Skip to main content
Full access
Reviews and Overviews
Published Online: 1 August 2007

Stress, Dysregulation of Drug Reward Pathways, and the Transition to Drug Dependence

Abstract

This review provides a neuroadaptive perspective regarding the role of the hormonal and brain stress systems in drug addiction with a focus on the changes that occur during the transition from limited access to drugs to long-term compulsive use of drugs. A dramatic escalation in drug intake with extended access to drug self-administration is characterized by a dysregulation of brain reward pathways. Hormonal studies using an experimenter-administered cocaine binge model and an escalation self-administration model have revealed large increases in ACTH and corticosterone in rats during an acute binge with attenuation during the chronic binge stage and a reactivation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis during acute withdrawal. The activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis with cocaine appears to depend on feed-forward activation of the mesolimbic dopamine system. At the same time, escalation in drug intake with either extended access or dependence-induction produces an activation of the brain stress system’s corticotropin-releasing factor outside of the hypothalamus in the extended amygdala, which is particularly evident during acute withdrawal. A model of the role of different levels of hormonal/brain stress activation in addiction is presented that has heuristic value for understanding individual vulnerability to drug dependence and novel treatments for the disorder.
Drug addiction has been conceptualized as a chronic relapsing disorder characterized by compulsive drug-taking behavior with impairment in social and occupational functioning. From a psychiatric perspective, drug addiction has aspects of both impulse control disorders and compulsive disorders (1) . Impulse control disorders are characterized by an increasing sense of tension or arousal before the commission of an impulsive act; pleasure, gratification, or relief at the time of commission of the act; and following the act, there may or may not be regret, self-reproach, or guilt (2) . In contrast, compulsive disorders are characterized by anxiety and stress before the commission of a compulsive repetitive behavior and relief from the stress by performing the compulsive behavior. As an individual moves from an impulsive disorder to a compulsive disorder, there is a shift from positive reinforcement driving the motivated behavior to negative reinforcement driving the motivated behavior. Drug addiction has been conceptualized as a disorder that progresses from impulsivity to compulsivity in a collapsed cycle of addiction composed of three stages: preoccupation/anticipation, binge/intoxication, and withdrawal/negative affect (3) . Different theoretical perspectives ranging from experimental psychology, social psychology, and neurobiology can be superimposed on these three stages, which are conceptualized as feeding into each other, becoming more intense, and ultimately leading to the pathological state known as addiction (3) . The thesis of the present review is that excessive drug taking in dependent animals can be studied in animal models, involves important perturbations in the stress response systems of the body, and contributes to both the positive reinforcement associated with impulsivity (binge stage of the addiction cycle) and the negative reinforcement of the withdrawal/negative affect stage of the addiction cycle.
For several years, the desirability of developing rodent self-administration models that more closely mimic the human patterns of self-administration of specific drugs of abuse has been a focus of research by both the Koob and Kreek groups. The most frequently used models are appropriate for assessing the impact of very modest exposure and first exposure to a drug of abuse as well as continued exposure to very modest amounts of a drug of abuse on a limited basis but are not designed to study addiction-like patterns of self-administration. From a clinical standpoint, it has long been recognized and well established that both opiate addicts and cocaine addicts have very different patterns of self-administration. For opiate abusers (primarily heroin abusers), intermittent opiate use is the initial pattern of intake and may continue for unpredictable lengths of time, ranging from 1 to 2 weeks up to several years or even a lifetime (for instance “weekend chippers”). On the other hand, it has been quite well documented from the very earliest work on developing an agonist treatment modality at Rockefeller University that heroin addicts (and other short-acting opiate addicts) self-administer their drug of abuse daily and at multiple times during the day at evenly spaced intervals (4) . These intervals are well-planned, either to prevent the onset and development of withdrawal symptoms or to maximize the limited euphorigenic effects that may be forthcoming from any single dose of a short-acting opiate, such as heroin, especially as tolerance develops. However, the heroin addict at the end of the day does eventually go to sleep for overnight rest. After awakening in the morning or midday, signs and symptoms of withdrawal have appeared, and thus acquisition of the “morning dose” of heroin immediately occurs. For cocaine addicts, the most common mode of self-administration after initial use is a binge pattern, in which from three to a dozen or more self-administrations of cocaine will occur at 30-minute to 2-hour intervals in a volley, or binge, with no cocaine self-administered for 1 day or even 1 week after a long string of binge self-administrations.
In the present review, we will build on earlier work (1, 527) to explore the role of the brain and hormonal stress systems in addiction. To accomplish this goal, we will primarily explore extension of previously used self-administration models to include animal models of the transition to addiction, such as 1) extended access to drug self-administration, 2) long-term exposure before self-administration, 3) and the use of very high doses per unit self-administered (compared to more conventional moderate and low doses).

New Findings Have Not Negated Our Earlier Hypotheses but Have Reinforced Them

In addition to the major sources of reinforcement in drug dependence, both the persistence of ongoing addiction and relapse to drug addiction days, months, or years after the last use of the drug may be due, in part, not only to conditioned positive and negative reinforcement but also to the negative reinforcement of protracted abstinence when it exists (as, for instance, has been well documented in the case of opiate addiction) and also to much more subtle factors that result from long-term changes or abnormalities in the brain after long-term exposure to a drug of abuse due to intrinsic neuroplasticity of the brain (4, 28, 29) . These changes may contribute to a general, ill-defined feeling of dysphoria, anxiety, or abnormality and also could be considered a form of protracted abstinence (3) . In addition, genetic factors and early environmental factors may contribute to variations or abnormalities in neurobiologic function that may render some individuals more vulnerable, both to acquisition of drug addiction and relapse to drug use after achieving the abstinent state (5) .
What also is new since 1998 is substantial evidence for our subhypothesis that corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), through its actions in activating the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and brain stress systems in the extended amygdala, is a key element contributing to the emotional dysregulation of drug dependence.

