Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: November 1997

Comparability of Telephone and Face-to-Face Interviews in Assessing Axis I and II Disorders

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The present study examined the comparability of data obtained by telephone and face-to-face interviews for diagnosing axis I and II disorders. METHOD: Sixty young adults from the community were interviewed face-to-face and over the telephone regarding axis I disorders; another 60 subjects were interviewed twice regarding axis II disorders. The order of interviews was counterbalanced, and subjects with a history of disorder were oversampled. Agreement between telephone and face-to-face interviews was contrasted with interrater values, which were obtained by having a second interviewer rate a recording of the original interview. RESULTS: Interrater reliability was excellent. Agreement between telephone and face-to-face assessment was excellent for anxiety disorders and very good for major depressive disorder and alcohol and substance use disorders; agreement was problematic, however, for adjustment disorder with depressed mood. Strong support was shown for the validity of the axis II telephone assessment format. Small but consistent trends were noted for lower rates of psychopathology reported in the second interview. CONCLUSIONS: This is the first study in which telephone and face-to-face assessments of axis I and II psychopathology were conducted with the same subjects assigned to conditions in a counterbalanced manner. The present findings provide qualified justification for the use of telephone interviews to collect axis I and II data. The apparent concerns do not appear sufficient to override the economic and logistic advantages of telephone interviewing. (Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1593–1598)
The reliability of psychiatric diagnosis has been markedly enhanced through the use of standardized interview schedules (e.g., the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia [1], the Diagnostic Interview Schedule [2], and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R [3]). Although these interviews were developed for use in a face-to-face format, interviews that assess axis I and II disorders in research settings are often done by telephone (J. Endicott, D. Kilpatrick, and R. Kessler, personal communications, 1996). The major advantage of telephone interviews over face-to-face interviews is cost efficiency (4). Telephone interviewing is also logistically simpler, especially if the participant resides in a geographically distant location. The extensive use of telephone interviews in research presupposes that the obtained diagnostic information is as valid as that obtained in person. The goal of the present study is to examine this assumption with regard to axis I and II diagnoses obtained in a group of young adults from the community.
In addition to the obvious research implications, understanding the adequacy of data obtained by telephone has clinical importance. Telephone-based programs have been used to screen for psychiatric difficulties such as depression and obsessive-compulsive disorder (5, 6), administer smoking-cessation programs (7), conduct psychotherapy (8), and provide expert consultation to underserved populations, such as individuals in rural settings (9, 10).
Most previous studies that examined the comparability of telephone and face-to-face interviewing have contrasted the relative prevalence rates of disorder associated with the two assessment procedures. Given similar rates of disorder for the two methods, it has been concluded that the methods are comparable (1113). A more rigorous test of the comparability of the two assessment methods is to repeat the interview by using both telephone and face-to-face procedures. A few studies have adopted this approach. Wells et al. (14) reinterviewed over the telephone 230 adults who had been interviewed face-to-face 3 months earlier as part of the Epidemiologic Catchment Area study. Diagnostic agreement for depression (major depression and dysthymia) was fair (kappa=0.57). The authors concluded that the telephone interview had acceptable agreement with the original “gold standard” face-to-face interview and that no evidence suggested that one method resulted in more positive reports regarding depression. Paulsen et al. (15) compared the reliability of lifetime anxiety disorders in 39 probands who initially had been interviewed face-to-face and were reinterviewed by telephone 12–19 months later. Interrater agreement ranged from good to excellent (kappa=0.69–0.84). Given the long interval between interviews, reliability may have been attenuated by change in the subjects' clinical status. In the present study, the interval between test-retest assessments was evaluated as one of the measures that may affect agreement.
A second limitation of previous studies is that the telephone interview always followed the face-to-face interview. The commonly noted finding of less psychopathology being reported on the second interview (16, 17) makes it impossible to determine if systematic differences were due to the method of assessment or the order of interviews. In the present study, the order of the two assessment methods was counterbalanced.
Finally, gender may influence the degree to which individuals reveal certain kinds of information in the context of different interview formats. There is some indication that between face-to-face and telephone interviews men show a greater discrepancy than women in the reporting of information, at least for sensitive material (18). Gender differences in comparability across assessment methods were examined in the present study.

