Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective: Processing affective facial expressions is an important component of interpersonal relationships. However, depressed patients show impairments in this system. The present study investigated the neural correlates of implicit processing of happy facial expressions in depression and identified regions affected by antidepressant therapy. Method: Two groups of subjects participated in a prospective study with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The patients were 19 medication-free subjects (mean age, 43.2 years) with major depression, acute depressive episode, unipolar subtype. The comparison group contained 19 matched healthy volunteers (mean age, 42.8 years). Both groups underwent fMRI scans at baseline (week 0) and at 8 weeks. Following the baseline scan, the patients received treatment with fluoxetine, 20 mg daily. The fMRI task was implicit affect recognition with standard facial stimuli morphed to display varying intensities of happiness. The fMRI data were analyzed to estimate the average activation (overall capacity) and differential response to variable intensity (dynamic range) in brain systems involved in processing facial affect. Results: An attenuated dynamic range of response in limbic-subcortical and extrastriate visual regions was evident in the depressed patients, relative to the comparison subjects. The attenuated extrastriate cortical activation at baseline was increased following antidepressant treatment, and symptomatic improvement was associated with greater overall capacity in the hippocampal and extrastriate regions. Conclusions: Impairments in the neural processing of happy facial expressions in depression were evident in the core regions of affective facial processing, which were reversed following treatment. These data complement the neural effects observed with negative affective stimuli.
A crucial component of interpersonal communication is the ability to correctly perceive affective facial expressions (1) . However, patients suffering from an acute episode of depression have a diminished ability to distinguish facial affects, in particular those of happiness and sadness (24) . Patients have difficulty in recognizing happy facial expressions (4) and instead misidentify them as sad (5) .
We previously reported on the neural correlates of implicit processing of sad facial expressions in a longitudinal study using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (6) . During an acute depressive episode, sad facial expressions were associated with greater activity in the amygdala, ventral striatum, and frontoparietal cortices than was observed in healthy comparison subjects. Following treatment with fluoxetine, there was an attenuation of this activity in patients to a level comparable with that in the comparison subjects, who had also undergone follow-up scans. The affected regions were consistent with neuropsychological models of facial emotional processing (79) and, more broadly, with antidepressant treatment studies in depression (1012) . The present study was a complementary investigation of happy facial expressions in the same subjects.
Awareness of the emotional tone of a facial expression begins at an implicit level (7), and the processing of facial affects is distinct from that of facial identities (8) . Haxby et al. (9) proposed that the processing of facial expressions and the recognition of facial identity are served by discrete but overlapping neural systems. The model describes a hierarchical structure consisting of a core system for initial visual analysis and extended neural streams for facial affective expressions and for identity recognition. The core system comprises the extrastriate cortices bilaterally, which include the inferior occipital gyrus, lateral fusiform gyrus, and superior temporal sulcus, and the affective processing system that measures the emotional tone of the facial expression includes the parietotemporal cortices, amygdala, insula, and limbic regions.
Within the core system, Surguladze et al. (13) observed a linear relationship of activation in the extrastriate regions to increasing intensities of emotional facial expressions, indicating that modulation of the neural network begins at this early stage of processing. In the affective processing system, the amygdala has a key role in the recognition of emotions, principally of negative valences, such as fear (14, 15) . Processing facial expressions of happy affective valences, in contrast, engages subcortical regions in the basal ganglia (15) . In healthy individuals, activation in the ventral striatum and putamen have been consistently reported with happy faces (1518), while individuals suffering from depression (19) have shown low activation in response to happy faces in subcortical and limbic regions, in a cluster that encompassed the thalamus, caudate nucleus, putamen, amygdala, and hippocampal region. The observation by Lawrence et al. (19) was a cross-sectional study of patients with depression, all of whom were receiving antidepressant medication.
In the present study we report the investigation of the neural correlates of happy facial processing in a longitudinal study of depression. Medication-free patients participated in fMRI scans during an acute episode and following 8 weeks of treatment with the antidepressant fluoxetine, and healthy comparison subjects underwent the same scans at the same time points. During the scans, the subjects viewed a standardized series of faces (20) that were morphed to present different intensities of happiness. Using this activation procedure, we were able to estimate both the mean difference between the affective stimuli and baseline trials and the dynamic range of brain response to affective intensity. The design of the experiment allowed the identification of brain regions that showed significant effects of group (depressed patients versus healthy volunteers), time (baseline versus week 8), and the interaction of group and time (indicative of an antidepressant treatment effect) for both the overall capacity and dynamic range of response to happy faces.
We hypothesized that patients with depression would have less neural response to happy faces than healthy comparison subjects in the basal ganglia and limbic regions, namely the caudate, putamen, and hippocampal regions. More speculatively, we expected that patients with depression would show less neural response in the extrastriate cortices than would healthy individuals.

