Skip to main content

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: This study sought to describe brain regions associated with the personality dimension of introversion/extraversion. METHOD: Measures of cerebral blood flow (CBF) were obtained from 18 healthy subjects by means of [15O]H2O positron emission tomography. Correlations of regional CBF with introversion/extraversion were calculated, and a three-dimensional map of those correlations was generated. RESULTS: Overall, introversion was associated with increased blood flow in the frontal lobes and in the anterior thalamus. Regions in the anterior cingulate gyrus, the temporal lobes, and the posterior thalamus were found to be correlated with extraversion. CONCLUSIONS: The findings of the study lend support to the notion that introversion is associated with increased activity in frontal lobe regions. Moreover, the study suggests that individual differences in introversion and extraversion are related to differences in a fronto-striato-thalamic circuit.
Personality is perhaps the most unique aspect of our individuality. It is a pattern of behavior and reactions that varies among individuals but within an individual is stable over time. Personality has traditionally been conceptualized as consisting of several factors or dimensions. Although there continues to be spirited debate among personality theorists concerning the exact number of dimensions (19), most major current models of personality include the dimension of introversion/extraversion at a fundamental level (10). Not only do most models incorporate introversion/extraversion, but they also have very similar conceptualizations of the construct. For example, Zuckerman, and colleagues (11) found that ratings of extraversion generated by the two most frequently used personality tests, the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (12) and the NEO Personality Inventory—Revised (13), correspond closely. At one end of the introversion/extraversion continuum are extraverts, who are described as gregarious, socially active, cheerful, assertive, and easily excitable. At the other end of the spectrum are introverts, who tend to be reclusive and quiet, preferring books to other people.
There have been several attempts to describe biological bases of normal and abnormal personality (1, 3, 5, 6). Eysenck (35) proposed that behavioral differences between introverts and extraverts result from an innate drive to compensate for overactive and underactive reticulo-thalamo-cortical pathways, respectively. Gray (6, 14) conceptualized extraverts as having lower activity than do introverts in the behavioral inhibition system, a functional loop that includes the ascending reticular activating system, the frontal lobes, septal regions, and the hippocampus. According to both of these models, introverts have higher cortical activity than extraverts, especially in the frontal lobes. The models differ in terms of the mechanisms responsible for differences in cortical activity.
Functional imaging allows researchers to examine metabolic or blood flow correlates of specific cognitive processes and mental states. These technologies have the potential to directly measure functional differences that may exist in the brains of extraverts and introverts.
Few studies have used functional imaging techniques to explore the relation between brain activity and personality in healthy individuals. Mathew et al. (15) examined the relation between extraversion and cerebral blood flow (CBF) in a group of 51 female subjects with the use of xenon inhalation and cortical probes. They found a negative relation between the two variables, with lower overall blood flow in highly extraverted individuals. Stenberg et al. (16) attempted to replicate these findings in a group of 19 male and 18 female subjects with the use of xenon single photon emission computed tomography. Mean CBF was related to extraversion only when the sample was limited to female subjects of the same age as those in the Mathew et al. study. In addition to whole brain blood flow, Stenberg et al. examined regional blood flow and found lower flow in the temporal lobes in extraverts relative to introverts.
Neither of the studies of extraversion mentioned above used standardized stereotaxic coordinate systems or aligned functional imaging data with anatomical magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or computerized tomography images. Thus, these studies were not able to accurately describe the exact location or spatial extent of regional blood flow patterns associated with extraversion. The positron emission tomography (PET) technique provides more precise spatial resolution than the methods described above and is able to measure blood flow from the entire brain. PET has been used extensively to describe the circuitry subserving various aspects of normal cognition. To our knowledge, the present study was the first to utilize PET imaging to examine the relation between regional CBF and the personality dimension of introversion/extraversion in normal individuals.
Normal subjects engaging in a series of loosely connected personal recollections and plans for future activities activate large regions of the frontal lobes (17), while memory retrieval specifically activates the right frontal cortex (1723). To the extent that introverted individuals engage in more cerebral activities (thinking, planning, remembering) than do extraverts, activity in regions associated with these functions, namely the frontal lobes, would be expected to be higher in introverts than in extraverts.