METHOD

Subjects and Procedures

The current study was conducted in the context of an ongoing follow-up of participants from the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project. Extensive data have been collected previously from these individuals on two separate occasions while they were in high school (14–18 years of age). A detailed description of the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project is provided elsewhere (19).
Subjects from the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project were invited to participate in a third wave of diagnostic assessments at age 24, which included structured psychiatric interviews to assess axis I and II disorders. Written informed consent was obtained after the procedures had been fully described. For the current study, 60 subjects who were residing in the local area were chosen to be interviewed both face-to-face and over the telephone regarding axis I disorders; an additional 60 subjects were chosen to be interviewed twice regarding axis II disorders. To guarantee an adequate representation of various psychiatric disorders, in each group of 60 participants, 20 were selected on the basis of a prior diagnosis of major depression, 20 were selected on the basis of a prior psychiatric disorder other than depression, and 20 had no diagnosed psychopathology. Of the 40 subjects selected because of a psychiatric disorder other than major depressive disorder, rates of disorder were as follows: substance use disorder (42.5%, N=17), adjustment disorder (32.5%, N=13), anxiety disorder (22.5%, N=9), disruptive behavior disorder (12.5%, N=5), dysthymia (7.5%, N=3), and an eating disorder (2.5%, N=1). There was no overlap between subjects who repeated the axis I interview and those who repeated the axis II interview. Fifty percent of the subjects participated in the face-to-face interview first, and 50% were interviewed by telephone first. The median duration between the two axis I interviews was 14 days (mean=22.5, range=2–92). Median duration between axis II interviews was 12 days (mean=16.8, range=1–55).
Seventy (58.3%) of the subjects were young women. The mean age at the time of interview was 24.4 years (SD=0.3). The vast majority (95.8%, N=115) identified themselves as Caucasian. Over two-thirds had either a high school diploma (67.5%, N=81) or a General Equivalency Diploma (2.6%, N=3), and 21.7% (N=26) had gone on to receive a bachelor's degree. Eighty percent (N=96) were working, 10.0% (N=12) were homemakers, and 3.3% (N=4) were students. The majority (55.0%, N=66) were single, 40.8% (N=49) were married, and 4.2% (N=5) were separated or divorced. The median household income was between $10,000 and $14,999.

Diagnostic Interviews

Assessment of axis I disorders. To cover the period between the previous assessment and the current study, the Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation (20) was administered to each participant. This methodology provided detailed information about the longitudinal course of all disorders that were present at the previous assessment. The Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation also probed for the occurrence of new disorders since the previous assessment. To maintain diagnostic continuity with the first two assessments, a modified version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (KIDDIE-SADS) (21) that combined features of the epidemiologic and present episode versions was used to assess axis I disorders that began in the interval before the current study. Additional questions were added to 1) assess disorders that were not previously examined (e.g., posttraumatic stress disorder [PTSD] and somatoform disorders), 2) reflect the adult presentations of disorders, and 3) incorporate changes associated with DSM-IV. In the present study, we focus on DSM-IV disorders or disorder categories that had prevalence rates greater than 5%. This included major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders (most often PTSD or panic disorder), alcohol use disorders (dependence and abuse), substance use disorders (most often cannabis abuse or dependence), and adjustment disorder with depressed mood.
Assessment of axis II disorders. The Personality Disorder Examination (22) was used to assess all axis II disorders. The Personality Disorder Examination, which is organized according to issues (work, self, interpersonal relations, affect, reality testing, impulse control) usually took 1–2 hours to administer. When an item was endorsed, specific examples were solicited to rate the trait (0=behavior or trait absent or normal, 1=exaggerated or accentuated, 2=criterion level or pathological). In general, traits needed to be present for 5 years to meet diagnostic criteria. A detailed scoring manual was available that defined the scope and meaning of each item. The Personality Disorder Examination has been found to be among the most reliable assessments of axis II disorders and to be less influenced by concurrent depression or anxiety levels than other assessment methods (23). Items that assessed the provisional disorders of self-defeating and sadistic personality disorder were not included. With the assistance of one of the interview's authors (Dr. Loranger), 20 items were added to the interview—which was originally developed to assess DSM-III-R criteria—to assess changes in criteria associated with DSM-IV (all forms used in this study are available from Dr. Rohde upon request).