Method

Subject Characteristics

The study groups and experimental design have been described previously (6) . Briefly, participants meeting the DSM-IV criteria for major depressive disorder (21), assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (22), were recruited through local newspaper advertisements. The inclusion criteria were an acute episode of major depressive disorder, unipolar subtype, and a score of at least 18 on the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D) (23), without comorbid psychiatric disorders or medical illnesses. All patients were free of psychotropic medication for a minimum of 4 weeks at recruitment. Nineteen patients completed the study, 13 of whom were women. The patients’ mean age was 43.2 years (SD=8.8), their mean full IQ was 109.2 (SD=14.5), and the baseline HAM-D score was 21.1 (SD=2.3). The healthy comparison subjects were recruited by advertisement in the community, were matched by age, gender, and IQ, and had no history of psychiatric or medical disorders. Of the 19 comparison subjects, 11 were women. Their mean age was 42.8 years (SD=6.7), their mean full IQ was 116.4 (SD=18.8), and their baseline HAM-D score was 0.3 (SD=0.7). All subjects provided written informed consent in accordance with the guidelines of the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley National Health Services (NHS) Trust Ethics (Research) Committee.

Experimental Design

In a parallel-group, repeated-measures design, all subjects were scanned in four separate sessions. In the first session, the subjects were familiarized with the MRI unit and underwent a structural MRI for neuroradiological examination. The subjects then participated in three separate 60–90-minute fMRI sessions: at baseline or week 0, at 2 weeks after baseline, and at 8 weeks after baseline. During each session the subjects performed a number of activation tasks, and we have previously reported the results of a formally analogous test of neural processing of sad facial affect in the same subjects (6) . In the current article, we present the data acquired at week 0 and week 8 for the happy facial affect task.
The depressed patients received antidepressant treatment with fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, 20 mg orally once daily. The treatment was begun as soon as possible after the baseline fMRI session (typically less than 1 day) and continued until completion of the study protocol 8 weeks later. The patients were clinically assessed every 2 weeks by a psychiatrist (C.H.Y.F. or A.J.C.) for the study duration, and depressive symptoms were serially rated by using the HAM-D. All patients recruited into the study were able to complete the protocol.

Test for Implicit Recognition of Happy Facial Affect

Ten faces (five male) from a standardized series of happy facial expressions (20) were morphed to represent three intensities of happiness: low, medium, and high ( Figure 1 ). In an event-related fMRI task, facial stimuli and baseline trials (crosshair fixation) were presented in random order. Each stimulus was presented twice at each intensity (60 faces in total), along with 12 baseline trials (crosshair visual fixation point), for a total of 72 trials. Each condition was presented for 3 seconds with the intertrial interval randomly varied according to a Poisson distribution, with a mean intertrial interval of 5 seconds, for a total duration of 360 seconds (6 minutes). The same stimulus set was used at baseline and at 8 weeks.
Figure 1. Examples of Happy Facial Stimuli Used in fMRI Procedure a
a Ten faces from a standardized series were morphed by computer to express discriminable low, medium, and high intensities of happiness. Each of these 30 stimuli was presented twice, along with 12 baseline trials (crosshair visual fixation point), for a total of 72 trials. During the facial trials, subjects indicated whether the face was male or female. Each stimulus was presented for 3 seconds, and the mean interval between trials was 5 seconds.
For each facial trial, the subject was asked to indicate the gender of the face (male or female) by lateral movement of a joystick; no hand movement was required in response to a baseline trial. Reaction time and accuracy of gender decision during scanning were recorded for each trial.