Animal Models of Excessive Drug Taking in Dependent Animals

Most animal self-administration models to date have sessions for only 1 or 2 hour per day with no access for longer periods of time, such as 6 to 24 hours, which would more closely mimic the human condition. Also, the doses per injection allowed in animals are usually low to extremely low, with the presumed scientific purpose of minimally altering neurobiological systems to elucidate threshold effects with the practical reason of preventing accidental animal overdose. Neither of these constraints pertain to humans; heroin addicts administer maximal doses monetarily affordable and within the limits of physiologic tolerance to prevent accidental opiate overdose (although this sometimes does occur on the street with surges in purity of heroin). Cocaine addicts similarly self-administer cocaine to the extent of funds available at the time within the limits of tolerable side effects, primarily jitteriness, nervousness, dysphoria, and depression.
The Koob and Kreek groups have created new models that more closely mimic the human condition. In the Koob group, extended-access models have been developed for long-term self-exposure, extinction, reexposure, and relapse and have included very long-term studies for each of several drugs of abuse (3033) . In the Kreek group, even longer sessions of extended access have been used for short-term through long-term exposure, and some studies have involved acquisition, extinction, and rechallenge (3438) . High and moderate doses of cocaine and morphine have been used in addition to the more usual low and very low doses per injection. Extended access to drugs of abuse produces dramatic increases in drug intake over time that mirror the human condition and that at a neurobiological level more clearly mimic the investigator-administered binge pattern.
To explore the possibility that differential access to intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats may produce different patterns of drug intake (the Koob group), rats were allowed access to intravenous self-administration of cocaine for 1 hour and 6 hours per day (30, 3436, 38, 39) ( Table 1 ). With 1 hour of access (short access) to cocaine per session through intravenous self-administration, drug intake remained low and stable, not changing from day to day as observed previously. In contrast, with 6-hour access (long access) to cocaine, drug intake gradually escalated over days (30) ( Figure 1 ). In the escalation group, there was increased early intake, sustained intake over the session, and an upward shift in the dose-effect function, suggesting an increase in the hedonic set point.
Figure 1. Effect of Drug Availability on Cocaine Intake a
a Reprinted with permission of AAAS/Science from Ahmed SH, Koob GF: Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: change in hedonic set point. Science 1998; 282:298–300, Figures 2A and 2B, p. 299.
b In long-access rats (N=12) but not in short-access rats (N=12), mean total cocaine intake started to increase significantly from session 5 (p<0.05; sessions 5–22 compared to session 1) and continued to increase thereafter (p<0.05; session 5 compared to sessions 8–10, 12, 13, and 17–22).
c During the first hour, long-access rats self-administered more infusions than short-access rats during sessions 5–8, 11, 12, 14, 15, and 17–22 (p<0.05).
In a similar 10-hour extended-access model (the Kreek group), intravenous cocaine was self-administered by randomly assigned rats allowed to self-administer 0.25, 0.50, 1.00, or 2.00 mg/kg per infusion intravenously in a continuous schedule of cocaine reinforcement during five consecutive daily 10-hour sessions (34) ( Table 1 ). When data from the animals self-administering any dose of cocaine were collapsed as a single group, the mean amount of self-administered cocaine exceeded 60 mg/kg per day, significantly greater than in our investigator-administered binge administration with 3×15 mg/kg cocaine per day (for a total of 45 mg/kg per day). In addition, when data from animals were analyzed by their randomly assigned group, the total daily dose administered by animals allowed to self-administer the highest and intermediate doses of cocaine (2.00 and 1.00 mg/kg, respectively) had a much steeper incremental daily total amount of cocaine self-administered than did low and very low dose groups. Animals allowed access to the highest (2.00 mg/kg per injection) dose were administering over 100 mg/kg by the end of the 5-day period. The slope of this acquisition was much steeper in the moderate and high-dose groups than in the animals allowed to self-administer very low (0.25 mg/kg) or low (0.50 mg/kg) doses of cocaine (34, 35) ( Figure 2 ).
Figure 2. Escalation of Cocaine Intake as a Function of Dose a
a Rats had access to cocaine self-administration (0.25, 0.50, 1.00, and 2.00 mg/kg per infusion) for five consecutive daily 10-hour sessions. As the available dose of cocaine increased, total cocaine intake increased (and the number of self-administered infusions decreased). Significant time-related escalations in both cocaine-reinforced responding and cocaine intake compared to self-administration day 1 (p<0.05) were observed at all cocaine doses except the lowest dose (0.25 mg/kg). Reprinted with permission of Springer from Mantsch JR, Ho A, Schlussman SD, Kreek MJ: Predictable individual differences in the initiation of cocaine self-administration by rats under extended access conditions are dose-dependent. Psychopharmacology 2001; 157:31–39, Figure 1A, p. 34.
In further studies of the escalation in drug intake with extended access, rats were randomly assigned to short-access and long-access groups (36) ( Table 1 ). The short-access animals were tested daily for multidose self-administration for 3 hours. The long-access animals were tested initially with multidose self-administration over 3 hours. Over the next 7 hours, the animals were allowed to self-administer a relatively high dose of cocaine (2.0 mg/kg). After 14 days, lever pressing was extinguished in 10 consecutive 3.5-hour extinction sessions. Following extinction, the ability of a single noncontingent investigator-administered infusion of cocaine at 0, 0.50, or 2.00 mg/kg to reinstate extinguished lever pressing was studied. Self-administration was not altered over time in the short-access rats. However, a general escalation of cocaine intake was found in the long-access, high-dose rats, which showed an increased susceptibility to reinstatement.
Similar changes in the self-administration of heroin and alcohol have been observed in animals with more prolonged access (31) or a history of dependence (39) . In related work, prolonged access to escalating doses of morphine in a rat self-administration model in which the animals self-regulated the dose of drug showed that repeated intake of opioids is associated with significant escalation in intake. Rats self-administered one of three doses of morphine (0.30, 1.00, or 3.00 mg/kg per infusion) during 7 daily 4-hour (short-access) sessions. In a second experiment, all animals were allowed 18-hour sessions of self-administration for 7 consecutive days and were randomly assigned to a self-escalation, individual-choice group or a fixed morphine dose group (38) ( Table 1 ). For the short-access 4-hour sessions, the dose of 0.30 mg/kg morphine per injection did not adequately support stable self-administration, but higher doses did. The animals who had 18-hour extended access in the self-escalation model even on day 1 administered more morphine than the fixed-dose group. The total daily consumption from day 1 was approximately 45 mg/kg and with escalation reached significance by day 4 and continued through day 7. By day 7, the animals were self-administering an average of 165 mg/kg per day of morphine. These results dramatically demonstrate escalation in morphine intake, consistent with studies of escalation in heroin intake described by heroin addicts (38) ( Figure 3 ). Similar results were obtained with 23-hour access to heroin, in which rats reached daily levels of up to 3.0 mg/kg per day and showed significant changes in circadian patterns that paralleled the escalation in intake (40) .
Figure 3. Escalation of Morphine Intake a
a Reprinted with permission of John Wiley & Sons.
b Significant between-group difference in morphine administration.
c Significant within-group increase of morphine intake over sessions 6 and 7 versus sessions 1–5.
Ethanol-dependent rats will self-administer significantly more ethanol during acute withdrawal than rats in a nondependent state. In these studies, Wistar rats are trained with a sweet solution fadeout procedure to self-administer ethanol in a two-lever operant situation in which one lever delivers 0.1 ml of 10% ethanol and the other lever delivers 0.1 ml of water. Nondependent animals typically self-administer doses of ethanol sufficient to produce blood alcohol levels averaging 25–30 mg % at the end of a 30-minute session, but rats made dependent on ethanol with ethanol vapor chambers self-administer three to four times as much ethanol ( Table 1 ). With unlimited access to ethanol during a full 12 hours of withdrawal, the animals will maintain blood alcohol levels above 100 mg % (39) . When the animals were subjected to repeated withdrawals and ethanol intake was charted over repeated abstinence, operant responding was enhanced by 30%–100% for up to 4–8 weeks postwithdrawal. Similar but even more dramatic results have been obtained with intermittent access to ethanol vapors (14 hours on, 10 hours off) (4143) . These results suggest an increase in ethanol self-administration in animals with a history of dependence that is not observed in animals maintained on limited access to ethanol of 30 minutes/day. The increase in responding has been hypothesized to be linked to changes in reward set point that invoke the theoretical concepts of tolerance or allostasis.

