Skip to main content
Full access
Reviews and Overviews
Published Online: 1 October 2000

Genetic Epidemiology of Major Depression: Review and Meta-Analysis

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The authors conducted a meta-analysis of relevant data from primary studies of the genetic epidemiology of major depression.METHOD: The authors searched MEDLINE and the reference lists of previous review articles to identify relevant primary studies. On the basis of a review of family, adoption, and twin studies that met specific inclusion criteria, the authors derived quantitative summary statistics. RESULTS: Five family studies met the inclusion criteria. The odds ratios for proband (subjects with major depression or comparison subjects) versus first-degree relative status (affected or unaffected with major depression) were homogeneous across the five studies (Mantel-Haenszel odds ratio=2.84, 95% CI=2.31–3.49). No adoption study met the inclusion criteria, but the results of two of the three reports were consistent with genetic influences on liability to major depression. Five twin studies met the inclusion criteria, and their statistical summation suggested that familial aggregation was due to additive genetic effects (point estimate of heritability of liability=37%, 95% CI=31%–42%), with a minimal contribution of environmental effects common to siblings (point estimate=0%, 95% CI=0%–5%), and substantial individual-specific environmental effects/measurement error (point estimate=63%, 95% CI=58%–67%). The literature suggests that recurrence best predicts the familial aggregation of major depression. CONCLUSIONS: Major depression is a familial disorder, and its familiality mostly or entirely results from genetic influences. Environmental influences specific to an individual are also etiologically significant. Major depression is a complex disorder that does not result from either genetic or environmental influences alone but rather from both. These findings are notably consistent across samples and methods and are likely to be generally applicable.
Major depression, a common (14), costly (5), and recurrent disorder (6), is associated with considerable morbidity (710) and excess mortality (11). Major depression has been projected to become the second leading cause of disability worldwide by 2020 (second to ischemic heart disease) (12). Moreover, major depression is of increasing importance in clinical psychiatry (13).
There are many primary studies of the genetic epidemiology of major depression, and a number of excellent reviews are available (1417). However, to our knowledge, no single source has provided a quantitative summation of these data, which have been gathered through considerable effort by many teams of researchers worldwide over the past three decades. We believe that this area of research is sufficiently mature to warrant meta-analysis.
This synthetic meta-analysis addressed three questions. First, to what extent is major depression familial? Second, if major depression is familial, what are the relative contributions of genes and environment in the etiology of the disorder? Third, are there clinical features of major depression that predict familial aggregation?

Method

The key advantage of synthetic meta-analysis over traditional literature review is the potential to yield a less biased quantitative summary of the findings of many primary empirical studies (1821). In performing this meta-analysis, we attempted to apply principles similar to those customarily applied to considerably larger bodies of literature (e.g., randomized clinical trials for common medical illnesses such as myocardial infarction).
First, to identify all relevant primary studies, we performed computerized MEDLINE searches for an inclusive list of descriptors and searched the reference lists of prior reviews of major depression to identify any reports that were not retrieved in the MEDLINE search. Second, we created an a priori set of criteria for studies’ inclusion in the meta-analysis. To be included, a study had to possess features essential to interpretability, such as 1) explicit distinction between unipolar major depression and bipolar disorder, 2) systematic proband recruitment and ascertainment of relatives, 3) direct collection of diagnostic data from all or nearly all subjects (by using direct interview or, in one instance [22], by using questionnaires), 4) use of operationalized diagnostic criteria, and 5) diagnostic determination by assessors who were blind to ascertainment source and diagnoses of other relatives. For family studies, we also required a comparison group that was studied in a similar manner. For twin studies, we also required that zygosity was estimated with reasonable accuracy and blind to diagnoses.
More than half of the extant family and twin studies did not meet one or more of these criteria (most were unblinded and/or uncontrolled). If relative or co-twin diagnoses are assigned without “blindfolding,” it is impossible to know the degree to which the results are contaminated by experimental bias. For archival purposes, we provide the citations for the family (2330) and twin studies (3134) that were not included.
We considered each family study as a two-by-two contingency table of proband diagnosis (major depression or comparison) versus relative diagnosis (affected or unaffected with major depression), with the odds ratio as a summary statistic (35). Assuming a fixed-effects model, each of the two-by-two tables for the five family studies forms a stratum. To pool these data, we used the Mantel-Haenszel method after first assessing the homogeneity of the data with the Breslow-Day chi-square test (21, 36).
No adoption study met our inclusion criteria, so the three published reports were considered qualitatively. We include these reports for three reasons. First, adoption studies have a fundamentally different set of assumptions than do twin studies. Consistent results from adoption and twin studies increase the likelihood that the conclusions more accurately reflect an underlying reality. Second, although adoption studies are theoretically elegant, they contain a host of practical and ethical difficulties: adoption studies are rather rare, and there may be few additional adoption studies of major depression in the future. Third, the results of adoption studies of major depression are often described in the literature as ambiguous or inconsistent with family and twin studies; we believe that this interpretation is incorrect.
For the twin studies, we first used structural equation modeling to perform univariate analysis of the primary studies (37). Briefly, by comparing the similarity of monozygotic twins with the similarity of dizygotic twins, it is possible to partition the variance in liability (38) to major depression into three components: additive genetic influences (a2), environmental influences shared by members of a twin pair (c2), and environmental influences specific to one or the other member of a twin pair (e2). (The abbreviations a2, c2, and e2 refer to quantitative estimates of the three components.) We used the structural equation modeling package Mx (39, 40) for these analyses. These statistical models can be fit to the raw contingency table data, and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) can be calculated for the relevant parameters (41). Mx is available on the World Wide Web at http://views.vcu.edu/mx.
After computing the estimates for each study, we created a more complex model that included the data from all of the twin studies. This model necessitated a series of assumptions: that prevalences of major depression were similar across studies, that the prevalences among monozygotic and dizygotic twins were similar, that the model parameters could be equated across studies, and that the data were similar across ascertainment methods and gender. We tested these assumptions by using the likelihood ratio chi-square difference test. For these tests, we in effect compared two models. For example, one model might allow a set of parameters to vary freely across studies, generating a goodness-of-fit chi-square with a certain number of degrees of freedom. A submodel might constrain these parameters to be equal, generating a goodness-of-fit chi-square with fewer degrees of freedom. The difference between the chi-square values of these two models is also a chi-square test. If the chi-square difference is large, the model fit worsens when moving from the unconstrained model to the submodel, indicating that the submodel provides a poorer representation of the data. On the other hand, a small chi-square difference suggests that the submodel provides a reasonable representation of the data.
On the basis of prior data about the biology of twinning (i.e., that monozygotic twins are, for most purposes, genetically identical and that dizygotic twins share half of their genes identical by descent) and assuming that etiologically relevant environmental factors are similar across monozygotic and dizygotic twins, a set of expectations can be generated. Comparing the experimental data to these expectations allows assessment of the goodness-of-fit of the observed data to the expectations.