fMRI Data Acquisition

Gradient echo single-shot echoplanar imaging was used to continuously acquire 180 T 2 *-weighted image volumes on a neuro-optimized 1.5-T IGE LX System (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wis.) at the Maudsley Hospital, South London and Maudsley National Health Service Trust, London, for the duration of the procedure. For each volume, 16 noncontiguous axial planes parallel to the intercommissural plane were collected with the following characteristics: TR=2000 msec, TE=40 msec, slice thickness=7 mm, slice skip=0.7 mm, in-plane resolution=3×3 mm.

fMRI Data Analysis

Following correction of differences in slice timing and the effects of head movements (24), linear regression was used to estimate experimentally induced signal changes. Each column of the regression matrix was convolved with a pair of Poisson kernels (λ=4 or 8 seconds). Statistic maps, or t maps, representing each of these two standardized effects for each individual at each scan session were registered in the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux (25) by an affine transformation to a template image (26) .
Regression analysis modeled two mutually orthogonal aspects of brain activation at each voxel: 1) overall facial processing capacity, i.e., the mean difference in elicited response between baseline trials and all facial trials taken together, and 2) facial processing dynamic range, or load-response, i.e., linear change in response elicited by facial trials presented at low, medium, and high levels of affective load.
Factorial effects of interest were identified in a 2×2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) model with a main effect of group (patients versus comparison subjects), main effect of time (week 0 versus week 8), and group-by-time interaction. To identify brain regions associated with symptomatic response, the change in HAM-D symptom score for each patient over 8 weeks of treatment was regressed on the corresponding change in overall affective capacity estimated over the same time period, by subtracting the t map at week 0 from the corresponding map at week 8.
The statistical significance of these factorial effects and regression coefficients was decided by a cluster-level permutation test that involved applying a preliminary probability threshold (p<0.05) to the voxel statistic maps and shrinking all suprathreshold voxels toward zero, thus creating a set of suprathreshold voxel clusters that were spatially contiguous in three dimensions. The sum of suprathreshold voxel statistics was tested by a permutation test (27) with a clusterwise probability of type I error of p<0.005. At this size of test and over the search volume of clusters tested, less than one false positive cluster is expected per map. Further methodological details can be accessed at http://www-bmu.psychiatry.cam.ac.uk/bamm.

Results

The behavioral and fMRI data are summarized in the following. Further details are available as supplementary material at http://www-bmu.psychiatry.cam.ac.uk/~js369/pubabcd_happy/pububdex.html.

Behavioral Data

ANOVA showed a main effect of group on latency (F=6.6, df=1, 34, p<0.02): the reaction time over all trials was longer for the patient group than for the healthy comparison group. There was also a main effect of time on latency (F=6.1, df=1, 34, p<0.02): all subjects showed a shorter reaction time at week 8 than at week 0. There were no significant effects of affective intensity nor any significant interactions for latency.
For accuracy of explicit gender recognition, there was a significant effect of group (F=5.3, df=1, 34, p<0.03): the patients were less accurate at identifying the gender of the facial stimuli than the healthy comparison subjects. There was also a main effect of intensity: the subjects were less accurate at the low intensity of the happy facial expressions (F=27.6, df=2, 33, p<0.001). The interaction of intensity with group fell short of significance (F=3.2, df=2, 33, p<0.054). There was no main effect of time, nor any significant interaction effects for the accuracy of gender judgment.