Brain Reward Dysfunction in Escalation

To test the hypothesis that the escalation of drug intake reflects the development of motivational dependence, brain reward thresholds were compared in escalated and nonescalated rats immediately before and after a session of cocaine self-administration. Two groups of rats were differentially exposed to cocaine self-administration for 1 hour (short access) or 6 hours (long access). The animals were prepared with bipolar electrodes in either the right or the left posterior lateral hypothalamus. One week after surgery, the animals were trained to respond to electrical brain stimulation. Brain stimulation reward thresholds were assessed in mA according to a modified discrete-trial current-threshold procedure (44) . Reward thresholds were measured in all rats two times per day, at 3 hours and 17–22 hours after each daily self-administration session.
Elevation in baseline reward thresholds temporally preceded and was highly correlated with escalation in cocaine intake (32) ( Figure 4 ). Further observation revealed that postsession elevations in reward thresholds failed to return to baseline levels before the onset of each subsequent self-administration session, thereby deviating more and more from control levels. The progressive elevation in reward thresholds was associated with a dramatic escalation in cocaine consumption in long-access rats, as previously observed. The rate of elevation in reward thresholds measured 1 hour before the daily access to cocaine (i.e., slope of the elevation) was highly correlated with the intensity of escalation in total cocaine intake. These results show that the elevation in brain reward thresholds following prolonged access to cocaine failed to return to baseline levels between repeated prolonged exposure to cocaine self-administration, thus creating a greater and greater elevation in baseline reward thresholds. These data provide compelling evidence for brain reward dysfunction in escalated cocaine self-administration. Similar results have been obtained during escalation of heroin intake in 23-hour-access rats (45) .
Figure 4. Relationship Between Elevation in Intracranial Self-Stimulation Reward Thresholds and Cocaine Intake Escalation a
a Adapted with permission of the Nature Publishing Group (http://www.nature.com/).
b Tests of simple main effects showed significant difference (p<0.05) compared to drug-naive and/or short-access rats.

Stress Hormone Measures in Drug Self-Administration Escalation Models

Previous work has shown a key role for activation of the HPA axis in all aspects of cocaine dependence as measured in animal models (46) . In an escalation model, when rats were divided into groups according to the doses of cocaine that were available for self-administration, positive correlations were found between presession corticosterone levels and the amount of cocaine self-administered (but only at the lowest level of 0.25 mg/kg and not at 0.50, 1.00, or 2.00 mg/kg) (34) . Locomotor activity and plasma corticosterone levels before self-administration and food-reinforced lever pressing predicted self-administration along with high response to novelty, but again only with the very lowest dose of cocaine (0.25 mg/kg). There were no correlations between any of these pre-cocaine exposure factors and the subsequent pattern of self-administration in low, moderate, or high doses of cocaine (0.50, 1.00, or 2.00 mg/kg per infusion) (35) . These findings indicated that predictable individual differences in cocaine self-administration are relevant only when the very low doses are used and are immediately reversed by increasing the doses of cocaine (34, 35) . Low-dose psychostimulant effects may be related to initial vulnerability to drug use, and activation of the HPA axis may contribute to such vulnerability (47, 48) . Such low-dose initial actions may parallel the phenomena of locomotor sensitization in which activation of the HPA axis has been shown to faciliate locomotor sensitization (49) . Also, glucocorticoid antagonists block low-dose cocaine self-administration (50) and stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine self-administration (51) .
Prolactin also is a well-recognized stress-responsive hormone involving mechanisms in both hypothalamic and pituitary regions. During each of 5 days of cocaine self-administration (the Kreek group), prolactin levels were significantly lower at the end of the self-administration period than at the beginning, presumably due to the increase in perisynaptic dopamine levels in the midbrain and in the tuberinfundibular dopaminergic system, the site of modulation of prolactin release in mammals. Unlike cortisol levels that persisted as abnormal for several days after cocaine withdrawal, prolactin levels were restored to baseline values within 1 day of withdrawal (35) . These results suggest the hypothesis that prolactin also may contribute to the dysregulation of neuroendocrine function that characterizes acute withdrawal from psychostimulant drugs.