Results

Familial Aggregation

Family studies can be conceptualized as a type of case-control study, as described in detail elsewhere (42, 43). In these studies, probands have major depression and comparison subjects have no history of major depression and are usually matched with probands on potentially confounding variables such as age and gender, and the outcome of interest is the prevalence of major depression in biological relatives (usually first-degree relatives).
Table 1 summarizes data from the five family studies of major depression that met our inclusion criteria for the meta-analysis (4448). Only the study by Gershon et al. (44) did not match comparison subjects to probands by age and gender. All five studies showed evidence in support of the familial aggregation of major depression in probands, compared with comparison subjects. Across the five studies, there was strong evidence for an association between major depression in the proband and major depression in first-degree relatives (χ2=97.7, df=1, p<0.00005). In addition, the hypothesis that the odds ratios were homogeneous across the five studies could not be rejected (Breslow-Day χ2=5.22, df=4, p=0.27), and the summary Mantel-Haenszel odds ratio across the five studies was 2.84 (95% CI=2.31–3.49). Thus, in aggregate, these five studies provided strong and consistent evidence in support of the familiality of major depression.
These findings have two important potential limitations. First, all extant studies recruited most or all subjects with major depression from clinical sources. If having a family history of major depression increases the chance of clinical referral, then the overall odds ratio will be biased upward. One study did not find evidence for this potential bias (48), although two others did (49, 50). The overall impact of this potential bias is unclear, although it may not materially alter the conclusions (49).
Second, screening comparison subjects for the presence of psychopathology (other than major depression) generally leads to lower morbidity risks of major depression in first-degree relatives in comparison subjects and biases the overall odds ratio upward (5153). This effect is evident in Table 1, which shows that the two studies with unscreened comparison subjects had lower odds ratios (45, 46) than the three studies with comparison subjects who were screened for psychopathology (44, 47, 48). When the data were analyzed more formally, there were indications (of marginal statistical significance) that the odds ratios from studies with screened and unscreened comparison samples were heterogeneous (Breslow-Day χ2=3.75, df=1, p=0.053). However, it is critical to note that although the summary Mantel-Haenszel odds ratio from the studies with screened comparison samples was greater (odds ratio=3.62, 95% CI=2.65–4.97), the summary odds ratio from the unscreened studies (odds ratio=2.38, 95% CI=1.78–3.17) was still significantly greater than 1. Hence, even if screening comparison subjects for psychopathology results in bias toward greater familiality of major depression, the basic conclusion that major depression is familial appears to be correct.
Taken together, these methodologically rigorous studies demonstrate that major depression is a familial disorder (i.e., that it “runs” in families). Family studies cannot, however, distinguish genetic influences from environmental risk factors that are also familial. Accomplishing this aim requires other methodological approaches.

Genetic and Environmental Influences

Adoption and twin studies are the two principal approaches to delineating genetic and environmental effects in humans. Adoption studies are social quasi-experiments in which the offspring of one set of parents are reared from early in life by unrelated strangers. In contrast, twin studies are biological quasi-experiments that contrast genetically identical monozygotic twins with dizygotic twins who share many environment features while sharing half their genes on average. One analytic approach to adoption and twin data yields a partition of the etiological sources of variation into three types of causes. These are additive genetic effects, environmental effects common to both members of a twin pair, and environmental effects unique to an individual. The approach is described briefly (54) and in detail elsewhere (17, 37).

Adoption studies

The three adoption studies of major depression that we are aware of (5557) did not meet our inclusion criteria for the meta-analysis, for the following reasons.
The study by von Knorring et al. (55) did not find evidence for a genetic influence on major depression. However, the diagnoses of major depression in that study were based on indirect sources (i.e., sick leave registrations and evidence of clinical treatment) that may underestimate the true lifetime prevalence of major depression, as only a subset of those with major depression seek treatment (58, 59). The odds ratio from this matched design was 0.71 (95% CI=0.16–2.94) (35, pp. 209–211).
The study by Cadoret et al. (56) had markedly limited data on biological parents and limited statistical power. This study is widely regarded as not supporting the hypothesis of genetic influences on major depression because the results were nonsignificant when they were analyzed separately by gender. However, the trend-level relationship evident in both genders became significant when the data were combined (χ2=3.87, df=1, p=0.05; odds ratio=2.54, 95% CI=1.00–6.47, calculations ours).
The large study by Wender et al. (57) found evidence for a genetic effect on the transmission of major depression (χ2=4.73, df=1, p=0.03; odds ratio=7.24, 95% CI=1.21–43.20). However, the diagnoses of major depression in that study were based on indirect sources rather than personal interviews.
In summary, two of these three adoption studies provided qualitative evidence consistent with the existence of significant genetic factors in the etiology of major depression.