Main Effect of Group

The patients had a smaller dynamic range than the comparison subjects in the right putamen, bilateral thalamus, bilateral hippocampal region (left>right), left lingual gyrus, precuneus, posterior cingulate gyrus, and right cerebellum ( Figure 2, Table 1 ). The main effect of group reflected an attenuated response in dynamic range at both the baseline and week 8 follow-up scans in the patients relative to the comparison subjects. There were no differences in overall capacity between groups.
Figure 2. Differences in Brain Responses to Happy Faces Between 19 Depressed Patients and 19 Healthy Comparison Subjects and Effect of Treatment a
a Each map is presented in radiological convention. The crosshair locates the origin of the x and y dimensions, and the numeral indicates the location in the z dimension of Talairach space.
b The top panel shows a smaller dynamic range in the patients than in the comparison subjects in the right putamen, bilateral thalamus, bilateral hippocampal region, left lingual gyrus, precuneus, posterior cingulate gyrus, and cerebellum. There were no differences in overall capacity between groups. The boxes on the right indicate interquartile ranges, the horizontal lines indicate medians, the limit lines indicate ranges excluding outliers, and the circles represent outliers, which were defined as points more than 1.5 times the interquartile range from the upper (or lower) limit of the interquartile range.
c The middle panel displays a significant group-by-time interaction for overall capacity in the cerebellum and extrastriate cortical regions: bilaterally in the lingual gyri and cuneus, extending to the precuneus and posterior cingulate gyrus. The overall capacity in the response to happy faces was significantly lower in the acutely depressed patients than in the healthy subjects at baseline, and it increased significantly in the patients after antidepressant treatment. There was no significant group-by-time interaction for the dynamic range of response.
d The bottom panel displays significant associations between changes in the HAM-D score and overall capacity in the left lingual gyrus, hippocampal region, and cerebellum. These regions showed significant positive correlations such that patients who had the greatest clinical improvement after treatment showed the greatest overall capacity in happy face processing. The regional correlation between the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response and the change in HAM-D score with treatment is presented in the graph.

Interaction of Group and Time (Effect of Antidepressant Treatment)

A significant interaction for overall capacity was observed in the cerebellum and extrastriate cortical regions: bilaterally in the lingual gyri and cuneus, extending to the precuneus and posterior cingulate gyrus. Post hoc analysis indicated that the overall capacity for processing happy faces was significantly lower in the acutely depressed patients than in the healthy subjects at baseline (F=7.6, df=1, 36, p<0.009) and that it increased significantly in the patients after antidepressant treatment (F=7.0, df=1, 36, p<0.02) ( Figure 2, Table 2 ). There was no significant group-by-time interaction for the dynamic range of response.

Relationship Between Symptomatic Response and Overall Capacity

There was a significant association between the change in HAM-D score and overall capacity in the following regions of the left brain: lingual gyrus, hippocampal region (including the parahippocampal gyrus), and cerebellum. These regions showed a significant positive correlation such that the patients who had the greatest clinical improvement after treatment showed the greatest overall capacity in happy face processing ( Figure 2, Table 3 ).

Main Effects of Time

There was a significant reduction over time in overall capacity bilaterally in the anterior and posterior cingulate gyri, medial prefrontal cortices, inferior and middle frontal cortices, precentral gyri, right inferior parietal cortex, left precuneus and cuneus, and cerebellum. A significant reduction in dynamic range over time was found in the following regions in the right brain: lingual and inferior occipital cortices, hippocampal region, inferior frontal cortex, insula, inferior parietal and superior temporal cortices, precentral gyri, and cerebellum. All figures and tables are available as supplementary material at http://www-bmu.psychiatry.cam.ac.uk/DATA.

Discussion

Impairments in affective facial processing have significant implications for interpersonal relationships in depression (1) . Although happy expressions are the most easily recognized of the affective facial expressions, they are typically misconstrued by patients suffering from depression (25) . We previously reported the neural correlates associated with sad facial expressions in patients with depression during an acute episode and following treatment, as compared with a matched group of healthy comparison subjects who participated in the same scans at the same time points (6) . The present study is a complementary investigation of the neural correlates of implicit processing of happy faces. The most striking observation was that the patients had less neural response to happy faces than did the healthy volunteers. This is in marked contrast to the processing of sad faces, in which neural activations were abnormally exaggerated in depression (6) . Moreover, the core regions involved in face processing subsequently showed significant increases in activation in response to happy faces following antidepressant treatment, and baseline activation in the hippocampal region was significantly associated with patient clinical response.