The Role of CRF in the Motivational Effects of Excessive Drug Taking in Dependent Animals

Long-term exposure to ethanol vapors sufficient to induce dependence produces increases in ethanol self-administration during acute withdrawal and during protracted abstinence (5254) . Neuropharmacological studies have shown that enhanced ethanol self-administration during acute withdrawal and protracted abstinence can be reduced dose-dependently by intracerebroventricular administration of a competitive CRF antagonist (55) . However, identical doses and administration of CRF antagonists to nondependent rats had no effect on the self-administration of ethanol. In these studies, male Wistar rats were trained to respond to ethanol (10%) or water in a two-lever free-choice design. The rats received either ethanol vapor (dependent group) or air control (nondependent group). Both groups of rats were tested following a 3–4 week period during which the dependent rats exhibited target blood alcohol levels of 150–200 mg % in alcohol vapor chambers. The rats were tested in 30-minute sessions 2 hours after the dependent rats were removed from the chambers. The results showed that the CRF antagonist d -Phe-CRF 12–41 dose-dependently decreased operant responding for ethanol in ethanol vapor-exposed rats during early withdrawal but had no effect in air control rats (55) ( Figure 5 ). The same competitive CRF antagonist also dose-dependently decreased operant responding for ethanol in rats after acute withdrawal (3–5 weeks after vapor exposure) with a history of ethanol vapor exposure but had no effect in air control rats. Similar results have been obtained with direct administration of the competitive CRF 1 /CRF 2 antagonist d -Phe-CRF 12–41 directly into the amygdala (42) and with systemic administration of small molecule CRF antagonists (43, 56) . CRF dysregulation in the amygdala has been observed to persist up to 6 weeks postabstinence (57) . These results suggest that during the development of ethanol dependence there is a recruitment of CRF activity in the rat of motivational significance that can persist into protracted abstinence. Preliminary results have shown similar effects of systemic administration of CRF 1 receptor antagonists in the escalation in cocaine intake associated with extended access (unpublished study by Specio SE et al.) and in rats with prolonged extended access to heroin (unpublished study by Greenwell TN et al.). The CRF 1 antagonist antalarmin and related CRF antagonists dose-dependently decreased cocaine and heroin self-administration in escalated animals.
Figure 5. Effects of d -Phe-CRF 12–41 on Responding for Ethanol and Water 2 to 5 Weeks After Exposure to Long-Term Ethanol Vapor a
a Reprinted with permission of Lippincott Williams & Wilkins from Valdez GR, Roberts AJ, Chan K, Davis H, Brennan M, Zorilla EP, Koob GF: Increased ethanol self-administration and anxiety-like behavior during acute ethanol withdrawal and protracted abstinence: regulation by corticotrophin-releasing factor. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 2002; 26:1498, Figure 2. Control rats were exposed to air vapor. Rats were microinjected intracerebroventricularly with 0–10 mg of d -Phe-CRF 12–41 (N=8 per group) with a within-subject Latin square design 2 weeks after removal from the vapor chambers. The number of lever presses for ethanol and water were measured 10 minutes after injection. Following the initial test session, the rats were returned to their home cages and left undisturbed. The testing procedures were repeated over the next 3 weeks until the Latin square design was complete. p<0.05.
b Tukey’s test, compared to controls; p<0.05.
c Tukey’s test, compared to ethanol-exposed rats injected with 0 mg d -Phe-CRF 12–41 ; p<0.05.
d Tukey’s test, compared to ethanol-exposed rats injected with 0 mg d -Phe-CRF 12–41 and controls.
Studies on CRF in addiction in humans have been largely limited to CRF challenge studies and measures of CRF in CSF lumbar samples. During short-term and protracted abstinence, human alcoholics showed a blunted cortisol response to CRF (58, 59) . An elevation of CSF CRF from lumbar samples in human alcoholics during acute withdrawal (day 1) has been observed (60) . These results are consistent with the animal studies cited above in that they reflect a dysregulation of the HPA axis during protracted abstinence and a potential activation of extrahypothalamic CRF during acute withdrawal.

Hormonal and Brain Changes in Excessive Drug-Taking Associated With Escalation

Animals allowed to self-administer cocaine for 10 hours during their active period showed a blunting of the normal circadian rhythm of cortisol plasma levels by the end of the first day of self-administration of cocaine (34) . By the third day of cocaine self-administration, there was a complete reversal of the normal cortisol circadian rhythm, with levels of plasma cortisol much higher at the end of the cocaine self-administration session than at the beginning, when they should have been the highest. The blunting of the normal circadian rhythm of cortisol levels continued into withdrawal at 1 and 4 days after the last self-administration of cocaine.
With repeated long-access high-dose cocaine self-administration, daily corticosterone levels, as measured by the area under the plasma corticosterone time curve, progressively decreased (and much further than those that had been observed with 5 days of cocaine self-administration). Similar findings have been made in human long-term cocaine addicts in a clinical laboratory setting (61) . In contrast, the daily corticosterone area under the plasma concentration time curve in the short-access rats increased across testing, despite a relatively constant rate of self-administration (36) ( Figure 6 ). In addition, mRNA levels for proopiomelanocortin and the glucocorticoid receptor in the anterior pituitary were significantly lower in the long-access rats than in the short-access rats. However, no differences were found for quantitative measures of CRF mRNA in the amygdala in a direct comparison of short-access long-access low-dose and long-access high-dose animals. Also, corticosterone and hypothalamic CRF mRNA are increased during acute withdrawal from long-term cocaine administration (62) . Similar decreases in HPA activity have been observed with repeated alcohol administration in a binge model (6365) . Long-term daily administration of alcohol in a liquid diet showed a decrease in HPA axis activity that persisted up to 3 weeks postabstinence (66) . Neurochemical studies revealed increases in mRNA for enkephalin in the caudate putamen and increases in the dopamine D 2 receptor in the nucleus accumbens in long-access high-dose rats (37) . Differences also were not found in preprodynorphin mRNA levels across any of these three groups. In contrast to the findings in the nucleus accumbens, D 2 receptor levels in the anterior pituitary were significantly lower in the long-access rats than in the short-access rats. These findings suggest that at the neuroendocrine and neurochemical levels in the escalation model, there are significant differences in responses of stress-responsive hormones of the HPA axis and some significant differences in quantitatively measured mRNA levels of genes of potential interest both for the reinforcing or rewarding effects and for stress-responsive systems (37) .
Figure 6. Daily Areas Under the Curve for Plasma Corticosterone Under Basal Conditions (prior to self-administration testing and after self-administration training) on Days 1, 8, and 14 of Self-Administration Testing and on Days 1 and 10 of Extinction in Short-Access (N=7) and Long-Access (N=6) Rats a
a Areas under the curve were calculated from plasma corticosterone concentrations determined at three daily time points: 07:30, 11:00, and 06:30 hours. Adapted from Mantsch JR, Yuferov V, Mathieu-Kia A-M, Ho A, Kreek MJ: Neuroendocrine alterations in a high-dose, extended-access rat self-administration model of escalating cocaine use. Psychoneuroendocrinology © 2003; 28:836–862, Figure 2, with permission from Elsevier.
In studies of the escalation of morphine intake, self-regulated dosing of morphine was associated with rapid escalation of total daily consumption but not with alteration in consumption rates. The Kreek group has shown that the m-opioid receptor system is of seminal importance in reward and has a role in modulating the expression of many of the hormonal stress-responsive genes both in the hypothalamus and the anterior pituitary. Studies were conducted to determine the status of the m-opioid receptor activation system focused on two regions related to reward and pain, respectively: the amygdala and the thalamus. Animals in the long-access escalating-dose group showed significantly decreased morphine-stimulated [ 35 S]GTPgS binding in membranes prepared from both amygdala and thalamic nuclei compared to the fixed-dose and control groups with cell biological assay studies (38) . Escalating doses, which mimic the human pattern of morphine or heroin use, are associated with profound alterations in the function of m-opioid receptors. Changes in N -methyl- d -aspartic acid (NMDA) NR1 labeling in the tractus solitarius and also changes in AMPA GluR1 subunit labeling on dendrites in the basolateral amygdala were observed in animals subjected to a similar escalation in morphine dosing (67) . These results suggest that subject-regulated dosing is a useful approach for modeling dose escalation associated with opiate dependence and addiction (38) .