Interviewer Training

Six diagnostic interviewers (five women, one man; five with master's or doctoral degrees in clinical or counseling psychology) were carefully trained in an extensive didactic and experiential course in interviewing. Before collecting data, interviewers were required to demonstrate a minimum kappa of 0.80 across all symptoms for at least two consecutive training interviews. In the first interview, the trainee scored diagnostic data collected by an experienced interviewer (either live or from recording). In the second training interview, the trainee conducted a live interview, while an experienced interviewer independently observed and scored diagnostic data. During data collection, biweekly discussion sessions were held between the supervisor and the interviewers to review interview procedures and diagnostic criteria. All interviews were audiotaped or videotaped, and a randomly selected subset were rated by a second experienced interviewer.

Statistical Analysis

Two types of reliability were examined: the degree of agreement between telephone and face-to-face interviews was contrasted with interrater reliabilities, in which a second interviewer made psychiatric ratings on the basis of reviewing a recording of the original interview. Fifty percent of the interviews (i.e., 60 KIDDIE-SADS and 60 Personality Disorder Examination interviews) were coded for interrater reliability.
Diagnostic reliability was evaluated by the kappa statistic (24), which assesses the degree of agreement for a dichotomous measure controlling for chance. Kappas less than 0.40 are generally considered poor, values that exceed 0.60 are good, and kappas above 0.75 or 0.80 are considered excellent (25). In addition to the kappa statistic, McNemar's chi-square (26) was computed to examine the tendency to report fewer symptoms with one method of interviewing than the other. Differences in kappa values were examined by using the z test (25).
Base rates were too low to examine specific axis II disorders. Therefore, agreement regarding axis II disorders was examined by using dimensional scores (i.e., summation of partial and full symptom criteria), which were computed for the three personality disorder clusters (cluster A: paranoid, schizoid, and schizotypal personality disorders; cluster B: antisocial, borderline, histrionic, and narcissistic personality disorders; cluster C: avoidant, dependent, obsessive-compulsive, and passive-aggressive personality disorders) as well as for all personality disorders. Agreement in the continuous dimensional scores, as well as the mean number of individual axis I symptoms, was examined by using the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) (27). All statistical tests were two-tailed.

RESULTS

Axis I Disorders

Prevalence. The prevalence rates of axis I disorders in the 60 young adults are shown in table 1. Substance use disorders were reported significantly less frequently in the telephone interview. A similar trend was present for alcohol use disorders (McNemar χ2=3.57, df=1, p=0.06). Although rates of disorder in the second interview were in each case lower than the first interview, none of the individual differences was statistically significant. When rates of all disorders in the first versus second interview were combined, the test of bias approached significance (McNemar χ2=3.77, df=1, p=0.052).
Level of agreement. Interrater kappas for 60 interviews (29 face-to-face and 31 telephone interviews obtained from 41 unique subjects) were computed and, with the exception of adjustment disorder with depressed mood, were excellent.
= The test-retest levels of agreement between face-to-face and telephone interviews for axis I disorders are also shown in table 1. Agreement across methods was excellent for anxiety disorders, very good for alcohol and substance use disorders and major depressive disorder, and poor for adjustment disorder with depressed mood. Test-retest reliability was significantly lower than interrater reliability for major depressive disorder, alcohol use disorders, and substance use disorders; differences for anxiety disorders and adjustment disorder with depressed mood were not statistically significant. However, with the exception of adjustment disorder with depressed mood, none of the test-retest kappas significantly differed from 0.80, the benchmark indication of excellent agreement.
Diagnostic agreement in the test-retest condition was evaluated as a function of gender and length of time between the two interviews. Subjects were divided by using the median length of time between the two interviews (14 days). Diagnostic agreement (kappa) between face-to-face and telephone interviews did not statistically differ for men versus women or for subjects who were interviewed within a 2-week period versus subjects who were interviewed over a longer time interval.
Axis I symptoms. The mean number of symptoms as a function of assessment method and order of interview are shown in table 2. Significantly fewer symptoms of alcohol and substance use disorders were reported in the telephone interview than in the face-to-face interview. In addition, significantly fewer symptoms of major depressive disorder and substance use disorders were reported in the second interview than in the first.
Level of agreement. Interrater agreement regarding the number of axis I symptoms also appears in table 2. ICCs indicated excellent agreement for all four disorder categories. Symptom agreement across assessment methods was good to excellent for all four disorder categories. Significantly lower agreement regarding symptom number in the test-retest condition than for interrater reliability was found for major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders, and substance use disorders.
The level of agreement in the test-retest condition did not differ as a function of gender, with the exception of anxiety symptoms: reliability across methods was significantly lower for men than for women (ICC=0.30 versus 0.76) (z=2.50, df=1, p<0.01). This may have been due to anxiety symptoms being much less frequent in men than women (mean number of symptoms=0.44 versus 4.70) (Kruskal-Wallis rank ordered χ2=6.36, df=1, p<0.01). Although agreement between the two assessment methods always appeared higher when the time interval between the two assessments was shorter, ICCs between face-to-face and telephone interviews were only statistically higher for shorter intervals for anxiety disorders (ICC=0.94 for shorter intervals versus ICC=0.63 for longer intervals; z=3.64, df=1, p<0.001).