METHOD

The subjects of this study were 18 healthy normal individuals (10 male and eight female) recruited from the community. Their mean age was 28.9 years (SD=8.5, range=19–48), and their mean level of education was 14.2 years (SD=1.7, range=12–18). All subjects were screened for exclusionary criteria including prior psychiatric, neurologic, or serious medical conditions. Before participation all subjects gave written informed consent to the protocols, which were approved by the University of Iowa Human Subjects Institutional Review Board.
Introversion/extraversion scores were taken from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (13), a 60-item abbreviated form of the NEO Personality Inventory—Revised. The extraversion scale of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory correlates highly (r=0.90) with the corresponding scale from the NEO Personality Inventory—Revised. It has demonstrated internal consistency (coefficient alpha=0.77) and test-retest reliability (at 3 months, r=0.79, N=208, p<0.001). In the present study percentile scores were used, based on separate gender norms as suggested in the test manual. Scores on the extraversion scale ranged from 31 to 75 (mean=53.8, SD=12.5). Men and women did not differ significantly in terms of extraversion scores.
Regional CBF was measured using the bolus water ([15O]H2O) method (2426) with a GE 4096 PLUS scanner. The subjects were oriented in the PET scanner with laser light guides aligned at the orbitomeatal line. The center of the most rostral slice was indicated by the laser guides. Fifteen slices (6.5 mm center to center) with an intrinsic in-plane resolution of 6.5 mm full width at half maximum and a 10-cm axial field of view were acquired. Imaging consisted of 20 5-second frames. To determine the time course of bolus transit, time-activity curves were generated for regions of interest placed over major vessels within the brain. The eight frames representing the first 40 seconds immediately after bolus transit were summed to make a composite image. This composite image was reconstructed into 2-mm pixels in a 128×128 matrix with a Butterworth filter (order=6, cutoff frequency=0.35 Nyquist). With use of the above-described image, blood curve (PET counts/pixel per second), and an assumed brain partition coefficient of 0.90, the regional CBF (expressed in milliliters per minute per 100 grams of tissue) was calculated pixel by pixel on the basis of the equation described for the autoradiographic technique (25). For a more detailed description of the method, see Hichwa et al. (27) and Hurtig et al. (28). During image acquisition, subjects were instructed to lie quietly with their eyes closed. No further instructions were given for this condition.
The MRI images consisted of contiguous coronal slices (1.5 mm thick) acquired on a 1.5-T GE Signa scanner. Technical parameters of the MRI acquisition were as follows: spoiled gradient/recall acquisition sequence, flip angle=40°, TE=5 msec, TR=24 msec, number of excitations=2. MRI images were transferred to the Image Processing Laboratory of the University of Iowa Mental Health Clinical Research Center for analysis with the use of Silicon Graphics workstations and locally developed software (BRAINS) (2934.
The initial step of postacquisition processing involved “removing” the brain from the skull with a combination of automated edge detection techniques and manual tracing. Pixels representing surface CSF were classified through a thresholding procedure and removed from the display. To ensure comparability of head position across subjects, the scans were volume rendered, realigned parallel to the anterior commissure/posterior commissure line and the interhemispheric fissure, and placed into standard Talairach coordinate space (35). At this point, images from multiple subjects or multiple scans from a single subject can be coregistered. Finally, images were resliced in three orthogonal planes to produce a three-dimensional data set that was used for visualization and analysis.
The quantitative PET blood flow images were transferred to the Image Processing Laboratory for further analysis (2934). The first step in image analysis involved registration of each individual’s PET and MRI images. This coregistration consisted of a two-stage process. Initially, a coarse fit based on surface matching of the MRI and PET images was generated (36); then, with the surface fit data as input, a variance minimization program (37) was used for the final coregistration. Brain landmarks identified on the MRI were used to place each coregistered image into standardized coordinate space (35). An 18-mm Hanning filter was applied to the PET images to eliminate residual anatomical variability.
Mean whole brain blood flow (milliliters per minute per 100 grams of tissue) was computed from PET flow within the voxels contained in the brain.
Locally developed software similar to that used by other groups (3843) was used to calculate correlational analyses of regional blood flow. Resolution was reduced from a voxel size of 1.08 mm × 1.35 mm × 1.48 mm to 3.00 mm × 2.70 mm × 2.00 mm. Voxel data are treated as a collection of data vectors; the length is defined by the number of subjects. A Pearson correlation between blood flow within each voxel and introversion/extraversion scores was calculated; this resulted in a three-dimensional correlation matrix displaying correlation coefficients for every voxel. This correlation image presents an overall pattern of the relation between blood flow and introversion/extraversion. A threshold can be applied so that only regions with a correlation coefficient above a certain value are displayed. Selection of a threshold in imaging research remains difficult because of the sheer number of voxels involved in the correlational analysis and the lack of independence of contiguous voxels. For the present study, a correlation threshold of 0.60 (p<0.005, uncorrected for multiple comparisons) with introversion/extraversion and a size criterion of at least 10 contiguous voxels were selected.