Main Effect of Group

In relation to the healthy subjects, the patients with depression showed a smaller dynamic range of responses to happy facial expressions in limbic, subcortical, and extrastriate regions. The hippocampus is part of the “limbic cortex” and has a crucial role in normal and impaired affective functioning (2831) . In healthy volunteers, Critchley et al. (18) observed increased activation in the hippocampal region during implicit processing of happy faces. The present study suggests that hippocampal function in response to happy faces is impaired in depression. In addition, the hippocampus is engaged in memory encoding and retrieval of both facial identity and affective facial expressions (32) . There may have been some involvement of memory processes in the present study, as the stimuli consisted of a series of faces of varying intensities that were randomly presented and included several repetitions of each expression.
The patients with depression also showed a smaller dynamic range in the putamen. In healthy individuals, activation of the putamen is consistently reported with happy faces (16) and pleasant pictures (33), as well as during memory recall following induction of a happy mood (34) . In patients with depression, reduced activation has been observed with happy faces (19), and more recently there has been a report of a smaller dynamic range than in healthy individuals (35) . The ventral striatum and putamen are implicated in reward and motivation (36) . Facial expressions provide social cues (1), which may be reflected by implicit imitation in the observer (37), and lesions in the basal ganglia lead to impaired recognition of affective facial expressions (38) . For healthy volunteers, the task of viewing happy faces is highly salient and may be experienced as pleasant (1), a process that is impaired in patients with depression (25) . The basal ganglia receive projections from the neocortex and have reciprocal connections via the thalamus (39) . An additional observation in the present study was a smaller dynamic range in the thalamus among patients with depression than among healthy volunteers. This contrasts with our finding for sad faces, whereby patients with depression showed greater activation in subcortical regions than did healthy volunteers (6) .
Extrastriate cortical activation is elicited by emotional scenes (40) and facial expressions (13, 41), and it appears to be driven by the affective nature of the stimuli rather than by their complexity (42) or by eye movements (43) . Moreover, Surguladze et al. (13, 35) reported a linear association of activation in extrastriate regions with increasing intensity of happy facial expressions in healthy volunteers, which we also observed in the present study, while patients with depression showed an impaired dynamic range.
Our finding of a smaller dynamic range in limbic, subcortical, and extrastriate regions provides a neural substrate for the behavioral expression of impaired recognition of happy faces in depression. Notably, the attenuation in dynamic range was evident at both the baseline and follow-up scans, indicating that impairments in regions of the emotional processing network of happy facial expressions not only are present during an acute depressive episode but persist into the remission phase. This suggests that the impairment in these regions may be a trait feature of depression, which has implications for its pathophysiology, clinical course, and treatment strategies.

Effects of Antidepressant Treatment

The lingual gyrus and cuneus showed impaired activation in the depressed patients at baseline, then a significant improvement following 8 weeks of treatment, which suggests an improvement in their implicit processing of happy faces. Activity in extrastriate regions has been proposed to be mediated through the amygdala (44), which is a key site of impairment in models of depression (2831) . Although no differences in amygdala activation were observed in the present study, previously we noted changes in its activity in response to sad facial expression (6) . In the present study, there may nonetheless have been modulation of extrastriate activity by the amygdala that would be measurable by an effective connectivity analysis.

