Skip to main content
Full access
Articles
Published Online: 15 January 2016

Effect of Hippocampal and Amygdala Connectivity on the Relationship Between Preschool Poverty and School-Age Depression

Abstract

Objective:

In this study, the authors tested the hypothesis that poverty experienced in early childhood, as measured by income-to-needs ratio, has an impact on functional brain connectivity at school age, which in turn mediates influences on child negative mood/depression.

Method:

Participants were from a prospective longitudinal study of emotion development. Preschoolers 3–5 years of age were originally ascertained from primary care and day care sites in the St. Louis area and then underwent annual behavioral assessments for up to 12 years. Healthy preschoolers and those with a history of depression symptoms underwent neuroimaging at school age. Using functional MRI, the authors examined whole brain resting-state functional connectivity with the left and right hippocampus and amygdala.

Results:

Lower income-to-needs ratio at preschool age was associated with reduced connectivity between hippocampus and amygdala and a number of regions at school age, including the superior frontal cortex, lingual gyrus, posterior cingulate, and putamen. Lower income-to-needs ratio predicted greater negative mood/depression severity at school age, as did connectivity between the left hippocampus and the right superior frontal cortex and between the right amygdala and the right lingual gyrus. Connectivity mediated the relationship between income-to-needs ratio and negative mood/depression at the time of scanning.

Conclusions:

These findings suggest that poverty in early childhood, as assessed by at least one measure, may influence the development of hippocampal and amygdala connectivity in a manner leading to negative mood symptoms during later childhood.
Poverty is one of the most powerful risk factors for poor developmental outcomes (17). Children who grow up in poverty have poorer cognitive and educational outcomes and are at a higher risk for a range of mental illnesses, including depression and antisocial behaviors (8). The mechanisms by which poverty contributes to these negative developmental outcomes in children are multifaceted and include factors such as limitations on educational opportunities, family stress, and adverse environmental exposures, such as lead, cigarette smoke, poor nutrition, and air pollution (3, 6, 9).
The clear evidence for the negative outcomes associated with poverty has led to a growing number of studies examining how the early experience of poverty influences brain development as a potential mediating pathway. This literature was summarized recently by Brito and Noble (10), who identified evidence for a range of structural brain differences associated with various indicators of poverty (i.e., income-to-needs ratio, parent education), including reductions in whole brain gray and white matter volumes and reduced thickness in some brain areas. One of the most consistent findings has been an association between poverty indicators and reductions in hippocampal and amygdala volumes (1119), with every study that examined hippocampal or amygdala gray matter volumes reporting significant effects. Notably, there is evidence that these alterations in hippocampal and amygdala volumes partially mediate the influence of poverty on later behavioral problems in children (12).
These findings in humans are consistent with the animal literature showing effects of stress and environmental enrichment on hippocampal cell proliferation, dendritic length, and branching (2023). The human and animal findings regarding the amygdala are more complicated. As noted above, poverty is consistently associated with reduced amygdala volume. However, animal studies show increased amygdala dendritic arborization following stress (23, 24), and human studies of institutional rearing have also shown increased amygdala volume (25). The differences in amygdala findings between poverty and institutional-rearing studies may reflect the fact that children living in poverty can have intact attachment relationships, and thus the mechanisms may differ. Nonetheless, it is possible that the hippocampus and amygdala volume reductions associated with poverty also influence the connectivity of these regions. Both the hippocampus and the amygdala show positive resting-state functional connectivity (i.e., correlated spontaneous fluctuations in blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional MRI [fMRI] measurements) with each other and other regions in the medial and anterior temporal lobes, as well with as the ventral medial prefrontal cortex (at age 10 or older for this region) (2628). In addition, both the hippocampus and the amygdala show negative functional connectivity with left and right dorsal prefrontal and parietal regions (28). These regions showing negative correlations with the hippocampus and amygdala are the same regions that are activated in studies of emotion regulation (29, 30), and these anticorrelations are thought to indicate top-down regulation of emotion and stress responsivity supported by hippocampal and amygdala regions, suggesting a critical role for the integrity of these connections in mood and affective function.
Several studies have shown that early life stress and/or maternal deprivation are associated with altered connectivity between the amygdala and the ventral medial prefrontal cortex and pregenual anterior cingulate (3133), as well as other regions (34)—connections that are thought to be critical for effective emotion regulation. Furthermore, patterns of altered amygdala and/or hippocampal connectivity with the ventral medial and/or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex have also been seen in individuals with depressive psychopathology (28, 3537). Given the large literature linking early stress to later depression (38, 39) (although not selectively to depression), and given previous results of connectivity alterations associated with depression, these findings taken together could suggest that disruptions in amygdala or hippocampal connectivity may be one pathway by which early adversity contributes to risk for negative emotionality and depression. Few studies have investigated the effects of poverty on functional connectivity, although one showed that poverty predicted altered connectivity of the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex while processing emotional faces (40). However, to our knowledge, no studies have examined whether early poverty influences resting-state functional connectivity of either the amygdala or the hippocampus, which could reflect alterations in the integrity of intrinsic brain networks important for emotion responsivity and regulation.
The goal of the present study was to investigate the effects of poverty on childhood hippocampal and amygdala resting-state connectivity as a means to understanding whether alterations in these neural circuits may be one pathway contributing to the negative impact of poverty on child emotional outcomes and mental health. We also investigated whether poverty-related differences in amygdala or hippocampal connectivity predicted subsequent negative mood and depression severity and, if so, whether these differences in functional brain connectivity mediated the influence of poverty on subsequent negative mood.