Twin studies

The data from the twin studies of major depression that met our inclusion criteria are summarized in Table 2. At the time of this writing, the twin data and the principal Mx script are available on the World Wide Web at http://views.vcu.edu/pub/mx/examples/mdreview. More than 21,000 individuals have been included in these studies, and, notably, all of the studies used the DSM-III-R criteria for major depression. Four studies included community samples (22, 6062), and two included clinical samples (22, 63). The original paper by Kendler and Prescott (61) has been amended (64), and the amended rather than the original data were analyzed for this report. Two published reports are not shown in Table 2. The limited data presented in one of those reports, by Andrews et al. (65), precluded critical evaluation of the claim that there were no important genetic effects for major depression. In addition, their sample partially overlapped with the larger sample studied by Bierut et al. (60). We present the most recent data from the longitudinal cohort first reported by Kendler et al. (3).
The results from the community and clinical studies summarized in Table 2 (also depicted in Figure 1) broadly support five conclusions. First, consistent with theoretical work on the statistical power of the classic twin study for discrete traits like major depression (66), the 95% CIs for the parameter estimates from these twin studies were fairly broad. One key purpose of a meta-analysis is to combine data across studies in an attempt to narrow the CIs on a parameter and thereby to increase precision. Second, the CIs for a2 (heritability in liability) did not include 0 for seven of 11 of these studies, suggesting that the aggregate value of a2 across the studies would be nonzero. Third, the 95% CIs for c2 (proportion of trait variance due to shared environmental factors) included 0 in all of the twin studies, indicating that environmental influences shared by members of a twin pair are unlikely to have substantial impact on the liability to major depression. Fourth, heritability point estimates from community and clinically ascertained samples were not markedly discrepant. Finally, there were no consistent gender differences across the twin studies.
Before summary estimates of these twin data could be generated, it was necessary to test a number of critical hypotheses and assumptions under which the data might be combined. First, it was necessary to allow the individual studies to have different major depression liability thresholds (i.e., prevalences) because equating the thresholds across studies led to an enormous worsening of the fit of the statistical model (χ2=739.5, df=10, p≈0). Possible explanations for these differences include cross-cultural differences in major depression prevalence (67), age cohort effects (2), and differences in diagnostic approaches. Second, unexpectedly, it was necessary to allow same-sex dizygotic twins to have higher liability thresholds for major depression than same-sex monozygotic twins because equating the thresholds led to a worsening of the fit of the statistical model (χ2=31.96, df=7, p=0.00004). Slight monozygotic-dizygotic differences have been observed for a number of traits; speculations about these findings generally focus on differential recruitment bias or a subtle protective effect of being a particular type of twin. Third, given the higher prevalence of major depression in women than in men, equating the thresholds for opposite-sex dizygotic pairs led to a considerable further worsening of fit (χ2=47.13, df=2, p<0.0001). Fourth, when we tested whether the a2, c2, and e2 parameters could be equated versus whether it was necessary for them to be free to vary across studies, we found that it was reasonable to equate the parameters (χ2=7.86, df=8, p=0.45). This was a critical step in the goal of aggregating data from the published twin studies of major depression. Fifth, we also found that it was reasonable to equate the a2, c2, and e2 parameters for studies from clinical and community sources (χ2=5.10, df=4, p=0.28). Sixth and finally, it was reasonable to equate the a2, c2, and e2 parameters across gender (χ2=3.23, df=2, p=0.20).
Although the principal reason for these comparisons was to establish the validity of our meta-analytic procedure, they also allow a substantiative conclusion that is important to our understanding of major depression. Despite the many differences across these twin studies in the prevalence of major depression, ascertainment, methods, ethnicity, and geographic location, the fundamental genetic architecture of major depression appears homogeneous across samples and across gender.
The next step was to generate the 95% CIs of the parameter estimates of the full model (bottom of Table 2 and Figure 1). These CIs suggest that the variance in liability to major depression is mostly due to individual-specific environmental effects (58%–67%) and additive genetic effects (31%–42%), with a negligible contribution by environmental effects shared by siblings (0%–5%). It is critical to note that the individual-specific environment includes the effects of measurement error and gene-environment interactions. As this meta-analysis had particular power to detect common environmental effects, it is notable that these effects accounted for a small proportion of variance.
In addition to estimating parameters under a full model, it is useful to investigate whether simpler models offer a more parsimonious explanation of the data (37). Fitting submodels reduces the number of parameters and leads to narrower CIs. As might be predicted from the summary results in Table 2, the best-fitting and most parsimonious submodel contained only additive genetic (a2=37%, 95% CI 33%–42%) and individual-specific environmental effects (e2=63%, 95% CI=58%–67%).
We investigated the heritability of major depression in community and clinical studies separately because of suggestions that clinically ascertained major depression might be “more genetic.” Estimates of heritability were similar in subjects ascertained from community (a2=37%, 95% CI=28%–42%) and clinical (a2=43%, 95% CI=21%–58%) sources.

Gender

There is strong evidence that the risk of depression is greater in women than in men (6769). These findings raise two questions. First, is the relative importance of genetic effects in the etiology of major depression the same in males and females? Four studies found no gender difference in heritability (22, 6163), although one study did (60). Unsurprisingly, our meta-analysis did not find substantial differences in heritability in liability for major depression between men and women. In addition, the adoption study by Cadoret et al. (56) reported similar odds ratios in men (2.5) and women (2.4). Thus, the weight of the evidence suggests similar genetic effects on liability to major depression in males and females.
Second, even if genetic influences are equivalent in males and females, to what extent do these pools of genes overlap? One study estimated the correlation between genetic effects for males and females (ra) to be 0.57 (61), and another study estimated the correlation to be 1.0 (22). Family data are consistent with partial overlap (7072). Although the data are limited, the most parsimonious explanation appears to be that men and women share most but not all genetic influences for major depression. Possible reasons for these differences are discussed elsewhere (61, p. 43).