RESULTS

To investigate the relation between introversion/extraversion and whole brain blood flow in this study group, a Pearson correlation coefficient was computed. Contrary to previous findings (15, 16), introversion/extraversion did not account for a significant amount of the variance in whole brain blood flow (r=–0.21, N=18, p=0.20).
The analysis identified 17 regions that exceeded a correlation of 0.60 with introversion/extraversion and a size of 10 contiguous voxels. The locations of these regions in standardized space as well as the size and correlation coefficient of the center voxel are presented in table 1. It should be noted that the correlational analysis was performed with the use of the entire continuum of introversion/extraversion scores. For ease of discussion, areas that were positively correlated with scores on the NEO Personality Inventory—Revised will be described as regions associated with extraversion. Similarly, areas negatively correlated with scores on the NEO Personality Inventory—Revised will be described as regions associated with introversion.
There were eight regions correlated with introversion and seven correlated with extraversion. Regions showing a relationship with introversion had a larger volume than did regions associated with extraversion (10.7 cc versus 4 cc).
In line with predictions, introversion was correlated with blood flow in the lateral extent of the frontal cortex, Broca’s area, the insular cortex, the right temporal cortex, and the anterior nucleus of the thalamus. Further, several cortical regions were found to be correlated with extraversion. These regions included the anterior cingulate gyrus, right insular cortex, bilateral temporal lobes, and pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus. Figure 1 and figure 2 depict regions correlated with introversion and extraversion, respectively.