Method

Participants

Participants were 105 children (54 of them girls) from a larger sample enrolled in the 12-year longitudinal Preschool Depression Study (N=305 at baseline, between ages 3 and 6). Children were invited to participate in the scanning portion of the study if they were psychiatrically healthy or if they had a history of clinical depression or anxiety (for additional detail, see the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article). All study procedures were reviewed and approved by the Washington University School of Medicine Institutional Review Board. Children were included in the present study if they had usable functional connectivity data at the first imaging session (between ages 7 and 12), as described below, and had parent report data on income-to-needs ratio from the baseline Preschool Depression Study assessment. There were no significant differences in demographic variables (age, sex, income-to-needs ratio) between the imaging subsample and the original sample. There were no significant differences in sex, baseline income-to-needs-ratio, or negative mood/depression scores at the time of scanning between the children with and without usable functional connectivity data. However, the children with usable data were slightly but significantly older than those without (mean ages, 9.93 and 9.53 years, respectively; t=2.12, df=181, p=0.035). Table 1 summarizes the demographic characteristics of the sample.
TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of a Sample of Children (N=105) Assessed Longitudinally From Preschool Age to School Age
Variable  
 N%
Parental education  
 <High school diploma44
 High school diploma55
 Some college4442
 College degree1918
 Some graduate school or graduate/professional degree3331
Race  
 African American5552
 Caucasian3937
 Other1110
Female5451
 MeanSD
Income-to-needs ratioa2.09b1.15
Age (years)9.93c1.31
a
Total family income divided by the federal poverty level for a family of that size closest to the year data were collected.
b
Range, 0–3.93.
c
Range, 7–12.

Measures

Income-to-needs ratio.

Income-to-needs ratio was operationalized as the total family income divided by the federal poverty level based on family size (41). The value was calculated based on baseline Preschool Depression Study data on caregiver-reported total family income and total number of people living in the household.

Psychiatric diagnosis and symptom severity.

Participants were assessed annually using the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (parent interview, ages 3–7) and the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (parent interview, ages 8 and up; child interview, ages 9 and up) (42; for further details, see reference 43). A negative mood/depression severity score at the time of study entry (preschool age, when income-to-needs ratio was measured) was computed by summing the number of core major depressive disorder symptoms (depressed mood, anhedonia, weight change, insomnia/hypersomnia, psychomotor agitation/retardation, fatigue, worthlessness/guilt, difficulty concentrating, suicidal ideation). A similar measure was created for school-age negative mood/depression severity, using items endorsed at the assessment wave closest to the scan. An anxiety severity score at the time of the scan was calculated using the core items from generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. An externalizing psychopathology score at the time of scanning was calculated using the core items from attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder.