Relationship Between Symptomatic Response and Overall Capacity

In the patient group, some regions also showed a significant relationship with clinical response to treatment. Greater overall capacity in the left hippocampal region in medication-free patients at baseline was associated with a greater improvement in clinical symptoms following antidepressant treatment. Several researchers have observed a high level of limbic-subcortical activity in depression that is attenuated after antidepressant treatment (10, 11), a feature that may be specific to pharmacological therapy, as this has not been observed with cognitive behavior therapy (45) . The hippocampus is a key node in models of depression (2831), and high levels of activity in the hippocampal region at baseline may indicate a resilience in neural responsivity in those patients who subsequently showed a clinical improvement.
The engagement of the hippocampus in the present study also indicates a specificity in the affected neural regions that may be task related, as we previously observed a positive relationship between treatment response and activation response to sad faces in other regions, namely the anterior cingulate cortex, striatum, and cerebellum (6) . Seminowicz et al. (46) reported significant differences in the effective connectivity between the hippocampus and subgenual cingulate cortex in patients with depression who showed either a successful or a poor response to antidepressant treatment. This analysis reveals a relevant relationship between cortical and limbic regions that is predictive of clinical response to therapy and that may be modulated by the task performed.
As well, there was a clear lateralization of the significant associations, toward the left hemisphere. One of the most replicated observations in the electroencephalographic (EEG) literature is the finding of frontal asymmetry, with greater alpha activity over the left than the right frontal regions in subclinical and clinical depression (47) . As alpha activity is inversely related to cortical activation, this is believed to reflect relatively greater right frontal and attenuated left frontal activity. Davidson (48) has conceptualized this in an approach/withdrawal motivational model in which right frontal activity is associated with avoidance or withdrawal behaviors and left frontal activity with general approach behaviors. Moreover, Bruder et al. (49) noted greater left hemispheric activation in patients who showed a clinical response to fluoxetine than in nonresponders. Although the neural regions mediating the EEG activity are not well defined, these observations broadly parallel our findings. Of note, the left lateralization of the treatment response relationship was elicited by happy facial stimuli, while the task with sad faces showed bilateral regional associations (6), which is consistent with the right visual field bias for positive emotions (50) and the engagement of approach behaviors (48) .

Study Limitations

All patients in the present study received fluoxetine as their treatment, which limits the interpretation of the neural responses as an effect of pharmacotherapy. In order to distinguish the treatment effects of medication from state changes associated with the natural course of the illness, comparison with a placebo-treated group of patients is necessary. We had initially considered including a group of patients who would receive placebo, but we decided to maintain full treatment options in the present study for ethical reasons. To account for possible test-retest effects, the comparison group consisted of matched healthy individuals who underwent the same scans at the same time points. The main effect of time revealed decreased responses in multiple cortical and limbic regions, likely reflecting effects of repeated scanning on affective neural processing networks, although possible systematic measurement errors cannot be fully excluded. In order to ascertain the specificity of the neural findings, all subjects also participated in a basic sensory stimulation task, which did not show any differences between groups or between the baseline and follow-up scans (6) .

Summary

Patients with depression had lower neural responses to happy facial expressions in limbic, subcortical, and extrastriate cortical regions as compared with healthy volunteers. Attenuated extrastriate activation at baseline showed an increase following antidepressant treatment. The affected regions encompass the core visual system and emotional processing stream involved in face perception. The findings reveal a neural substrate for impaired recognition of happy facial expressions in depression and demonstrate the ameliorative effects of antidepressant treatment. The observations in the present study complement our previous finding of greater neural responses to sad facial expressions among the same group of depressed subjects than among healthy subjects. These data offer further support for a network of regions implicated in depression and indicate the importance of task specificity in engaging components of the neural network.

Footnotes

Received Nov. 15, 2005; revisions received Jan. 23 and April 27, 2006; accepted June 19, 2006. From the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Brain Mapping Unit, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Sciences, Manchester University, Manchester, U.K.; and Clinical Pharmacology Discovery Medicine, Psychiatry Centre of Excellence for Drug Discovery, GlaxoSmithKline SpA, Verona, Italy. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Fu, Institute of Psychiatry, 103 Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, U.K.; [email protected] (e-mail).
Supported by an experimental medicine research grant from GlaxoSmithKline. Drs. Fu and Bullmore were supported by the Wellcome Trust. Software development was supported by Human Brain Project grant EB01518-01 from the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering and NIMH.
The authors thank Professor Mary Phillips for assistance with the experimental stimuli, the volunteers who participated in this study, and the staff of the MRI Unit, Maudsley Hospital, London, for their technical assistance.
CME Disclosure: Dr. Brammer is the Director of Neurosense P.L.C. Dr. Cleare is on an advisory board of Eli Lilly. Dr. Pich is an employee of GlaxoSmithKline. Dr. Bullmore is a part-time employee of GlaxoSmithKline. Drs. Fu, Williams, Suckling, and Kim, Mr. Walsh, Mr. Andrew, and Ms. Mitterschiffthaler report no competing interests.
APA policy requires disclosure by CME authors of unapproved or investigational use of products discussed in CME programs. Off-label use of medications by individual physicians is permitted and common. Decisions about off-label use can be guided by scientific literature and clinical experience.