Axis II Disorders

Dimensional scores. The mean axis II dimensional scores are shown in table 3. Mean dimensional scores as a function of assessment method were significantly higher in the telephone interview for cluster A personality disorders and approached significance for cluster C disorders (F=3.80, df=1,59, p=0.06). Differences between first and second interviews were not statistically significant, although differences for cluster A personality disorders approached significance (F=3.56, df=1,59, p=0.06).
Level of agreement. Interrater ICCs for 60 interviews (30 face-to-face and 30 telephone interviews from 36 unique subjects) were computed and appear in table 3. As can be seen, interrater reliability coefficients were excellent.
The test-retest levels of agreement between face-to-face and telephone interviews for axis II disorder clusters are also shown in table 3. Agreement across methods was excellent for dimensional scores that assessed cluster B personality disorders (which had the highest base rates) and for any personality disorder. Agreement was lower, although still acceptable, for dimensional scores that assessed personality disorders from clusters A and C. Reliability in the test-retest condition was significantly lower than interrater reliability for dimensional scores that assessed personality disorders in clusters A and C as well as for any personality disorder. Only the dimensional score that assessed agreement for cluster A personality disorders was significantly below an ICC=0.75, which is the lower range of excellent agreement for a continuous measure.
Dimensional score agreement (ICC) between face-to-face and telephone interviews did not statistically differ as a function of gender or for subjects who were interviewed within a 12-day period (i.e., median time interval) versus subjects who were interviewed over a longer time interval.