Neuroimaging

Participants completed a neuroimaging battery that included high-resolution structural, diffusion imaging, functional task, and resting-state scans collected on a Siemens 3-T TIM TRIO scanner at Washington University. The resting-state data from the first imaging session were the focus of the present analysis. T1-weighted structural images were acquired in the sagittal plane using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) three-dimensional sequence (TR=2400 ms, TE=3.16 ms, flip angle=8°, 176 slices, field of view=256 mm, voxel size=1×1×1 mm). T2-weighted images were collected for registration purposes using a 3D-SPACE acquisition (TR=3200 ms, TE=497 ms, 160 slices, field of view=256 mm, voxel size=1×1×1 mm). Two resting-state fMRI scan runs were acquired in the vast majority of children (N=102 in the present sample; the other children ran out of time or were unable to stay still during the second run), each including 164 frames (6.8 minutes). Participants were instructed to rest with their eyes closed and to remain awake. Data were acquired using a spin-echo, echo-planar sequence sensitive to blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (T2*) (TR=2500 ms, TE=27 ms, field of view=256 mm, flip=90°, voxel size=4×4×4 mm, slices=36).

fMRI Preprocessing

Imaging data were preprocessed using the following steps: 1) correction for slice-dependent time shifts; 2) removal of the first four images of each run to allow BOLD signal to reach steady state; 3) elimination of odd/even slice intensity differences due to interpolated acquisition; 4) realignment of data acquired from each participant within and across scan runs to compensate for rigid body motion; 5) image intensity normalization to a whole-brain mode value of 1,000; 6) registration of the three-dimensional structural volume (T1) to an atlas template (WU 711-2B) in the Talairach coordinate system using a 12-parameter affine transform and resampling to 1-mm cubic representation (44, 45); 7) coregistration of the three-dimensional fMRI volume to the T2, and the T2 to the participant’s T1 structural image; and 8) transformation of the fMRI data to 3×3×3 mm voxel atlas space using a single affine 12-parameter transform. Additional processing of the resting-state functional connectivity was done with in-house software and is described in more detail in the online data supplement.

fMRI Analysis

We used FreeSurfer, version 5.1 (46, 47), to create anatomical region-of-interest masks. The hippocampus and amygdala were segmented bilaterally from each participant’s T1 anatomical image, downsampled to match the functional resolution of the atlas space (3×3×3 mm), and registered to the common atlas space. These images were summed, and a group-level anatomical mask was created by thresholding the region where at least half of the participants had overlap in their hippocampal and amygdala segmentations, allowing a more anatomically precise region of interest than relying on atlas regions of interest. The time-series from these four regions of interest were correlated with the time-series at every other voxel in the brain to create four whole brain voxel-wise correlation maps for each participant. Values in these maps were converted to z-statistics using Fisher’s r-to-z transform. These maps were used as the dependent measures described below.

Statistical Analysis

Normative connectivity patterns.

To establish the overall patterns of amygdala connectivity in our sample, two whole-brain one-sample t tests were run using in-house software (FIDL analysis package, http://www.nil.wustl.edu/labs/fidl/index.html) (48) to characterize significant voxel-wise resting-state functional connectivity (r-to-z transformed) with the left or right hippocampus and amygdala (patterns for the amygdala are reported in reference 49). Whole-brain t test results were thresholded based on Monte Carlo simulations (using 3dClustSim; afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/program_help/3dClustSim.html) at z≥3 and ≥17 contiguous voxels to achieve a whole brain false positive rate of p<0.05.
To test our main hypotheses, we examined whole-brain regression analyses predicting voxel-wise functional connectivity with the left or right hippocampus and the left or right amygdala. After screening for outliers (none were above three standard deviations in univariate analysis, and none had Mahalanobis distance metrics greater than p<0.001 in multivariate analyses), baseline income-to-needs ratio was the predictor of interest, controlling for ethnicity (African American versus white and other versus white), sex (female versus male), and age at the time of scanning, with both income-to-needs ratio and age mean centered. Whole brain z-maps for the effects of income-to-needs ratio were thresholded as above based on Monte Carlo simulations at z≥3 and ≥17 contiguous voxels to control for multiple comparisons (whole brain p<0.05). Average connectivity values within each significant cluster were extracted for each participant to examine the relationship of these connectivity measures to potential mediators and outcomes.
To examine whether the regions showing relationships between income-to-needs ratio and amygdala/hippocampal connectivity related to negative mood/depression, we computed linear regressions using connectivity measures extracted from the identified regions of interest to predict negative mood/depression scores at the time of the scan, controlling for age and sex. To control false positives, we corrected for the number of regions identified within the amygdala connectivity analyses (p=0.05/7=0.007) and within the hippocampal connectivity analyses (p=0.05/4=0.0125). When these results were significant, we then tested whether either income-to-needs ratio or connectivity related to depression severity at the time of scanning even when accounting for depression severity at study entry (when income-to-needs ratio was measured). Significant effects in such analyses indicated a relationship to change in negative mood/depression severity over time. We then asked whether the connectivity measures mediated the relationship between income-to-needs ratio and negative mood/depression at the time of scanning, using the PROCESS procedure in SPSS (50, 51), with age at the time of scanning, sex, and ethnicity as covariates.