References

1.
Snyder M, Cantor N: Thinking about ourselves and others: self-monitoring and social knowledge. J Pers Soc Psychol 1980; 43:163–175
2.
Mandal MK, Bhattacharya BB: Recognition of facial affect in depression. Percept Mot Skills 1985; 61:13–14
3.
Gur RC, Erwin RJ, Gur RE, Zwil AS, Heimberg C, Kraemer HC: Facial emotion discrimination, II: behavioral findings in depression. Psychiatry Res 1992; 42:241–251
4.
Surguladze SA, Young AW, Senior C, Brebion G, Travis MJ, Phillips ML: Recognition accuracy and response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression. Neuropsychology 2004; 18:212–218
5.
Persad SM, Polivy J: Differences between depressed and nondepressed individuals in the recognition of and response to facial emotional cues. J Abnorm Psychol 1993; 102:358–368
6.
Fu CHY, Williams SC, Cleare AJ, Brammer MJ, Walsh ND, Kim J, Andrew CM, Pich EM, Williams PM, Reed LJ, Mitterschiffthaler MT, Suckling J, Bullmore ET: Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:877–889
7.
Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B, Zuck E: Segregated processing of facial identity and emotion in the human brain: a PET study. Vis Cogn 1994; 1:349–369
8.
Bruce V, Young AW: Understanding face recognition. Br J Psychol 1986; 77:305–327
9.
Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI: The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn Sci 2000; 4:223–233
10.
Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, Silva JA, Mahurin RK, McGinnis S, Jerabek PA: Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: serial changes and relationship to clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:830–843
11.
Kennedy SH, Evans KR, Krüger S, Mayberg HS, Meyer JH, McCann S, Arifuzzman AI, Houle S, Vaccarino FJ: Changes in regional brain glucose metabolism measured with positron emission tomography after paroxetine treatment of major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:899–905
12.
Davidson RJ, Irwin W, Anderle MJ, Kalin NH: The neural substrates of affective processing in depressed patients treated with venlafaxine. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:64–75
13.
Surguladze SA, Brammer MJ, Young AW, Andrew C, Travis MJ, Williams SCR, Phillips ML: A preferential increase in the extrastriate response to signals of danger. Neuroimage 2003; 19:1317–1328
14.
Breiter HC, Etcoff NL, Whalen PJ, Kennedy WA, Rauch SL, Buckner RL, Strauss MM, Hyman SE, Rosen BR: Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 1996; 17:875–887
15.
Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberson I: Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 2002; 16:331–348
16.
Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AW, Calder AJ, Dolan RJ: A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 1996; 383:812–815
17.
Phillips ML, Bullmore ET, Howard R, Woodruff PW, Wright IC, Williams SC, Simmons A, Andrew C, Brammer M, David AS: Investigation of facial recognition memory and happy and sad facial expression perception: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Res 1998; 83:127–138
18.
Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson D, David A, Murphy D: Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum Brain Mapp 2000; 9:93–105
19.
Lawrence NS, Williams AM, Surguladze S, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, Andrew C, Frangou S, Ecker C, Phillips ML: Subcortical and ventral prefrontal cortical neural responses to facial expressions distinguish patients with bipolar disorder and major depression. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55:578–587
20.
Ekman P, Freisen WV: Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, Calif, Consulting Psychologists Press, 1976
21.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed (DSM-IV). Washington, DC, APA, 1994
22.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1995
23.
Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62
24.
Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Rabe-Hesketh S, Curtis VA, Morris RG, Williams SCR, Sharma T, McGuire PK: Methods for diagnosis and treatment of stimulus-correlated motion in generic brain activation studies using fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 1999; 7:38–48
25.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: A Coplanar Stereotactic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart, Germany, Thieme Verlag, 1988
26.
Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, Simmons A, Williams SCR, Grasby PM, Howard RJ, Woodruff PWR, Rabe-Hesketh S: Generic brain activation mapping in fMRI: a non-parametric approach. Magn Reson Imaging 1997; 15:763–770