DISCUSSION

On the basis of the results presented here, is it possible to conclude that diagnostic information from telephone interviews is comparable to data obtained from a face-to-face assessment? The answer appears to depend on the specific disorder that is being assessed. Test-retest agreement, in which assessment method, interviewers, informant report, and time interval varied, was contrasted with interrater agreement, in which all factors were held constant except interpretation of the subject's report by the interviewer. It was presumed that interrater agreement represented the upper limit of possible agreement between the telephone and face-to-face procedures. Given that agreement was excellent for both the interrater and test-retest conditions, one can assert with considerable confidence that the assessment of anxiety disorders is unaffected by interview method. Assessing major depressive disorder by the telephone also appears justified, although support was not as strong as for anxiety disorders.
The present findings provided strong support for the validity of the axis II telephone assessment format. There was no indication that significantly less axis II psychopathology was reported over the telephone, and excellent test-retest reliability was obtained for “any personality disorder” and for cluster B (i.e., antisocial, borderline, histrionic, narcissistic) personality disorders, which were the most frequently reported and probably included some of the most behaviorally anchored items.
The reliability of the assessment of adjustment disorders with depressed mood, which are usually brief and remit spontaneously, was problematic. However, discrepancies regarding this diagnosis did not appear to be due to either assessment method or order effects. Disagreements probably emerged because of the overlap with major depressive disorder at the upper end of adjustment disorder and with either depression not otherwise specified or no diagnosis at the lower end of the continuum.
The only area of potential concern for telephone assessment involved the diagnosis of alcohol and substance use disorders. Although the repeated interview kappas indicated very good levels of comparability across assessment methods, the telephone condition elicited fewer symptoms of alcohol and substance use disorders. When scheduling the telephone assessment, the interviewer tried to set up a time when the participant could talk in private. This was not always possible. In 19 of the telephone interviews (12 KIDDIE-SADS and seven Personality Disorder Examination interviews), someone else was present during the assessment: child (N=13), spouse or partner (N=4), parent (N=1), or other (N=1). The presence of someone else may have hindered the participant's comfort in reporting drug use. If at all possible, clinicians who work with patients by telephone should schedule a time when they will have complete privacy. Interviewers who use either assessment method need to be especially sensitive to developing rapport with subjects before gathering information regarding drug and alcohol use and also need to convey to the individual the degree to which this information will remain confidential.
While the present findings provide qualified justification for the assumption that comparable data are obtained in telephone and face-to-face assessment formats, there are some concerns with the telephone interview data that temper our enthusiasm. These concerns, however, are not sufficient to override the economic and logistic advantages of telephone interviewing in research, especially the ability of telephone interviewing to maximize participation.
There were small but consistent trends for lower rates of axis I disorders and axis II cluster A psychopathology to be reported in the second interview. Fenig et al. (28) suggested that lower reporting of disorder in the second interview may be due to subject confusion and speculation regarding the purpose of the second interview, desire on the part of the subject to create a more favorable impression at the second assessment, and even the therapeutic effects of the first interview. The present results indicate that the nature of symptoms being assessed has an impact on the order effect. A reduction in reporting from first to second interview was seen most strongly for symptoms of major depressive disorder and substance use disorders.
With only one possible exception, gender did not moderate the degree of agreement across assessment methods. Also, within the time range that was examined, a greater interval between assessments only significantly reduced agreement levels in one instance.
Several limitations to the present study should be noted. First, we are unable to draw any conclusions about disorders with low base rates in the cohort (e.g., schizophrenia, somatoform disorders). Second, we could not evaluate the impact of sex of the interviewer. Third, it is unknown if these findings will generalize to younger and older individuals. Finally, because of the predominancy of Caucasians in the study, the impact of ethnic factors could not be examined. Underreporting of sensitive information on telephone interviews may be greater for African Americans and Hispanics than for Caucasians (12, 29).
There are many positive aspects to the present study. Subjects were interviewed twice in a counterbalanced design, and most of the major disorders were covered. The use of a nonpatient cohort also represents a strength, since disorders in community samples are milder and therefore less consistently reported (30). Psychiatric problems in community samples are more likely to hover around the threshold of diagnosis, which would require the interviewer to make the difficult decision of whether the condition represents a normal variation of functioning or an actual disorder. Thus, the present study is a particularly rigorous test of the comparability of telephone and face-to-face assessment procedures.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3

Footnote

Received Oct. 30, 1996; revision received March 12, 1997; accepted May 16, 1997. From the Oregon Research Institute. Address reprint requests to Dr. Rohde, Oregon Research Institute, 1715 Franklin Blvd., Eugene, OR 97403-1983. Supported in part by NIMH award MH-50522. The authors thank Helen Orvaschel, Ph.D., for her assistance.