DISCUSSION

This study revealed a pattern of increased blood flow in the frontal lobes associated with introversion. This pattern generally supports both Eysenck’s and Gray’s biological theories of personality (46, 14) and is consistent with results of previous studies (15, 16, 44, 45). Eysenck’s model (4, 5) proposes that introverts have higher cortical activity than extraverts; indeed, in the present study, there were more cortical regions associated with introversion than with extraversion. Gray suggested lower than normal activity in the behavioral inhibition system in extraverts; in the present study, extraverts did show lower blood flow in several regions in the behavioral inhibition system, namely, the frontal lobes and the hippocampus.
Blood flow measures were acquired while subjects were free to think about anything, providing a picture of the activity of the undirected and uncensored mind. Normal subjects have reported engaging in a series of loosely connected personal recollections and plans for future activities during the uncontrolled cognitive state (17). Higher flow in bilateral frontal lobe regions suggests that introverts were engaged in frontally based cognition, including remembering events from their past, making plans for the future, or problem solving. In addition, Gale (46) speculated that introverts might engage in a running monologue in the absence of external stimulation. The observed increased blood flow in Broca’s area in introverts might be interpreted as biological evidence of “self-talk.”
The present study is the first to describe thalamic regions associated with introversion/extraversion. The thalamus has long been recognized as an obligatory relay station for sensory information headed for the primary sensory cortices. Changes in the activity of the thalamic nuclei have the potential to decrease or increase the amount of information reaching cortical regions. Two regions, a negatively correlated region in the anterior nucleus and a positively correlated region in the pulvinar nucleus, were identified. The anterior thalamus projects primarily to the anterior cingulate gyrus, while the pulvinar nucleus projects mainly to visual cortex and sensory association areas (47, 48).
This study found that blood flow in the anterior insula is correlated with introversion, whereas blood flow in the posterior insula is correlated with extraversion. Recent studies of emotion have found that the anterior portion of the insula is activated when subjects recall or imagine events, whereas the posterior insula is activated by interpretation of current sensory information (49, 50). The distinction between an inward focus and an outward focus is very much the difference between introverts and extraverts. Stenberg et al. (16) reported that extraverts have higher flow in the temporal lobes than do introverts, but because of technological limitations they were not able to localize this region more accurately. It is likely that the increased flow in extraverts was based in the posterior insula.
The greater activity in the anterior thalamic nuclei and frontal lobe regions in introverts likely reflects the introspective nature of these individuals. The posterior thalamus and anterior cingulate regions that were found to be more active in extraverts may underlie these individuals’ high drive for sensory and emotional stimulation.
Although this study used a resting condition, there are many other equally valid conditions for assessing differences in CBF associated with extraversion. Future research should explore these differences in the context of various cognitive tasks, e.g., memory paradigms. Future research could also explore the relation between CBF and other personality dimensions for which biological substrates might be hypothesized (e.g., neuroticism).
While the PET technique allows researchers to explore the relation between activity in brain regions and various cognitive states and brain illnesses, it is not without limitations. Although state-of-the-art analysis techniques are used, the problems of possible edge artifacts, imperfect image registration, and limited spatial resolution remain.
The present study did not provide a definitive answer concerning the relation between personality and brain activity. It does, however, add to a growing literature which suggests that human personality traits are based on individual differences in brain function. This study, combined with others, suggests that frontal cortical regions are highly active in introverts, while more posterior regions are active in extraverts. Moreover, findings of this study suggest that a circuit involving the frontal lobes, the striatum, and the thalamus plays an integral role in modulating individual differences in extraversion.
TABLE 1
FIGURE 1. Brain Regions Correlated With Introversion in a PET Study of 18 Healthy Normal Subjectsa
aImages are presented according to radiologic convention with the left hemisphere displayed on the right side of the image. Three orthogonal planes are shown, transaxial on top, sagittal in the middle, and coronal on the bottom, with the green crosshairs indicating the location of the slice. The right window displays all regions that exceeded the preset correlation threshold within a transparent brain. In this set of images, regions are seen as if one were looking through the brain in each of three planes. The left window displays a representative region from the image on the left in three orthogonal planes. The scale on the right reflects the continuum of correlation coefficients. Positive correlation coefficients (those in the red or yellow spectrum) reflect areas correlated with introversion, while negative correlation coefficients (those in the blue or violet spectrum) reflect areas correlated with extraversion. The pattern of correlations between blood flow and the extraversion/introversion dimension reveals that predominantly frontal cortex regions are associated with introversion. These regions are best visualized in the axial and sagittal planes. Activity in these frontal regions is thought to mediate memory retrieval and planning, reflecting the inward focus of cognitive processes in introverts. The significant activation in the anterior thalamus is appreciable in all planes. The axial view shows an activation in Broca’s area that likely reflects “self-talk” on the part of introverted individuals.
FIGURE 2. Brain Regions Correlated With Extraversion in a PET Study of 18 Healthy Normal Subjectsa
aImages are presented according to radiologic convention with the left hemisphere displayed on the right side of the image. Three orthogonal planes are shown, transaxial on top, sagittal in the middle, and coronal on the bottom, with the green crosshairs indicating the location of the slice. The right window displays all regions that exceeded the preset correlation threshold within a transparent brain. In this set of images, regions are seen as if one were looking through the brain in each of three planes. The left window displays a representative region from the image on the left in three orthogonal planes. The scale on the right reflects the continuum of correlation coefficients. Positive correlation coefficients (those in the red or yellow spectrum) reflect areas correlated with introversion, while negative correlation coefficients (those in the blue or violet spectrum) reflect areas correlated with extraversion.
The anterior cingulate region that is correlated with extraversion is seen in the axial and sagittal sections. The axial and sagittal views in the left set of images show the region in the posterior thalamus (pulvinar nucleus). It is thought that these regions play a role in sensation seeking. Other regions correlated with extraversion include the posterior parietal lobe, right insula, and left amygdala.

Footnote

Received Sept. 12, 1997; revision received July 8, 1998; accepted Aug. 4, 1998. From the Mental Health Clinical Research Center and the Departments of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City. Address reprint requests to Dr. Johnson, Department of Psychology, Seashore Hall, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242. Supported in part by NIMH grants MH-31593, MH-40856, and MHCRC-43271.