Assumptions

There are several potential threats to the interpretation of these studies. First, the critical equal-environment assumption posits that monozygotic and dizygotic twins are equally correlated in their exposure to environmental events of etiologic relevance to major depression. If this were not true, the greater similarity between monozygotic versus dizygotic twins for major depression could result from environmental and not genetic factors. The equal-environment assumption has been examined repeatedly, and there is considerable evidence supporting its validity for major depression (73, 74). Second, conclusions about major depression from twins may not generalize to singletons if there are protective or risk factors specifically associated with twins. Moreover, twins are distinctive from singletons in a number of potentially important ways (75); however, the incidence of treated major depression is similar in singletons, monozygotic twins, and dizygotic twins (see reference 76 for review), as are the mean levels of current depressive symptoms (77).
Finally, some unknown or poorly measured bias may be responsible for the results from twin studies. Estimates of heritability in liability from an alternative design can provide an important cross-check of this possibility. We computed tetrachoric correlations from the results of the controlled family studies of major depression in Table 1 (38). If a set of fairly restrictive assumptions are met, these tetrachoric correlations multiplied by 2 estimate heritability in liability to major depression (78). These assumptions are distinct from those of twin studies’ design, and convergent results from different methodological approaches suggest that the assumptions of both methods are problematic or, more parsimoniously, that the results reflect the fundamental nature of major depression. From the family studies summarized in Table 1, the weighted mean heritability in liability to major depression was 58% (95% CI=42%–74%). Given that probands in all family studies were clinically ascertained, this result is in reasonable agreement with the heritability in liability from clinically ascertained twin studies (a2=43%, 95% CI=21%–58%).
Without recourse to a more formal experimental design, it is not possible to prove that the results from twin studies are uncontaminated by all conceivable biases and violations of critical assumptions. However, given the available data, there is no reason at present to question the validity of the conclusions about major depression from twin studies.

Clinical Indices of Familial Aggregation

Table 3 summarizes data from studies of the clinical indices that predict familial aggregation of major depression (25, 29, 62, 63, 7986). Given the biases possible with clinical ascertainment (87, 88), it is notable that only a few of these studies were based on community samples (62, 81, 82). Several of these reports are based on partially or wholly overlapping samples (8084, 86).
Given the diversity of these studies and of the analytic methods used, we are limited to qualitative comparisons. The most widely studied clinical feature is early-onset major depression, although there are two critical conceptual difficulties. First, comparison across studies is problematic, as early onset has been defined in many different ways (e.g., onset at age <30, <32, <36, <40, or <52.2 years). Second, only a few studies (81, 82, 86) controlled for confounding between age at onset and age cohort effects for major depression (89), without which the analyses are difficult to interpret. Among the community-based studies we reviewed, a study of one twin cohort found that age at onset of major depression did not predict familial aggregation (81, 82), but another twin study found that age at onset was related to increased heritability (62). Early onset of major depression predicted increased familial risk in four other studies (29, 79, 85, 86) (only one of these controlled for year of birth [86]), but not in two others (25, 63).
The overall picture for recurrent major depression is clearer: recurrence has predicted familial aggregation in every study in which it was investigated (25, 63, 8082, 86). In one study, however, recurrence was a significant predictor in univariate but not in multivariate analyses (86). It is critical to note, however, that groups defined by early onset of major depression and by recurrent major depression overlap considerably.
Other clinical features have been found to be associated with increased familiality of major depression. Impairment associated with major depression was a significant predictor of familial aggregation in the three studies in which it was examined (8082). The duration of the longest episode was associated with familiality of major depression in two studies (63, 82), but not in an overlapping cohort in a third report (81). Number of symptoms was associated with familiality in one study (62), but not in two others (81, 82). Finally, a number of specific depression symptoms and comorbid disorders have been investigated without the emergence of any clear pattern of results.
In conclusion, recurrent major depression appears to be the subtype of major depression that most consistently identifies increased familial risk. We are aware that early onset of major depression is prominently viewed as the clinical feature associated with familial aggregation (90), although this view is not entirely consistent with findings reported in the literature. Several other features warrant further investigation, including: 1) impairment associated with major depression, 2) atypical major depression with reversed vegetative features (91), 3) mood response to tryptophan depletion (92), and 4) several EEG sleep measures (93).

Discussion

The results of this synthetic meta-analysis of the literature on the genetic epidemiology of major depression support six conclusions.

Findings of the Meta-Analysis

Aggregation of major depression in families

Five large and carefully conducted family studies each demonstrated the familiality of major depression. Aggregating these data yielded a summary odds ratio of 2.84 (95% CI=2.31–3.49).

Genetic influences and familial aggregation

The conclusion that genetic influences are the most important contributor to familial aggregation is supported by two of the three adoption studies and all of the twin studies of major depression we reviewed. The heritability of major depression is likely to be in the range of 31%–42%. This is probably the lower bound, and the level of heritability is likely to be substantially higher for reliably diagnosed major depression or for subtypes such as recurrent major depression. In comparison, the heritabilities of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are estimated to be approximately 70% (94).

Etiological importance of environmental influences

Environmental influences that are etiologically important appear to be specific to an individual. There was little evidence that environmental influences common to family members (e.g., general parenting style, socioeconomic status, or local environmental qualities) were important. The large sample size of the twin meta-analysis should have afforded reasonable power to detect common environmental effects. If these effects are in fact nonzero, their magnitude is quite small (less than 5%).

Major depression as a complex disorder

Data from the studies we reviewed are consistent with the conceptualization of major depression as a complex disorder (95) that does not result from either genetic or environmental influences alone but rather from both.

Consistency

The consistency of a finding across samples and methodologies is a key element for determining causation (96, 97). In psychiatric research, consistency of research findings has often been elusive. The four conclusions discussed above are remarkably consistent across three quite different methodological approaches (five of five family studies, two of three adoption studies, and all twin studies). In addition, the estimates of heritability in liability from clinically ascertained family and twin studies are quite similar.