Allostasis Versus Homeostasis in Dependence and the Role of the Brain Stress Systems

Allostasis is defined as the process of achieving stability through change. An allostatic state is a state of chronic deviation of the regulatory system from its normal (homeostatic) operating level. Allostasis originally was formulated as a hypothesis to explain the physiological basis for changes in patterns of human morbidity and mortality associated with modern life (68) . High blood pressure and other pathology was linked to social disruption by brain-body interactions. Allostatic load is the cost to the brain and body of the deviation accumulating over time and reflecting in many cases pathological states and accumulation of damage. Using the arousal/stress continuum as their physiological framework, Sterling and Eyer (68) argued that homeostasis was not adequate to explain such brain-body interactions. The concept of allostasis has several unique characteristics that lend it more explanatory power. These characteristics include a continuous reevaluation of the organism’s need and continuous readjustments to new set points depending on demand. Allostasis can anticipate altered need, and the system can make adjustments in advance. Allostasis systems also were hypothesized to use past experience to anticipate demand (68) .
Self-administration of drugs may initiate a cascade of stress responses that play a role in allostatic, as opposed to homeostatic, responses (69) ( Figure 7 ). An acute binge of drug-taking beyond that of limited access produces an activation of the HPA axis, which activates or prolongs the activation of the brain reward systems and which, during a more prolonged binge, activates the brain stress systems. However, the neuroendocrine aspect of the stress response also has its capacity blunted, either by negative feedback or by depletion or both. Acute withdrawal from drugs of abuse produces opponent process-like changes in reward neurotransmitters in specific elements of reward circuitry associated with the ventral forebrain, as well as recruitment of brain stress systems that motivationally oppose the hedonic effects of drugs of abuse.
Figure 7. Brain Circuits Hypothesized to be Recruited at Different Stages of the Addiction Cycle as Addiction Moves from Positive Reinforcement to Negative Reinforcement a
a The top left circuit refers to the brain reward system, with a focus on the extended amygdala/lateral hypothalamic loop and extended amygdala/ventral pallidum loop. The bottom left circuit refers to the obsessive-compulsive loop of the dorsal striatum/pallidum and thalamus. The top right circuit refers to the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis which 1) feeds back to regulate itself, 2) activates the brain reward neurocircuit, and 3) facilitates the extrahypothalamic stress neurocircuit. The bottom right circuit refers to the brain stress circuits in feed-forward loops. CRF=corticotropin-releasing factor; BNST=bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; NE=norepinephrine. Adapted with permission of Cambridge University Press from Koob GF, Le Moal M: Drug addiction and allostasis, in Allostatis, Homeostasis, and the Costs of Physiological Adaptation. Edited by Schulkin J. New York, 2004, pp. 150–163, Figure 5.3, p. 155.
From the drug addiction perspective, allostasis is the process of maintaining apparent reward function stability through changes in reward and stress system neurocircuitry. The changes in brain and hormonal systems associated with the development of motivational aspects of withdrawal are hypothesized to be a major source of potential allostatic changes that drive and maintain addiction. The neuropharmacological contribution to the altered set point is hypothesized to involve not only decreases in reward function, including dopamine, serotonin, and opioid peptides, but also recruitment of brain stress systems such as CRF. All of these changes are hypothesized to be focused on a dysregulation of function within the neurocircuitry of the basal forebrain associated with the extended amygdala (central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis). The present formulation is an extension of the opponent process of Solomon and Corbit (70) to an allostatic framework with a hypothesized neurobiologic mechanism.
The initial experience of a drug with no prior drug history shows a positive hedonic response ( a-process ) and a subsequently minor negative hedonic response ( b-process ), each represented by increased and decreased functional activity of reward transmitters, respectively. The b-process also is hypothesized to involve recruitment of brain stress neurotransmitter function. However, insufficient time between readministering the drug to retain the a-process and limit the b-process leads to the transition to an allostatic reward state, as has been observed in the escalation of cocaine, methamphetamine, heroin, and ethanol intake in animal models. Under conditions of an allostatic reward state, the b-process never returns to the original homeostatic level before drug-taking resumes. This dysregulation is driven in part by an overactive HPA axis and subsequently overactive CNS CRF system and thus creates a greater and greater allostatic state in the brain reward systems and, by extrapolation, a transition to addiction.
Thus, in escalation situations (extended access), the counteradaptive opponent process does not simply balance the activational process ( a-process ) but, in fact, shows residual hysteresis. The results with cocaine escalation and brain reward thresholds provide empirical evidence for this hypothesis. The neurochemical, hormonal, and neurocircuitry changes observed during acute withdrawal are hypothesized to persist in some form even during postdetoxification, defining a state of “protracted abstinence.” The results described above are shedding some light on a potential role for the CRF brain and hormonal stress systems in protracted abstinence.
An allostatic view of drug addiction thus provides a heuristic model with which to explore residual changes following drug binges that contribute to vulnerability to relapse (1, 15, 71) . What should be emphasized is that from an allostatic perspective, the allostatic load in addiction is a persistent state of stress in which the CNS and HPA axis are chronically dysregulated. Such a state provides a change in baseline such that environmental events that would normally elicit drug-seeking behavior have even more impact. Recently, direct evidence for this has been documented in human cocaine addicts (72, 73) . Much work remains to define the neurochemistry and neurocircuitry of this residual stress state, and such information will be the key to its reversal and will provide important information for the prevention and treatment of drug addiction. The hypothesis suggested here is that the neurobiological bases for this complex syndrome of protracted abstinence may involve subtle molecular and cellular changes in stress system neurocircuitry associated with the extended amygdala.
The present review has focused on the CRF system and the HPA axis because this system is known to be a critical modulator of both hormonal and behavioral responses to stressors (7479) . However, there are many more stress regulatory systems in the brain that may also contribute to the allostatic changes hypothesized to be critical to the development and maintenance of motivational homeostasis, including norepinephrine (17), neuropeptide Y (80, 81), nociceptin (82), orexin, and vasopressin (83) . These same neurochemical systems may be involved in mediating anxiety disorders and other stress disorders, and the elucidation of the role of the stress axis in drug dependence may also provide insights into the role of these systems in other psychopathology.

