relapsed patients displayed a pattern of behavior governed by the reinforcing effects of immediate rewards which in everyday situations may prevent them from resisting the impulse to drink, despite the ultimately deleterious effects. This substantiates findings made by Bechara et al. on a mixed group of alcohol and stimulant abusers12 in that our relapsed patients may represent a subgroup of alcohol dependent individuals with a specific premorbid dysfunction of ventromedial-prefrontal cortex.
Emotionally unstable and antisocial behavior has long been recognized as a risk factor for subsequent alcoholism. Compared to abstinent patients, relapsed patients were younger and scored higher on ratings of impulsiveness—features of Cloninger Type II alcoholism.13 However, although our patients had a high frequency of personality disorder (29%) and displayed higher than normal ratings of personality dimensions considered to reflect emotional instability/harm avoidance and dissocial behavior/sensation seeking,11,13 these measures did not distinguish those who relapsed or abstained. This is consistent with other studies, which have failed to characterize alcoholic patients using personality assessments.14
Although these results are from a pilot study and need to be replicated in a much larger group of patients, we tentatively suggest that the use of neuropsychological tasks of decision making may have two advantages over more conventional strategies for investigating alcoholism. First, these tasks may be more sensitive to behaviors fundamental to subtypes of alcoholism. Second, these tasks provide a neuroscience framework for the investigation of the neurobiological substrates of addiction, for example, abnormalities of serotonin function have been linked with impulsive behavior and alcoholism.15 In alcoholics, investigation of the serotoninergic modulation of ventromedial-prefrontal cortex function, in association with performance on these tasks, may provide an important avenue for further research.7

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Dr. Bowden-Jones was supported by a Priory Hospital Fellowship.
Findings in this study were presented at the 26th Annual Research Society on Alcoholism Scientific Meeting, Ft Lauderdale, FL, June 21–25, 2003.
TABLE Age, Ratings of Impulsiveness and Neuropsychological Function in Detoxified Alcohol Dependent Patients Who Subsequently Relapsed (n=6) or Remained Abstinent (n=15 at 3 Months Postdetoxification

References

1.
Kril J, Halliday G, Svoboda M, Cartwright H: The cerebral cortex is damaged in chronic alcoholics. Neuroscience 1997; 79:983–998
2.
Dao-Castellana M, Samson Y, Legault F, Martinot J, Aubin H, Crouzel C, Feldman L, Barrucand D, Rancurel G, Feline A, Syrota A: Frontal dysfunction in neurology normal chronic alcoholic subjects: metabolic and neuropsychological findings. Psychol Med 1998; 28:1039–1048
3.
Owen AM, Doyon J, Petrides M, Evans AC: Planning and spatial working memory examined with a positron emission tomography study in humans. Eur J Neuroscience 1996; 8:353–364
4.
Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 1994; 50:7–15
5.
Rogers R, Owen A, Middleton H, Williams E, Pickard J, Sahakian B, Robbins T: Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital cortex. J Neuroscience 1999; 19:9029–9038
6.
Owen A, Downes J, Sahakian B, Polkey C, Robbins T: Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 1990; 28:1021–1034
7.
Rogers R, Everitt B, Baldacchino A, Johnson A, Swainson R, London M, Deakin J, Sahakian B, Robbins T: Dissociable effects in the decision-making of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate users, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, tryptophan normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 20:322–329
8.
Fowler K, Saling M, Conway E, Semple J, Louis W: Paired associate performance inthe early detection of DAT. J the Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002; 8:58–71
9.
Nelson H, Willison J: The revised National Adult Reading Test (NART)-Test Manual. Windsor, Berks, NFER-NELSON, 1991
10.
Patton J, Stanford M, ES B: Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol 1995; 51:768–774
11.
Livesley WJ, Lang KL, Vernon PA: Phenotypic and genetic structure of traits delineating personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:941–948
12.
Bechara A, Dolan S, Denburg N, Hindes A, Anderson S, Nathan P: Decision-making deficits linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontalcortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia 2001; 39:376–389
13.
Cloninger CR: Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Science 1987; 236:410–416
14.
Mulder R: Alcoholism and personality. Aust NZ J Psychiatry 2002; 36:44–52
15.
Higley D, Linnoila M: A non-human primate model of excessive alcohol intake. personality and neurobiological parallels of type I and type II-like alcoholism. Rec Dev Alcohol 1997; 13:191–219

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 417 - 420
PubMed: 16179667

History

Published online: 1 August 2005
Published in print: August 2005

Authors

Details

Henrietta Bowden-Jones, M.R.C.Psych.
Received May 15, 2003; revised April 13, 2004; accepted June 8, 2004. From the Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, London, England; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England. Address correspondence to Professor Joyce, Dept Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, Charing Cross Campus, St Dunstan’s Rd., London W6 8RP, UK; [email protected] (E-mail).
Mike McPhillips, MRCP, F.R.C.Psych
Received May 15, 2003; revised April 13, 2004; accepted June 8, 2004. From the Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, London, England; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England. Address correspondence to Professor Joyce, Dept Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, Charing Cross Campus, St Dunstan’s Rd., London W6 8RP, UK; [email protected] (E-mail).
Robert Rogers, Ph.D.
Received May 15, 2003; revised April 13, 2004; accepted June 8, 2004. From the Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, London, England; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England. Address correspondence to Professor Joyce, Dept Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, Charing Cross Campus, St Dunstan’s Rd., London W6 8RP, UK; [email protected] (E-mail).
Sam Hutton, Ph.D.
Received May 15, 2003; revised April 13, 2004; accepted June 8, 2004. From the Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, London, England; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England. Address correspondence to Professor Joyce, Dept Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, Charing Cross Campus, St Dunstan’s Rd., London W6 8RP, UK; [email protected] (E-mail).
Eileen Joyce, Ph.D., MRCP, F.R.C.Psych.
Received May 15, 2003; revised April 13, 2004; accepted June 8, 2004. From the Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, London, England; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England. Address correspondence to Professor Joyce, Dept Psychological Medicine, Imperial College, Charing Cross Campus, St Dunstan’s Rd., London W6 8RP, UK; [email protected] (E-mail).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share