References

1.
Cloninger CR: A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatr Dev 1986; 3:167–226
2.
Costa PT, McCrae RR: Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences 1992; 13:653–665
3.
Eysenck HJ: The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield, Ill, Charles C Thomas, 1967
4.
Eysenck HJ: A Model for Personality. New York, Springer-Verlag, 1981
5.
Eysenck HJ: Psychophysiology and personality: extraversion, neuroticism and psychoticism, in Physiological Correlates of Human Behavior, vol 3: Individual Differences and Psychopathology. Edited by Gale A, Edwards JA. Orlando, Fla, Academic Press, 1986, pp 13–30
6.
Gray JA: The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behav Res Ther 1970; 8:249–266
7.
Gray JA: A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality, in A Model for Personality. Edited by Eysenck HJ. New York, Springer-Verlag, 1981, pp 246-276
8.
Millon T: Disorders of Personality: DSM-III, Axis II. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1981
9.
Millon T: A theoretical derivation of pathological personalities, in Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Toward the DSM-IV. Edited by Millon T, Klerman GL. New York, Guilford Press, 1985, pp 639-670
10.
Zuckerman M: What is a basic factor and which factors are basic? turtles all the way down. Personality and Individual Differences 1992; 13:675–681
11.
Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM, Joireman J, Teta P, Kraft M: A comparison of three structural models for personality: The Big Three, the Big Five, and the Alternative Five. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993; 65:757–768
12.
Eysenck SBG, Eysenck HJ: Manual of the EPQ. New York, Crane, Russak, 1975
13.
Costa PT, McCrae RR: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychol Assessment Resources, 1992
14.
Gray JA: The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion: a modification of Eysenck’s theory, in The Biological Bases of Individual Behavior. Edited by Nebylitsyn VD, Gray JA. New York, Academic Press, 1972, pp 182–205
15.
Mathew RJ, Weinman ML, Barr DL: Personality and regional cerebral blood flow. Br J Psychiatry 1984; 144:529–532
16.
Stenberg G, Risberg J, Warkentin S, Rosen I: Regional patterns of cortical blood flow distinguish extraverts from introverts. Personality and Individual Differences 1990; 11:663–673
17.
Andreasen NC, O"Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD: Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1576–1585
18.
Andreasen NC, O"Leary DS, Arndt S, Cizadlo T, Rezai K, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD: PET studies of memory, I: novel and practiced free recall of complex narratives. Neuroimage 1995; 2:284–295
19.
Andreasen NC, O"Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD: PET studies of memory, II: novel versus practiced free recall of word lists. Neuroimage 1995; 2:296–305
20.
Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Shallice T, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ: Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory. Brain 1995; 118:401–416
21.
Kapur S, Craik FIM, Tulving E, Wilson AA, Houle S, Brown GM: Neuroanatomical correlates of encoding in episodic memory: levels of processing effect. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1994; 91:2008–2011
22.
Shallice T, Fletcher P, Frith CD, Grasby P, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ: Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory. Nature 1994; 368:633–635
23.
Tulving E, Kapur S, Markowitch HJ, Craik FI, Habib R, Houle S: Neuroanatomical correlates of retrieval in episodic memory: auditory sentence recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1994; 91:2012–2015
24.
Ginsberg MD, Lockwood AH, Busto R, Finn R: A simplified in vivo autoradiographic strategy for the determination of regional cerebral blood flow by position emission tomography: theoretical considerations and validation studies in the rat. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1982; 2:89–98
25.
Herscovitch P, Markham J, Raichle ME: Brain blood flow measured with intravenous H215O, I: theory and error analysis. J Nucl Med 1983; 24:782–789
26.
Raichle ME, Martin WRW, Herscovitch P: Brain blood flow measured with H215O, II: implementation and validation. J Nucl Med 1983; 24:790–798
27.
Hichwa RD, Ponto LLB, Watkins GL: Clinical blood flow measurements with [15O] water and positron emission tomography (PET), in Chemists’ Views of Imaging Centers: Proceedings of an ACS Symposium Held in Chicago, Illinois, August 22–27, 1993. Edited by Emran AM. New York, Plenum, 1995, pp 401–417
28.