Generalizability

The first three conclusions above are likely to generalize to groups other than those studied. The studies included in the meta-analysis used different methodologies and had as subjects men and women from diverse cultures. Yet nearly all supported the same conclusions. This finding is particularly relevant for heritability of liability to major depression, which is a population-specific statistic. When nearly all twin studies suggest essentially the same heritability figure, it is considerably more likely to reflect the fundamental nature of major depression.

Individual-Specific Environment

Measurement error inflates estimates of individual-specific environmental effects (e2) at the expense of additive genetic (a2) and/or shared environmental influences (c2) (37). Measurement error was a concern for all the types of studies discussed here. The test-retest reliability (one type of measurement error) of a lifetime history of major depression has not been impressive in community samples (98, 99), although it has been somewhat higher in clinical samples (100). However, if two or more assessments are conducted over time, measurement error can be parsed out of the analysis (99, 101). In the longitudinal sample of female twins studied by Kendler et al. (3, 61, 99), the heritability of reliably diagnosed major depression was 66% (95% CI=53%–78%) (102). This finding strongly suggests that the true heritability of liability of major depression is substantially greater than the point estimate of 37% reported in Table 2.
Gene-environment interactions have been hypothesized to be of importance in the etiology of major depression. For example, a genetic vulnerability to major depression might be expressed only if an individual is exposed to stressful life events (103). Twin studies generally have poor power to resolve gene-environment interactions, and the presence of these interactions will lead to an increase in the estimate of individual-specific environmental effects. These more complex, but perhaps more likely, etiological pathways could lead to an overall underestimate of the importance of genetic effects in the etiology of major depression.

Clinical Implications

The results of this meta-analysis have implications for how clinicians conceptualize the etiology of major depression and how they might answer the question commonly encountered clinically, What causes major depression? The answer remains regrettably imprecise and has three components: 1) major depression results from both genetic and environmental factors, 2) major depression is heterogeneous, and 3) it is likely that a number of pathways lead to the common endpoint of major depression. Because of the astonishing complexity of major depression—and despite a broad array of research efforts—more precise explanations are not currently possible.
The second implication relates to how clinicians conceptualize the etiology of major depression in a particular patient, as exemplified by the question, Why do I have major depression? Overtly or covertly imbedded in this question is the complex question of who or what is to blame. Although the studies summarized here add weight to our understanding of the etiology of major depression at the level of large groups of individuals, the relevance of these findings to a particular patient is difficult to determine. Indeed, a key task in many types of clinical work—from pharmacological management to intensive psychotherapy—is to develop an individualized understanding of etiology. Our conclusions are relevant, but an important role for the clinician is to attempt to avoid a number of common misinterpretations (see reference 104 for review). The following points may help clinicians in their work with individual patients:
1. Heritability of liability applies to groups, not individuals. We cannot now state with confidence that a particular person’s major depression is or is not genetic in origin, much less that some percentage of the disorder is “due to genes.”
2. The causation of major depression is probabilistic, not deterministic. If we could measure liability to major depression directly, we would find substantial numbers of people with high liability who are not affected with major depression and people with low liability who are affected. Individuals’ life experiences, intentions, choices, and actions are critically important.
3. Environmental influences are important. Even if heritability of liability to major depression is predominant in the etiology of the disorder, environmental events account for substantial portions of the variance in liability. The absence of shared environmental effects does not mean that the influences of parents and other family members are irrelevant. The most parsimonious explanation may be one that is familiar to clinicians: it is not the general qualities of some facet of the environment (e.g., a parent’s rearing style, neighborhood, poverty) but rather how these qualities influence an individual and how he or she interacts with this part of the environment across developmental stages.

Research Implications

Although the conclusions summarized above constitute an important step in conceptualizing the nature of major depression, they do not immediately lead to the essential goal of a clear understanding of the pathophysiology of major depression. We submit, however, that these conclusions have the following implications for research on major depression:
1. These conclusions are consistent with efforts to identify susceptibility loci for major depression, as noted in an NIMH report on genetics and mental disorders (www. nimh.nih.gov/research/genetics.htm). We are aware of three major efforts to identify susceptibility loci that began in early 2000 and could yield results within 4–5 years.
2. If certain depressive features (i.e., recurrence) predict familiality of major depression, it may be useful to investigate the rationality of family-based screening as a primary and secondary preventive measure. Moreover, for clinical and biological studies, it may be possible to stratify heterogeneous samples to identify more homogeneous subsamples. Recurrence and family history of major depression are two variables that could be used to stratify samples. However, the most efficient means of obtaining these data (i.e., using the major depression proband as informant) may not be sufficiently sensitive (105108) to be applied with confidence.
3. The finding that the prevalence of major depression was higher in dizygotic than monozygotic twins was unexpected but intriguing. Possible explanations include differential cooperation in monozygotic versus dizygotic twins in the presence of major depression, a protective effect of being a monozygotic twin, and/or a predisposing effect of being a dizygotic twin.

Limitations

This review has three general limitations. First, there may be published or unpublished family, adoption, or twin studies that were not included in this analysis. We attempted to limit the impact of this problem as described in the Method section. Second, the extant studies focused on predominantly Caucasian subjects from developed nations. It is not known whether the results generalize worldwide or across time cohorts. Third, although it is reassuring that the extant data support the validity of the equal-environment assumption in twin studies of major depression, it is impossible to prove that all potential violations have been assessed. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that a cryptic violation of the equal-environment assumption could influence the results.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
Figure 1. Estimates of the Heritability in Liability to Major Depression in Studies of Male and Female Twinsa
aVertical bars indicate point estimates; horizontal bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
bAggregate values across studies of heritability in liability to major depression.
cMost parsimonious submodel, consisting of aggregate value across studies of a2.

Footnote

Received Sept. 3, 1999; revision received Mar. 23, 2000; accepted May 10, 2000. From the Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University. Address reprint requests to Dr. Sullivan, Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, P.O. Box 980126, Richmond, VA 23298-0126; [email protected] (e-mail).This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grants MH-59160, A138429, MH-01458, MH-40828, MH/AA-49492, and MH-01277. The authors thank Laura Bierut, M.D., and Andrew Health, Ph.D., for providing the raw data on major depression from the Australian Twin Registry.