Footnotes

Received March 22, 2005; revision received Nov. 2, 2005; accepted Dec. 14, 2005 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05030503). From the Committee on the Neurobiology of Addictive Disorders, the Scripps Research Institute, and the Laboratory on the Biology of Addictive Diseases, the Rockefeller University, New York. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Koob, Committee on the Neurobiology of Addictive Disorders, SP30-2400, the Scripps Research Institute, 10550 North Torrey Pines Rd., La Jolla, CA 92037; [email protected] (e-mail).
All authors report no competing interests.
Supported by NIH grants DA04398 (to Dr. Koob), DA04043 (to Dr. Koob), DA05130 (to Dr. Kreek), and DA00049 (to Dr. Kreek) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse; AA06420 (to Dr. Koob) and AA08459 (to Dr. Koob) from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; RR024143 (to Dr. Barry Coller, vice president of Rockefeller University) from the National Center for Research Resources; MH076537 (to Dr. Kreek) from NIMH; and DK26741 (to Dr. Koob) from the National Institute on Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases.
The authors thank Mike Arends, Mellany Santos, and Kitt Lavoie for their help with the preparation of this article.
This is publication number 16873-NP from the Scripps Research Institute.
This article was written as part of a celebration of the 30th anniversary of the inception of the National Institute on Drug Abuse and represents a synthesis of the work of the authors’ two laboratories.