Hurtig RR, Hichwa RD, O’Leary DS, Boles Ponto LL, Nara­yana S, Watkins GL, Andreasen NC: Effects of timing and duration of cognitive activation in [15O]water PET studies. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1994; 14:423–430
29.
Andreasen NC, Cohen G, Harris G, Cizadlo T, Parkkinen J, Rezai K, Swayze VW II: Image processing for the study of brain structure and function: problems and programs. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1992; 4:125–133
30.
Andreasen NC, Cizadlo T, Harris G, Swayze V II, O"Leary DS, Cohen G, Ehrhardt J, Yuh WT: Voxel processing techniques for the antemortem study of neuroanatomy and neuropathology using magnetic resonance imaging. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1993; 5:121–130
31.
Andreasen NC, Harris G, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, O"Leary D, Swayze V, Flaum M: Techniques for measuring sulcal/gyral patterns in the brain as visualized through magnetic resonance scanning: BRAINPLOT and BRAINMAP. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1994; 91:93–97
32.
Andreasen NC, Rajarethinam R, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Swayze VW II, Flashman LA, O"Leary DS, Ehrhardt JC, Yuh WTC: Automatic atlas-based volume estimation of human brain regions from MR images. J Comp Assist Tomogr 1996; 20:98–106
33.
Cizadlo T, Andreasen NC, Zeien G, Rajaprabhakaran R, Harris G, O"Leary D, Swayze V, Arndt S, Hichwa R, Ehrhardt J, Yuh WTC: Image registration issues in the analysis of multiple-injection 15O H2O PET studies: BRAINFIT. Proceedings of the Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation Engineers 1994; 2168:423–430
34.
Cohen G, Andreasen NC, Alliger R, Arndt S, Kuan J, Yuh WTC, Ehrhardt J: Segmentation techniques for the classification of brain tissue using magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 1992; 45:33–51
35.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York, Thieme Medical, 1988
36.
Levin DN, Pelizzari CA, Chen GTY, Chen CT, Cooper MD: Retrospective geometric correlations of MR, CT, and PET images. Radiology 1988; 169:817–823
37.
Woods RP, Cherry SR, Mazziotta JC: Rapid automated algorithm for aligning and reslicing PET images. J Comp Assist Tomogr 1992; 16:620–633
38.
Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS: Investigating a network model of word generation with positron emission tomography. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1991; 244:101–106
39.
Frith CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RSJ: A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia 1991; 29:1137–1148
40.
Horwitz B, Sporns O: Neural mapping and functional imaging. Human Brain Mapping 1994; 1:269–283
41.
Horwitz B: Data analysis paradigms for metabolic-flow data: combining neural modeling and functional imaging. Human Brain Mapping 1994; 2:112–122
42.
Silberswieg DA, Stern E, Schnorr L, Frith CD, Ashburner J, Cahill C, Frackowiak RS, Jones T: Imaging transient, randomly occurring neuropsychological events in single subjects with positron emission tomography: an event-related count rate correlational analysis. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1994; 14:771–782
43.
Zeki S, Watson JDG, Lueck CJ, Friston KJ, Kennard C, Frackowiak RSJ: A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 1991; 11:641–649
44.
Daruna JH, Karrer R, Rosen AJ: Introversion, attention and the late positive component of event-related potentials. Biol Psychol 1985; 20:249–259
45.
Deakin JFW, Exley KA: Personality and male-female influences on the EEG alpha rhythm. Biol Psychol 1979; 8:285–290
46.
Gale A: Electroencephalographic studies of extraversion-introversion: a case study of the physiology of individual differences. Personality and Individual Differences 1983; 4:371–380
47.
Jones EG: The Thalamus. New York, Plenum, 1985
48.
Williams PM, Warwick R: Functional Neuroanatomy of Man. Edinburgh, Churchill Livingstone, 1975
49.
Kosslyn S, Shin L, Thompson W, McNally RJ, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, Alpert NM: Neural effects of visualizing and perceiving aversive stimuli: a PET investigation. Neuroreport 1996; 7:1569–1576
50.
Lane RD, Reiman EM, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ: Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:926–933

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 252 - 257
PubMed: 9989562

History

Published online: 1 February 1999
Published in print: February 1999

Authors

Details

Debra L. Johnson, Ph.D.
Nancy C. Andreasen, M.D., Ph.D.
Richard D. Hichwa, Ph.D.
G. Leonard Watkins, Ph.D.
Laura L. Boles Ponto, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share