References

1.
Wells JE, Bushnell JA, Hornblow AR, Joyce PR, Oakley-Browne MA: Christchurch psychiatric epidemiology study, part I: methodology and lifetime prevalence for specific psychiatric disorders. Aust NZ J Psychiatry 1989; 23:315–326
2.
Weissman MM, Bruce ML, Leaf PJ, Florio LP, Holzer C III: Affective disorders, in Psychiatric Disorders in America: The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study. Edited by Robins LN, Regier DA. New York, Free Press, 1991, pp 53–80
3.
Kendler K, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: A population-based twin study of major depression in women: the impact of varying definitions of illness. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:257–266
4.
Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, Wittchen H-U, Kendler KS: Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:8–19
5.
Greenberg PE, Stiglin LE, Finkelstein SN, Berndt ER: The economic burden of depression in 1990. J Clin Psychiatry 1993; 54:405–418
6.
Piccinelli M, Wilkinson G: Outcome of depression in psychiatric settings. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 164:297–304
7.
Wells KB, Stewart A, Hays RD, Burnam MA, Rogers W, Daniels M, Berry S, Greenfield S, Ware J: The functioning and well-being of depressed patients: results from the Medical Outcomes Study. JAMA 1989; 262:914–919
8.
Broadhead WE, Blazer DG, George LK, Tse CK: Depression, disability days, and days lost from work in a prospective epidemiologic survey. JAMA 1990; 264:2524–2528
9.
Judd LL, Paulus MP, Wells KB, Rapaport MH: Socioeconomic burden of subsyndromal depressive symptoms and major depression in a sample of the general population. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:1411–1417
10.
Ustun TB, Rehm J, Chatterji S, Saxena S, Trotter R, Room R, Bickenbach J (WHO/NIH Joint Project CAR Study Group): Multiple-informant ranking of the disabling effects of different health conditions in 14 countries. Lancet 1999; 354:111–115
11.
Harris EC, Barraclough BB: Excess mortality of mental disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1998; 173:11–53
12.
Murray CJL, Lopez AD: Evidence-based health policy: lessons from the Global Burden of Disease Study. Science 1996; 274:740–743
13.
Pincus HA, Tanielian TL, Marcus SC, Olfson M, Zarin DA, Thompson J, Magno Zito J: Prescribing trends in psychotropic medications: primary care, psychiatry, and other medical specialties. JAMA 1998; 279:526–531
14.
McGuffin P, Katz R: The genetics of depression and manic-depressive disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1989; 155:294–304
15.
Tsuang MT, Faraone SV: The Genetics of Mood Disorders. Baltimore, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1990
16.
Merikangas KR, Kupfer DJ: Mood disorders: genetic aspects, in Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, 6th ed, vol 1. Edited by Kaplan HI, Sadock BJ. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins, 1995, pp 1102–1116
17.
Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, Rutter M: Behavioral Genetics. New York, WH Freeman, 1997
18.
Glass GV, McGaw B, Smith ML: Meta-Analysis in Social Research. Beverly Hills, Calif, Sage Publications, 1981
19.
Smith ML, Glass GV, Miller TI: The Benefits of Psychotherapy. Baltimore, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1980
20.
Thacker SB: Meta-analysis: a quantitative approach to research integration. JAMA 1988; 259:1685–1689
21.
Petitti DB: Meta-Analysis, Decision Analysis, and Cost-Effectiveness Analysis. New York, Oxford University Press, 1994
22.
Kendler KS, Pedersen NL, Neale MC, Mathé A: A pilot Swedish twin study of affective illness including hospital- and population-ascertained subsamples: results of model fitting. Behav Genet 1995; 25:217–232
23.
Angst J, Frey R, Lohmeyer B, Zerbin-Rüdin E: Bipolar manic-depressive psychosis: results of a genetic investigation. Hum Genet 1980; 55:237–254
24.
Gershon ES, Mark A, Cohen N, Belizon N, Baron M, Knobe KE: Transmitted factors in the morbid risk of affective disorders: a controlled study. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:283–299
25.
McGuffin P, Katz R, Bebbington P: Hazard, heredity, and depression: a family study. J Psychiatr Res 1987; 4:365–375
26.
Perris C: A study of bipolar (manic-depressive) and unipolar recurrent depressive psychoses. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1966; 194:1–188
27.
Scharfetter C: Subdividing the functional psychoses: a family hereditary approach. Psychol Med 1981; 11:637–640
28.
Smeraldi E, Negri F, Melica AM: A genetic study of affective disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1977; 56:382–398
29.
Stancer HC, Persad E, Wagener DK, Jorna T: Evidence for homogeneity of major depression and bipolar affective disorder. J Psychiatr Res 1987; 21:37–53
30.
Winokur G, Cadoret R, Dorzab J, Baker M: Depressive disease: a genetic study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1971; 24:135–144
31.
Allen MG, Cohen S, Pollin W, Greenspan SI: Affective illness in veteran twins: a diagnostic review. Am J Psychiatry 1974; 131:1234–1239
32.
Bertelsen A, Harvald B, Hauge M: A Danish twin study of manic-depressive disorders. Br J Psychiatry 1977; 130:330–351
33.
Slater E, Shields J: Psychotic and Neurotic Illnesses in Twins. London, Her Majesty’s Stationery Office, 1953
34.
Torgersen S: Genetic factors in moderately severe and mild affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:222–226
35.
Schlesselman JJ: Case-Control Studies: Design, Conduct, Analysis. New York, Oxford University Press, 1982
36.
SAS/STAT Software: Changes and Enhancements Through Release 6.11. Cary, NC, SAS Institute, 1996
37.
Neale MC, Cardon LR: Methodology for the Study of Twins and Families. Dordrecht, the Netherlands, Kluwer, 1992
38.
Falconer DS: The inheritance of liability to certain diseases, estimated from the incidence among relatives. Ann Hum Genet (London) 1965; 29:51–76
39.
Neale MC, Boker S, Xie G, Maes H: Mx: Statistical Modeling. Richmond, Medical College of Virginia, Department of Psychiatry, 1999
40.
Hamagami F: A review of the Mx computer program for structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling 1997; 4:157–175
41.
Neale MC, Miller MB: The use of likelihood-based confidence intervals in genetic models. Behav Genet 1997; 27:113–120
42.