Results

Characterizing Normative Hippocampal and Amygdala Connectivity Patterns

Figure 1 presents the results of whole-brain one-sample t tests of hippocampal and amygdala functional connectivity in this sample, with these seeds defined anatomically using FreeSurfer. Consistent with results reported in the literature, both the left and right hippocampus and amygdala showed strong positive connectivity with much of the subcortex, including contralateral homologous regions, striatum, brainstem, posterior insula, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Additionally, both the hippocampus and amygdala showed strong negative connectivity with much of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, lateral prefrontal cortex, anterior insula, cingulate cortex, and parietal lobe. The hippocampus also showed positive connectivity with posterior components of the default mode network (e.g., the posterior cingulate and the lateral parietal cortex).
FIGURE 1. Functional Connectivity of Hippocampus and Amygdala and Its Relationship to Income-to-Needs Ratioa
a Regions in red are those that show positive functional connectivity with the respective seed regions. Regions in blue are those that show negative functional connectivity with the respective seed regions. Regions in green are those that show relationships to income-to-needs ratio. The regions listed correspond to those listed in Table 2.

Does Income-to-Needs Ratio Predict Hippocampal and/or Amygdala Connectivity?

Whole-brain z-maps for the effects of income-to-needs ratio, controlling for ethnicity, sex, and age at the time of scanning, are presented in Table 2 and Figure 1. Income-to-needs ratio negatively predicted connectivity between the left hippocampus and regions in the right fusiform and the right superior frontal gyrus, as well as between the right hippocampus and one region in the right superior frontal gyrus (higher income-to-needs ratio predicted more negative connectivity). In addition, income-to-needs ratio positively predicted connectivity between the left hippocampus and a region in the right posterior cingulate (higher income-to-needs ratio predicted more positive connectivity). Income-to-needs ratio also negatively predicted connectivity between the left amygdala and the right superior frontal gyrus and right precuneus as well as between the right amygdala and regions in the right lingual gyrus, left superior parietal lobe, and left paracentral gyrus. Income-to-needs ratio also positively predicted connectivity between the left amygdala and both the left putamen and the left middle temporal gyrus. For the majority of these connectivity patterns related to poverty, higher income-to-needs ratio predicted stronger connectivity in the normative direction (e.g., more positive for regions that typically show positive connectivity and more negative for those regions that typically show negative connectivity). The overlap between regions showing overall connectivity with either the hippocampus or the amygdala and those connections showing relationships to income-to-needs ratio are illustrated in green in Figure 1. There were some regions showing functional connectivity relations to income-to-needs ratio that did not appear in the average functional connectivity maps, including left hippocampus to right fusiform, right hippocampus to medial superior frontal cortex, and left amygdala to middle temporal regions.
TABLE 2. Functional Connectivity of Hippocampus and Amygdala Predicted by Income-to-Needs Ratio
Seed RegionRegionaAverage Fisher’s r to Z With Seed RegionbBrodmann’s AreaxyzSize (mm3)r With Income-to-Needs Ratioc
Left hippocampus        
 Right fusiform gyrus–0.05***1946–72–12648–0.40
 Right posterior cingulate0.26***3010–60158370.29
 Right superior frontal gyrus–0.14***8264839972–0.40
Right hippocampus        
 Medial superior frontal cortex–0.001945424594–0.34
Left amygdala        
 Left putamen0.08*** –20335130.30
 Left middle temporal gyrus–0.00337–50–5134590.37
 Right superior frontal gyrus–0.11***8223939513–0.31
 Right precuneus–0.06***72–7248516–0.39
Right amygdala        
 Right lingual gyrus–0.04**182–93–18459–0.32
 Left superior parietal lobe–0.07***7–8–63571,755–0.28
 Left paracentral gyrus–0.03*5–4–4263972–0.25
a
The regions listed correspond to those listed in Figure 1.
b
To illustrate the direction of “typical” connectivity.
c
For descriptive purposes, to illustrate the direction of the relationships. Significance is not indicated since the regions were selected based on their significant relationship to income-to-needs ratio.
*
p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

Does Poverty and/or Brain Connectivity Predict Negative Mood/Depression Severity at the Time of Scanning?