References

1.
Koob GF: Allostatic view of motivation: implications for psychopathology, in Motivational Factors in the Etiology of Drug Abuse (series title: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol 50). Edited by Bevins RA, Bardo MT. Lincoln, Neb, University of Nebraska Press, 2004, pp 1–18
2.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994
3.
Koob GF, Le Moal M: Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science 1997; 278:52–58
4.
Dole VP, Nyswander ME, Kreek MJ: Narcotic blockade. Arch Intern Med 1966; 118:304–309
5.
Kreek MJ, Koob GF: Drug dependence: stress and dysregulation of brain reward pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend 1998; 51:23–47
6.
Kreek MJ: Methadone-related opioid agonist pharmacotherapy for heroin addiction: history, recent molecular and neurochemical research and future in mainstream medicine, in New Medications for Drug Abuse (series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol 909). Edited by Glick SD, MaisonNeuve IM. New York, New York Academy of Sciences, 2000, pp 186–216
7.
Kreek MJ: Opiates, opioids, SNP’s and the addictions, in Problems of Drug Dependence, 1999: Proceedings of the 61st Annual Scientific Meeting, The College on Problems of Drug Dependence, Inc. (series title: NIDA Research Monograph, vol 180). Edited by Harris LS. Bethesda, Md, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2000, pp 3–22
8.
Kreek MJ: Drug addictions: molecular and cellular endpoints, in The Biological Basis of Cocaine Addiction (series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol 937). Edited by Quinones-Jenab V. New York, New York Academy of Sciences, 2001, pp 27–49
9.
Kreek MJ: Gene diversity in the endorphin system: SNPs, chips and possible implications, in The Genomic Revolution: Unveiling the Unity of Life. Edited by Yudell M, DeSalle R. Washington, DC, Joseph Henry Press, 2002, pp 97–108
10.
Kreek MJ: Molecular and cellular neurobiology and pathophysiology of opiate addiction, in Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. Edited by Davis KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C. New York, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2002, pp 1491–1506
11.
Kreek MJ, Vocci FJ: History and current status of opioid maintenance treatments: blending conference session. J Subst Abuse Treat 2002; 23:93–105
12.
Kreek MJ, LaForge KS, Butelman E: Pharmacotherapy of addictions. Nat Rev Drug Discov 2002; 1:710–726, correction, 1:920
13.
Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA, LaForge KS: Genes associated with addiction: alcoholism, opiate, and cocaine addiction. Neuromolecular Med 2004; 5:85–108
14.
Mathieu-Kia AM, Kellogg SH, Butelman ER, Kreek MJ: Nicotine addiction: insights from recent animal studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002; 162:102–118
15.
Koob GF, Le Moal M: Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001; 24:97–129
16.
Koob GF, Ahmed SH, Boutrel B, Chen SA, Kenny PJ, Markou A, O’Dell LE, Parsons LH, Sanna PP: Neurobiological mechanisms in the transition from drug use to drug dependence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004; 27:739–749
17.
Koob GF: Corticotropin-releasing factor, norepinephrine and stress. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:1167–1180
18.
Zorrilla EP, Tache Y, Koob GF: Nibbling at CRF receptor control of feeding and gastrocolonic motility. Trends Pharmacol Sci 2003; 24:421–427
19.
Koob GF, Heinrichs SC: A role for corticotropin-releasing factor and urocortin in behavioral responses to stressors. Brain Res 1999; 848:141–152
20.
Baldwin HA, Britton KT, Koob GF: Behavioral effects of corticotropin-releasing factor, in Behavioral Aspects of Neuroendocrinology (series title: Current Topics in Neuroendocrinology, vol 10). Edited by Pfaff DW, Ganten D. Berlin, Springer-Verlag, 1990, pp 1–14
21.
Zorrilla EP, Koob GF: The therapeutic potential of CRF1 antagonists for anxiety. Expert Opin Investig Drugs 2004; 13:799–828
22.
Koob GF: The neurobiology of self-regulation failure in addiction: an allostatic view (commentary on Khantzian, “Understanding addictive vulnerability: an evolving psychodynamic perspective”). Neuro-Psychoanal 2003; 5:35–39
23.
Koob GF: The role of the striatopallidal and extended amygdala systems in drug addiction, in Advancing from the Ventral Striatum to the Extended Amygdala: Implications for Neuropsychiatry and Drug Abuse (series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol 877). Edited by McGinty JF. New York, New York Academy of Sciences, 1999, pp 445–460
24.
Koob GF: Neuroadaptive mechanisms of addiction: studies on the extended amygdala. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2003; 13:442–452
25.
Yuferov V, Nielsen D, Butelman E, Kreek MJ: Microarray studies of psychostimulant-induced changes in gene expression. Addict Biol 2005; 10:101–118
26.
Kreek MJ, Bart G, Lilly C, LaForge KS, Nielsen DA: Pharmacogenetics and human molecular genetics of opiate and cocaine addictions and their treatments. Pharmacol Rev 2005; 57:1–26
27.
Kreek MJ, Schlussman SD, Bart G, Laforge KS, Butelman ER: Evolving perspectives on neurobiological research on the addictions: celebration of the 30th anniversary of NIDA. Neuropharmacology 2004; 47(suppl 1):324–344
28.
Kreek MJ: Multiple drug abuse patterns and medical consequences, in Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation of Progress. Edited by Meltzer HY. New York, Raven Press, 1987, pp 1597–1604
29.
Kreek MJ: Rationale for maintenance pharmacotherapy of opiate dependence, in Addictive States (series title: Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease Research Publications, vol 70). Edited by O’Brien CP, Jaffe JH. New York, Raven Press, 1992, pp 205–230
30.
Ahmed SH, Koob GF: Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: change in hedonic set point. Science 1998; 282:298–300
31.
Ahmed SH, Walker JR, Koob GF: Persistent increase in the motivation to take heroin in rats with a history of drug escalation. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 22:413–421
32.
Ahmed SH, Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A: Neurobiological evidence for hedonic allostasis associated with escalating cocaine use. Nat Neurosci 2002; 5:625–626
33.
Ahmed SH, Lin D, Koob GF, Parsons LH: Escalation of cocaine self-administration does not depend on altered cocaine-induced nucleus accumbens dopamine levels. J Neurochem 2003; 86:102–113
34.
Mantsch JR, Schlussman SD, Ho A, Kreek MJ: Effects of cocaine self-administration on plasma corticosterone and prolactin in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2000; 294:239–247
35.
Mantsch JR, Ho A, Schlussman SD, Kreek MJ: Predictable individual differences in the initiation of cocaine self-administration by rats under extended-access conditions are dose-dependent. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 157:31–39
36.
Mantsch JR, Yuferov V, Mathieu-Kia AM, Ho A, Kreek MJ: Neuroendocrine alterations in a high-dose, extended-access rat self-administration model of escalating cocaine use. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003; 28:836–862
37.
Mantsch JR, Yuferov V, Mathieu-Kia AM, Ho A, Kreek MJ: Effects of extended access to high versus low cocaine doses on self-administration, cocaine-induced reinstatement and brain mRNA levels in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004; 175:26–36
38.
Kruzich PJ, Chen AC, Unterwald EM, Kreek MJ: Subject-regulated dosing alters morphine self-administration behavior and morphine-stimulated [35S]GTPgammaS binding. Synapse 2003; 47:243–249
39.
Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Cole M, Griffin P, Koob GF: Excessive ethanol drinking following a history of dependence: animal model of allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 22:581–594
40.
Chen SA, O’Dell L, Hoefer M, Greenwell TN, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF: Unlimited access to heroin self-administration: independent motivational markers of opiate dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006; 31:2692–2707; correction, 31:2802
41.
O’Dell LE, Roberts AJ, Smith RT, Koob GB: Enhanced alcohol self-administration after intermittent versus continuous alcohol vapor exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2004; 28:1676–1682
42.
Funk CK, O’Dell LE, Crawford EF, Koob GF: Corticotropin-releasing factor within the central nucleus of the amygdala mediates enhanced ethanol self-administration in withdrawn, ethanol-dependent rats. J Neurosci 2006; 26:11324–11332
43.