Weissman MM, Merikangas KR, John K, Wickramaratne P, Prusoff BA, Kidd KK: Family-genetic studies of psychiatric disorders: developing technologies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:1104–1116
43.
Khoury MJ, Beaty TH, Cohen BH: Fundamentals of Genetic Epidemiology. New York, Oxford University Press, 1993
44.
Gershon ES, Hamovit J, Guroff JJ, Dibble E, Leckman JF, Sceery W, Targum SD, Nurnberger JI Jr, Goldin LR, Bunney WE Jr: A family study of schizoaffective, bipolar I, bipolar II, unipolar, and normal control probands. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:1157–1167
45.
Maier W, Lichtermann D, Minges J, Hallmayer J, Heun R, Benkert O, Levinson DF: Continuity and discontinuity of affective disorders and schizophrenia: results of a controlled family study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:871–883
46.
Tsuang MT, Winokur G, Crowe RR: Morbidity risks of schizophrenia and affective disorders among first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia, mania, depression, and surgical conditions. Br J Psychiatry 1980; 137:497–504
47.
Weissman MM, Gershon ES, Kidd KK, Prusoff BA, Leckman JF, Dibble E, Hamovit J, Thompson WD, Pauls DL, Guroff JJ: Psychiatric disorders in the relatives of probands with affective disorders: the Yale University-NIMH Collaborative Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:13–21
48.
Weissman MM, Wickramaratne P, Adams PB, Lish JD, Horwath E, Charney D, Woods SW, Leeman E, Frosch E: The relationship between panic disorder and major depression: a new family study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:767–780
49.
Kendler KS: Is seeking treatment for depression predicted by a history of depression in relatives? implications for family studies of affective disorder. Psychol Med 1995; 25:807–814
50.
Sullivan PF, Wells JE, Joyce PR, Bushnell JA, Mulder RT, Oakley-Browne MA: Family history of depression in clinic and community samples. J Affect Disord 1996; 40:159–168
51.
Tsuang MT, Fleming JA, Kendler KS, Gruenberg AS: Selection of controls for family studies: biases and implications. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:1006–1008
52.
Schwartz S, Link BG: The “well control” artefact in case/control studies of specific psychiatric disorders. Psychol Med 1989; 19:737–742
53.
Kendler KS: The super-normal control group in psychiatric genetics: possible artifactual evidence for coaggregation. Psychiatr Genet 1990; 1:45–53
54.
Kendler KS: Twin studies of psychiatric illness: current status and future directions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:905–915
55.
von Knorring A-L, Cloninger CR, Bohman M, Sigvardsson S: An adoption study of depressive disorders and substance abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:943–950
56.
Cadoret RJ, O’Gorman TW, Heywood E, Troughton E: Genetic and environmental factors in major depression. J Affect Disord 1985; 9:155–164
57.
Wender PH, Kety SS, Rosenthal D, Schulsinger F, Ortmann J, Lunde I: Psychiatric disorders in the biological and adoptive families of adopted individuals with affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:923–929
58.
Regier DA, Goldberg ID, Taube CA: The de facto US mental health services system: a public health perspective. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35:685–693
59.
Regier DA, Narrow WE, Rae DS, Manderscheid RW, Locke BZ, Goodwin FK: The de facto US mental and addictive disorders service system: Epidemiologic Catchment Area prospective 1-year prevalence rates of disorders and services. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:85–94
60.
Bierut LJ, Heath AC, Bucholz KK, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PAF, Statham DJ, Dunne MP, Martin NG: Major depressive disorder in a community-based twin sample: are there different genetic and environmental contributions for men and women? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:557–563
61.
Kendler KS, Prescott CA: A population-based twin study of lifetime major depression in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:39–44; correction, 2000; 57:94–95
62.
Lyons MJ, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, True W, Lin N, Meyer JM, Toomey R, Faraone SV, Merren J, Tsuang MT: A registry-based twin study of depression in males. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:468–472
63.
McGuffin P, Katz R, Watkins S, Rutherford J: A hospital-based twin register of the heritability of DSM-IV unipolar depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:129–136
64.
Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Prescott CA: Corrections to two prior published articles (letter). Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:94–95
65.
Andrews G, Stewart G, Allen R, Henderson AS: The genetics of six neurotic disorders: a twin study. J Affect Disord 1990; 19:23–29
66.
Neale MC, Eaves LJ, Kendler KS: The power of the classical twin study to resolve variation in threshold traits. Behav Genet 1994; 24:239–258
67.
Weissman MM, Bland R, Joyce PR, Newman S, Wells JE, Wittchen H-U: Sex differences in rates of depression: cross-national perspectives. J Affect Disord 1993; 29:77–84
68.
Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB: Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey, I: lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. J Affect Disord 1993; 29:85–96
69.
Gater R, Tansella M, Korten A, Tiemens BG, Mavreas VG, Olatawura MO: Sex differences in the prevalence and detection of depressive and anxiety disorders in general health care settings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:405–413
70.
Faraone SV, Lyons MJ, Tsuang MT: Sex differences in affective disorder: genetic transmission. Genet Epidemiol 1987; 4:331–343
71.
Merikangas KR, Weissman MM, Pauls DL: Genetic factors in the sex ratio of major depression. Psychol Med 1985; 15:63–69
72.
Reich T, Van Eerdewegh P, Rice J, Mullaney J, Endicott J, Klerman GL: The familial transmission of primary major depressive disorder. J Psychiatr Res 1987; 21:613–624
73.
Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: Parental treatment and the equal environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness. Psychol Med 1994; 24:579–590
74.