Income-to-needs ratio from study entry at preschool age significantly negatively predicted negative mood/depression severity at the time of scanning at school age (β=−0.27, p=0.03). In addition, connectivity between the right amygdala and the right lingual gyrus (β=0.28, p=0.004) and between the left hippocampus and the right superior frontal cortex (β=0.27, p=0.005) were associated with negative mood/depression after correcting for multiple comparisons. These relationships, illustrated in the scatterplots shown in Figure 2, indicate that less negative connectivity (more positive) was associated with greater depression. Furthermore, both income-to-needs ratio and the connectivity measures continued to relate to negative mood/depression at the time of the scan even after accounting for negative mood/depression at preschool age (all p values, <0.05), indicating a relationship between preschool levels of poverty, connectivity, and a change (i.e., increase) in negative mood/depression over time.
FIGURE 2. Functional Connectivity of Hippocampus and Amygdala in Relation to Income-to-Needs Ratio and Depression at Time of Scana
a Scatterplots A and B illustrate the relationships of amygdala to lingual gyrus and hippocampal connectivity to income-to-needs ratio, respectively, and scatterplots C and D illustrate the relationships of these connections to depression symptoms, respectively, at the time of scanning.

Does Amygdala/Hippocampal Connectivity Mediate the Relationship Between Poverty and Negative Mood/Depression at the Time of Scanning?

We next examined whether connectivity between the right amygdala and the right lingual gyrus and/or between the left hippocampus and the right superior frontal gyrus mediated the relationship between poverty and negative mood/depression measured at the time of the scan, since all three were related to depression in the analysis presented above. As shown in Figure 3, hippocampal and amygdala connectivity significantly mediated the relationship between income-to-needs ratio and negative mood/depression at the time of the scan.
FIGURE 3. Illustration of Functional Connectivity Mediation of Relationship Between Income-to-Needs Ratio and Depression Severity at Time of Scana
a This dual mediation model includes two “A” paths (income-to-needs ratio to both left hippocampus/right superior frontal and right amygdala/right lingual gyrus connectivity) and thus also two corresponding “B” paths for the relationship of these functional connections to depression. The fact that both A and both B paths are statistically significant indicates that each of these functional connections accounts for independent variance. Beta weights are standardized. Path C (in black) is the total effect of income-to-needs ratio on depression severity with functional connectivity not in the model; it is statistically significant. Path C′ (in red) is the direct effect of income-to-needs ratio on depression severity with functional connectivity in the model. Path C′ is no longer significant with the connectivity measures in the modeling, indicating that the connectivity measures statistically mediate the relationship between poverty and subsequent depression.
All of the relationships reported above hold if we control for whether or not the child’s mother had depression. In addition, as noted, numerous studies have found that poverty predicts hippocampal and amygdala volume, including our own previous work on the hippocampus. While poverty was related to reduced hippocampal and amygdala volume in the present sample (see the online data supplement), these volumetric changes do not account for the relationship between income-to-needs ratio and connectivity (see the data supplement). Furthermore, these relationships were not present for measures of anxious mood or externalizing psychopathology (see the data supplement).