References

1.
Endicott J, Spitzer RL: A diagnostic interview: the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35:837–844
2.
Robins LN, Helzer JE, Croughan J, Ratcliff KS: The National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics, and validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981; 38:381–389
3.
Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID), I: history, rationale, and description. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:624–629
4.
Groves R: Telephone Survey Methodology. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1988
5.
Baer L, Brown-Beasley MW, Sorce J, Henriques AI: Computer-assisted telephone administration of a structured interview for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1737–1738
6.
Dorfman RA, Lubben JE, Mayer-Oakes A, Atchison K: Screening for depression among a well elderly population. Social Work 1995; 40:295–304
7.
Schneider SJ, Schwartz MD, Fast J: Computerized, telephone-based health promotion, I: smoking cessation program. Computers and Hum Behavior 1995; 11:135–148
8.
McLaren P: Psychotherapy by telephone: experience of patient and therapist. J Ment Health UK 1992; 1:311–313
9.
Allen A, Hayes J: Patient satisfaction with telemedicine in a rural clinic (letter). Am J Public Health 1994; 84:1693
10.
Jerome L: Assessment by telemedicine (letter). Hosp Community Psychiatry 1993; 44:81
11.
Weeks MF, Kulka RA, Lessler JT, Whitmore K: Personal versus telephone surveys for collecting household data at the local level. Am J Public Health 1983; 73:1389–1394
12.
Aneshensel CS, Frericks RR, Clark VA, Yokopenic PA: Measuring depression in the community: a comparison of telephone and personal interviews. Public Opinion Q 1982; 46:110–121
13.
Sobin E, Weissman MM, Goldstein RB, Adams P: Diagnostic interviewing for family studies: comparing telephone and face-to-face methods for the diagnosis of lifetime psychiatric disorders. Psychiatr Genet 1993; 3:227–233
14.
Wells KB, Burnam MA, Leake B, Robins LN: Agreement between face-to-face and telephone-administered versions of the depression section of the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule. J Psychiatr Res 1988; 22:207–220
15.
Paulsen AS, Crowe RR, Noyes R, Pfohl B: Reliability of the telephone interview in diagnosing anxiety disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:62–63
16.
Robins LN: Epidemiology: reflections on testing the validity of psychiatric interviews. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42:918–924
17.
Wittchen H-U, Burke JD, Semler G, Pfister H, Von Cranch M, Zaudig M: Recall and dating of psychiatric symptoms: test-retest reliability of time-related symptom questions in a standardized psychiatric interview. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989; 46:437–443
18.
Johnson TP, Hougland JG, Moore RW: Sex differences in reporting sensitive behavior: a comparison of interview methods. Sex Roles 1991; 24:669–680
19.
Lewinsohn PM, Hops H, Roberts RE, Seeley JR, Andrews JA: Adolescent psychopathology, I: prevalence and incidence of depression and other DSM-III-R disorders in high school students. J Abnorm Psychol 1993; 102:133–144
20.
Keller MB, Lavori PW, Friedman B, Nielsen E, Endicott J, McDonald-Scott P, Andreasen NC: The Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation: a comprehensive method for assessing outcome in prospective longitudinal studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:540–548
21.
Chambers WJ, Puig-Antich J, Hirsch M, Paez P, Ambrosini PJ, Tabrazi MA, Davies M: The assessment of affective disorders in children and adolescents by semistructured interview: test-retest reliability of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children, Present Episode Version. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42:696–702
22.
Loranger AW, Susman VL, Oldham JM, Russakoff M: The Personality Disorder Examination (PDE) Manual. Yonkers, NY, DV Communications, 1988
23.
Loranger AW, Susman VL, Oldham JM, Russakoff LM: The Personality Disorder Examination: a preliminary report. J Personality Disorders 1987; 1:1–13
24.
Cohen J: A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychol Measurement 1960; 20:37–46
25.
Fleiss JL: Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions, 2nd ed. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1981
26.
Dixon WJ, Brown MB, Engleman L, Hill MA, Jennrich RI: BMDP Statistical Software Manual. Berkeley, University of California Press, 1988
27.
Shrout PE, Fleiss JL: Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull 1979; 86:420–428
28.
Fenig S, Levav I, Kohn R, Yelin N: Telephone vs face-to-face interviewing in a community psychiatric survey. Am J Public Health 1993; 83:896–898
29.
Aquilino WS, Lo Sciuto LA: Effects of interview mode on self-reported drug use. Public Opinion Q 1990; 54:362–395
30.
Bromet EJ, Dunn LO, Connell MM, Dew M, Schulberg HC: Long-term reliability of diagnosing lifetime major depression in a community sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:435–440

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1593 - 1598
PubMed: 9356570

History

Published in print: November 1997
Published online: 1 April 2006

Authors

Details

Peter M. Lewinsohn, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share