27.
Bullmore ET, Suckling J, Overmeyer S, Rabe-Hesketh S, Taylor E, Brammer MJ: Global, voxel and cluster tests, by theory and permutation, for a difference between two groups of structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1999; 18:32–42
28.
Papez JW: A proposed mechanism of emotion: 1937. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995; 7:103–112
29.
Mayberg HS: Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997; 9:471–481
30.
Davidson RJ, Lewis DA, Alloy LB, Amaral DG, Bush G, Cohen JD, Drevets WC, Farah MJ, Kagan J, McClelland JL, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Peterson BS: Neural and behavioral substrates of mood and mood regulation. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52:478–502
31.
Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R: Neurobiology of emotion perception, II: implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54:515–528
32.
Iidaka T, Terashima S, Yamashita K, Okada T, Sadato N, Yonekura Y: Dissociable neural responses in the hippocampus to the retrieval of facial identity and emotion: an event-related fMRI study. Hippocampus 2003; 13:429–436
33.
Lane RD, Reiman EM, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ: Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness and disgust. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:926–933
34.
George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Horwitz B, Herscovitch P, Post RM: Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in healthy women. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:341–351
35.
Surguladze S, Brammer M, Keedwell P, Giampietro V, Young A, Travis M, Williams SCR, Phillips M: A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:201–209
36.
Robbins TW, Everitt BJ: Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1996; 6:228–236
37.
Walbott HG: Recognition of emotion from facial expression via imitation? some indirect evidence for an old theory. Br J Soc Psychol 1991; 30:207–219
38.
Cancelliere AE, Kertesz A: Lesion localization in acquired deficits of emotional expression and comprehension. Brain Cogn 1990; 13:133–147
39.
Ray JP, Price JL: The organization of projections from the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1993; 337:1–31
40.
Reiman EM, Lane RD, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ, Friston KJ, Yun LS, Chen K: Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:918–925
41.
Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ: Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 2001; 30:829–841
42.
Taylor SF, Liberzon I, Koeppe RA: The effect of graded aversive stimuli on limbic and visual activation. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38:1415–1425
43.
Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Fitzsimmons JR, Cuthbert BN, Scott JD, Moulder B, Nangia V: Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: an fMRI analysis. Psychophysiology 1998; 35:199–210
44.
Davis M, Whalen PJ: The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry 2001; 6:13–34
45.
Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, Lau M, Bieling P, Kennedy S, Mayberg HS: Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:34–41
46.
Seminowicz DA, Mayberg HS, McIntosh AR, Goldapple K, Kennedy S, Segal Z, Rafi-Tari S: Limbic-frontal circuitry in major depression: a path modeling metanalysis. Neuroimage 2004; 22:409–418
47.
Coan JA, Allen JJB: Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biol Psychol 2004; 67:7–49
48.
Davidson RJ: Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain Cogn 1992; 20:125–151
49.
Bruder GE, Stewart JW, Tenke CE, McGrath PJ, Leite P, Bhattacharya N, Quitkin FM: Electroencephalographic and perceptual asymmetry differences between responders and nonresponders to an SSRI antidepressant. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 49:416–425
50.
Adolphs R, Jansari A, Tranel D: Hemispheric perception of emotional valence from facial expressions. Neuropsychology 2001; 15:516–524

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 599 - 607
PubMed: 17403973

History

Published online: 1 April 2007
Published in print: April, 2007

Authors

Details

Cynthia H.Y. Fu, M.D., Ph.D., F.R.C.P.C.
Steve C.R. Williams, Ph.D.
Michael J. Brammer, Ph.D.
Anthony J. Cleare, M.R.C.Psych.
Nicholas D. Walsh, B.Sc.
Martina T. Mitterschiffthaler, M.Sc.
Emilio Merlo Pich, M.D.
Edward T. Bullmore, M.R.C.Psych., Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share