Kendler KS, Gardener CO: Twin studies of adult psychiatric and substance dependence disorders: are they biased by differences in the environmental experiences of mono- and dizygotic twins in childhood and adolescence? Psychol Med 1998; 28:625–633
75.
Bryan E: Twins and Higher Multiple Births: A Guide to Their Nature and Nurture. London, Edward Arnold, 1992
76.
Kendler KS, Pedersen NL, Farahmand BY, Persson P-G: The treated incidence of psychotic and affective illness in twins compared to population expectation. Psychol Med 1996; 26:1135–1144
77.
Kendler KS, Martin NG, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: Self-report psychiatric symptoms in twins and their non-twin relatives: are twins different? Am J Med Genet Neuropsychiatr Genet 1995; 60:588–591
78.
Reich T, Cloninger CR, Guze SB: The multifactorial model of disease transmission, I: description of the model and its use in psychiatry. Br J Psychiatry 1975; 127:1–10
79.
Cadoret RJ, Woolson R, Winokur G: The relationship of age of onset in unipolar affective illness to risk of alcoholism and depression in parents. J Psychiatr Res 1977; 13:137–142
80.
Gershon ES, Weissman MM, Guroff JJ, Prusoff BA, Leckman JF: Validation of criteria for major depression through controlled family study. J Affect Disord 1986; 11:125–131
81.
Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: The clinical characteristics of major depression as indices of the familial risk to illness. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165:66–72
82.
Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Prescott CA: Clinical characteristics of major depression that predict risk of depression in relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:322–327; correction, 2000; 57:94–95
83.
Leckman JF, Weissman MM, Prusoff BA, Caruso KA, Merikangas KR, Pauls DL, Kidd KK: Subtypes of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:833–838
84.
Leckman JF, Caruso KA, Prusoff BA, Weissman MM, Merikangas KR, Pauls DL: Appetite disturbance and excessive guilt in major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:839–844
85.
Mendlewicz J, Baron M: Morbidity risks in subtypes of unipolar depressive illness: differences between early and late onset forms. Br J Psychiatry 1981; 139:463–466
86.
Weissman MM, Merikangas KR, Wickramaratne P, Kidd KK, Prusoff BA, Leckman JF, Pauls DL: Understanding the clinical heterogeneity of major depression using family data. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:430–434
87.
Berkson J: Limitations of the application of fourfold table analysis to hospital data. Biometrics Bull 1946; 2:47–53
88.
Cohen P, Cohen J: The clinician’s illusion. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:1178–1182
89.
Klerman GL, Weissman MM: Increasing rates of depression. JAMA 1989; 261:2229–2235
90.
NIMH Genetics Workgroup: Genetics and Mental Disorders. Bethesda, Md, National Institute of Mental Health, 1998
91.
Kendler KS, Eaves LJ, Walters EE, Neale MC, Heath AC, Kessler RC: The identification and validation of distinct depressive syndromes in a population-based sample of female twins. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:391–399
92.
Benkelfat C, Ellenbogen MA, Dean P, Palmour RM, Young SN: Mood-lowering effect of tryptophan depletion. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:687–697
93.
Giles DE, Kupfer DJ, Rush AJ, Roffwarg HP: Controlled comparison of electrophysiological sleep in families of probands with unipolar depression. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:192–199
94.
Kendler KS: Overview: a current perspective on twin studies of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:1413–1425
95.
Lander ES, Schork NJ: Genetic dissection of complex traits. Science 1994; 265:2037–2048
96.
Hill AB: The environment and disease: association or causation. Proc R Soc Med 1965; 58:295–300
97.
Rothman KJ: Modern Epidemiology. Boston, Little, Brown, 1986
98.
Bromet EJ, Dunn LO, Connell MM, Dew MA, Schulberg HC: Long-term reliability of diagnosing lifetime major depression in a community sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:435–440
99.
Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: The lifetime history of major depression in women: reliability of diagnosis and heritability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:863–870
100.
Keller MB, Klein DN, Hirschfeld RMA, Kocsis JH, McCullough JP, Miller I, First MB, Holzer CP III, Keitner GI, Marin DB, Shea T: Results of the DSM-IV mood disorders field trial. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:843–849
101.
Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Kendler KS: Heritability and reliability of binge-eating and bulimia nervosa. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:1210–1218
102.
Foley DL, Neale MC, Kendler KS: Reliability of a lifetime history of major depression: implications for heritability and co-morbidity. Psychol Med 1998; 28:857–870
103.
Kendler KS, Kessler RC, Walters EE, MacLean CJ, Neale MC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: Stressful life events, genetic liability, and onset of an episode of major depression in women. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:833–842
104.
Rutter M, Plomin R: Opportunities for psychiatry from genetic findings. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 171:209–219
105.
Andreasen NC, Rice J, Endicott J, Reich T, Coryell W: The family history approach to diagnosis: how useful is it? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:421–429
106.
Chapman TF, Mannuzza S, Klein DF, Fyer AJ: Effects of informant mental disorder on psychiatric family history data. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:574–579
107.
Orvaschel H, Thompson WD, Belanger A, Prusoff BA, Kidd KK: Comparison of the family history method to direct interview: factors affecting the diagnosis of depression. J Affect Disord 1982; 4:49–59
108.
Thompson WD, Orvaschel H, Prusoff BA, Kidd KK: An evaluation of the family history for ascertaining psychiatric disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:53–58

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1552 - 1562
PubMed: 11007705

History

Published online: 1 October 2000
Published in print: October 2000

Authors

Details

Patrick F. Sullivan, M.D., F.R.A.N.Z.C.P.
Michael C. Neale, Ph.D.
Kenneth S. Kendler, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share