Funk CK, Zorrilla EP, Lee M-J, Rice KC, Koob GF: Cortiotropin-releasing factor 1 antagonists selectively reduce ethanol self-administration in ethanol-dependent rats. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:78–86
44.
Markou A, Koob GF: Intracranial self-stimulation thresholds as a measure of reward, in Behavioural Neuroscience: A Practical Approach, vol 2. Edited by Sahgal A. Oxford, UK, IRL Press, 1993, pp 93–115
45.
Kenny PJ, Chen SA, Kitamura O, Markou A, Koob GF: Conditioned withdrawal drives heroin consumption and decreases reward sensitivity. J Neurosci 2006; 26:5894–5900
46.
Goeders NE: Stress and cocaine addiction. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2002; 301:785–789
47.
Piazza PV, Le Moal M: Pathophysiological basis of vulnerability to drug abuse: role of an interaction between stress, glucocorticoids, and dopaminergic neurons. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 1996; 36:359–378
48.
Piazza PV, Le Moal M: The role of stress in drug self-administration. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1998; 19:67–74
49.
Antelman SM, Eichler AJ, Black CA, Kocan D: Interchangeability of stress and amphetamine in sensitization. Science 1980; 207:329–331
50.
Goeders NE, Peltier RL, Guerin GF: Ketoconazole reduces low dose cocaine self-administration in rats. Drug Alcohol Depend 1998; 53:67–77
51.
Mantsch JR, Goeders NE: Ketoconazole blocks the stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats: relationship to the discriminative stimulus effects of cocaine. Psychopharmacology 1999; 142:399–407
52.
Roberts AJ, Cole M, Koob GF: Intra-amygdala muscimol decreases operant ethanol self-administration in dependent rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1996; 20:1289–1298
53.
Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Cole M, Griffin P, Koob GF: Excessive ethanol drinking following a history of dependence: animal models of allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 22:581–594
54.
Rimondini R, Arlinde C, Sommer W, Heilig M: Long-lasting increase in voluntary ethanol consumption and transcriptional regulation in the rat brain after intermittent exposure to alcohol. FASEB J 2002; 16:27–35
55.
Valdez GR, Roberts AJ, Chan K, Davis H, Brennan M, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF: Increased ethanol self-administration and anxiety-like behavior during acute withdrawal and protracted abstinence: regulation by corticotropin-releasing factor. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2002; 26:1494–1501
56.
Gehlert DR, Cippitelli A, Thorsell A, Le AD, Hipskind PA, Hamdouchi C, Lu J, Hembre EJ, Cramer J, Song M, McKinzie D, Morin M, Ciccocioppo R, Heilig M: 3-(4-Chloro-2-morpholin-4-yl-thiazol-5-yl)-8-(1-ethylpropyl)-2,6-dimethyl-imidazo[1,2-b]pyridazine: a novel brain-penetrant, orally available corticotrophin-releasing factor receptor 1 antagonist with efficacy in animal models of alcoholism. J Neurosci 2007; 27:2718–2726
57.
Zorrilla EP, Valdez GR, Weiss F: Changes in levels of region CRF-like-immunoreactivity and plasma corticosterone during protracted drug withdrawal in dependent rats. Psychopharmacology 2001; 158:374–381
58.
Heuser I, von Bardeleben U, Boll E, Holsboer F: Response of ACTH and cortisol to human corticotropin-releasing hormone after short-term abstention from alcohol abuse. Biol Psychiatry 1988; 24:316–321
59.
Bailly D, Dewailly D, Beuscart R, Couplet G, Dumont P, Racadot A, Fossati P, Parquet PJ: Adrenocorticotropin and cortisol responses to ovine corticotropin-releasing factor in alcohol dependence disorder, preliminary report. Horm Res 1989; 31:72–75
60.
Adinoff B, Kiser JM, Martin PR, Linnoila M: Response of dehydroepiandrosterone to corticotropin-releasing hormone stimulation in alcohol-dependent subjects. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:1305–1307
61.
Aouizerate B, Ho A, Schluger JH, Perret G, Borg L, Le Moal M, Piazza PV, Kreek MJ: Glucorticoid negative feedback in methadone-maintained former heroin addicts with ongoing cocaine dependence: dose-response to dexamethasone suppression. Addict Biol 2006; 11:84–96
62.
Mantsch JR, Taves S, Khan T, Katz ES, Sajan T, Tang LC, Cullinan WE, Ziegler DR: Restraint-induced corticosterone secretion and hypothalamic CRH mRNA expression are augmented during acute withdrawal from chronic cocaine administration. Neurosci Lett (in press)
63.
Li Z, Kang SS, Lee S, Rivier C: Effect of ethanol on the regulation of corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF) gene expression. Mol Cell Neurosci 2005; 29:345–354
64.
Lee S, Schmidt ED, Tilders FJ, Rivier C: Effect of repeated exposure to alcohol on the response of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis of the rat, I: role of changes in hypothalamic neuronal activity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2001; 25:98–105
65.
Lee S, Rivier C: An initial, three-day-long treatment with alcohol induces at long-lasting phenomenon of selective tolerance in the activity of the rat hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. J Neurosci 1997; 17:8856–8866
66.
Rasmussen DD, Bolt BM, Bryant CA, Mitton DR, Larsen SA, Wilkinson CW: Chronic daily ethanol and withdrawal: 1, long-term changes in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Alcoholism: Clin Exper Res 2000; 24:1836–1849
67.
Glass MJ, Kruzich PJ, Kreek MJ, Pickel VM: Decreased plasma membrane targeting of NMDA-NR1 receptor subunit in dendrites of medial nucleus tractus solitarius neurons in rats self-administering morphine. Synapse 2004; 53:191–201
68.
Sterling P, Eyer J: Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain arousal pathology, in Handbook of Life Stress, Cognition and Health. Edited by Fisher S, Reason J. Chichester, UK, John Wiley & Sons, 1988, pp 629–649
69.
Koob GF, Le Moal M: Drug addiction and allostasis, in Allostasis, Homeostasis, and the Costs of Physiological Adaptation. Edited by Schulkin J. New York, Cambridge University Press, 2004, pp 150–163
70.
Solomon RL, Corbit JD: An opponent-process theory of motivation: 1, temporal dynamics of affect. Psychol Rev 1974; 81:119–145
71.
Koob GF, Le Moal M: Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the “dark side” of drug addiction. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8:1442–1444
72.
Fox HC, Talih M, Malison R, Anderson GM, Kreek MJ, Sinha R: Frequency of recent cocaine and alcohol use affects drug craving and associated responses to stress and drug-related cues. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2005; 30:880–891
73.
Sinha R, Garcia M, Paliwal P, Kreek MJ, Rounsaville BJ: Stress-induced cocaine craving and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses are predictive of cocaine relapse outcomes. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:324–331
74.
Koob GF, Heinrichs SC, Menzaghi F, Pich EM, Britton KT: Corticotropin releasing factor, stress and behavior. Semin Neurosci 1994; 6:221–229
75.
Vale W, Rivier C, Brown MR, Spiess J, Koob G, Swanson L, Bilezikjian L, Bloom F, Rivier J: Chemical and biological characterization of corticotropin releasing factor. Recent Prog Horm Res 1983; 39:245–270
76.
Smagin GN, Heinrichs SC, Dunn AJ: The role of CRH in behavioral responses to stress. Peptides 2001; 22:713–724
77.
Dunn AJ, Berridge CW: Physiological and behavioral responses to corticotropin-releasing factor administration: is CRF a mediator of anxiety or stress responses? Brain Res Rev 1990; 15:71–100
78.
Sarnyai Z, Shaham Y, Heinrichs SC: The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in drug addiction. Pharmacol Rev 2001; 53:209–243
79.
Heinrichs SC, Koob GF: Corticotropin-releasing factor in brain: a role in activation, arousal, and affect regulation. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2004; 311:427–440
80.
Heilig M, Koob GF, Ekman R, Britton KT: Corticotropin-releasing factor and neuropeptide Y: role in emotional integration. Trends Neurosci 1994; 17:80–85
81.
Heilig M, Soderpalm B, Engel JA, Widerlov E: Centrally administered neuropeptide Y (NPY) produces anxiolytic-like effects in animal anxiety models. Psychopharmacology 1989; 98:524–529
82.
Reinscheid RK, Civelli O: The orphanin FQ/nociceptin knockout mouse: a behavioral model for stress responses. Neuropeptides 2002; 36:72–76
83.
Zhou Y, Bendor J, Hofmann L, Randesi M, Ho A, Kreek MJ: Mu opioid receptor and orexin/hypocretin mRNA levels in the lateral hypothalamus and striatum are enhanced by morphine withdrawal. J Endocrinol 2006; 191:137–145

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1149 - 1159
PubMed: 17671276

History

Published online: 1 August 2007
Published in print: August, 2007

Authors

Details

Mary Jeanne Kreek, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share