Discussion

Our goal in this study was to investigate whether poverty was associated with alterations in the functional connectivity of either the amygdala or the hippocampus and, if so, whether these alterations were associated with severity of negative mood/depression. We found that income-to-needs ratio significantly predicted the connectivity of both the amygdala and the hippocampus bilaterally, a novel finding that has not previously been shown. For the majority of regions, lower income-to-needs ratio was associated with a reduction in the normative pattern of connectivity for these regions, such as reduced negative connectivity between the left hippocampus and amygdala and the right superior frontal cortex, functional connections thought to play a key role in emotion regulation. Furthermore, both income-to-needs ratio and amygdala/hippocampus connectivity were associated with the severity of negative mood/depression at the time of scanning, even after controlling for negative mood/depression at preschool age. These findings indicate that both poverty and connectivity were associated with an increase in negative mood/depression over time, as well as with overall severity levels at the time of scanning. Connectivity between the left hippocampus and the right superior frontal gyrus and between the right amygdala and the right lingual gyrus mediated the relationship between income-to-needs ratio and negative mood/depression at the time of scanning. Numerous studies have shown volumetric differences associated with poverty, including consistent reductions in amygdala and hippocampal volumes, and a recent large-scale study showed reductions in surface area associated with poverty (52), which mediated links between poverty and cognitive function. Notably, the alterations in connectivity associated with income-to-needs ratio found in the present study were not secondary to an influence of poverty on amygdala or hippocampal volume, and they point to the importance of function as well as structure in the pathways by which poverty shapes child brain function and subsequent negative cognitive, emotional, and mental health outcomes.
Our results are consistent with a growing body of literature documenting that the early experience of poverty has a host of negative influences on child development and that at least some of these negative outcomes are mediated by the relationship of poverty to brain structure and function. The present findings identify a relationship between functional connectivity among regions known to be critical for effective emotion regulation as a mediator of the detrimental effects of poverty on negative emotional outcomes. In particular, one of our key findings was a relationship between lower income-to-needs ratio and reduced negative connectivity between the left hippocampus and amygdala and the right superior frontal gyrus. Patterns of anticorrelations between frontal regions and amygdala/hippocampal regions are thought to reflect top-down regulation of emotion and stress reactivity by cognitive control systems that can help implement effective emotion regulation (29, 30). Of note, we do not mean to imply that these alterations in hippocampal-to-prefrontal connectivity associated with poverty reflect changes in structural connectivity, as regions can show functional connectivity even in the absence of direct structural connections. Furthermore, we do not suggest a unique relationship to either the left hippocampus or the superior prefrontal cortex, as examining images with a less conservative statistical threshold indicates that this area of connectivity extends down into the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and is present for the right as well as the left hippocampal seeds, albeit at a less significant level. Nonetheless, the fact that the reduction in left hippocampus-to-right superior frontal connectivity was associated with negative mood/depression at the time of scanning is consistent with a hypothesized role for such connectivity patterns in emotion regulation and is consistent with other work showing a relationship between altered hippocampal-to-prefrontal connectivity and depression (28). Furthermore, these findings are generally consistent with other published results from this sample, including reduced hippocampal and amygdala volumes associated with poverty (11), reduced amygdala-to-cognitive control network connectivity among children who have a history of depression or who are at risk for depression (35), and an interaction between genetic variation associated with increased stress reactivity and life events in predicting reduced hippocampal and amygdala volume (53).
The finding that connectivity between the right amygdala and the right lingual gyrus also mediated the relationship between income-to-needs ratio and negative mood/depression at the time of scanning was somewhat more surprising. Our whole-brain analysis of the normative patterns of connectivity with the amygdala did not reveal significant amygdala-to-lingual gyrus connectivity, although average connectivity was significant, albeit modest, when examined as a region of interest (see Table 2). Thus, it is not clear whether this relationship indicates the presence of anomalous positive connectivity only in individuals with low income and high depression, or the presence of negative connectivity only in those with high income and low depression. However, previous work on amygdala connectivity has demonstrated negative correlations between both lateral basal and superficial amygdala regions and the lingual gyrus, although in a somewhat more superior region than we saw (26). The lingual gyrus has been related to visual form and word processing, but not typically with emotion processing. However, a growing number of studies have reported altered activity of the lingual gyrus in relationship to negative emotion processing in individuals with mood and psychotic disorders (5457), particularly in relationship to the modulation of negative emotions. Combined with our current results, this previous work suggests the need for more focused research on the role of the lingual gyrus, and its connectivity with the amygdala, in emotion processing and emotion regulation.
A potential limitation of these data is that the original study sample was oversampled for preschoolers with symptoms of depression, which may limit generalizability. Specifically, we found that lower income-to-needs ratio was associated with higher negative mood/depression. Some previous work focusing on categorical diagnoses suggested that change in poverty had stronger effects on disruptive behavioral problems (e.g., conduct) than on emotional (e.g., depression/anxiety) problems in children (58, 59). However, work in adults provides evidence for a causal relationship between poverty and depression (6063). In general, rates of emotional disorders are much lower than rates of disruptive behavioral symptoms in children, potentially limiting the power to detect effects in childhood. Our sample was enriched for depression symptoms, which may have allowed greater power to detect such relationships with depression as compared with anxiety or disruptive disorders. Thus, it would be important in future work to replicate these results in a more epidemiological sample. It is also likely that the relationships between connectivity and depression are more complex and bidirectional, and the present study design cannot definitely address causality. Another limitation is that this analysis focused on income-to-needs ratio, and further work is needed to elucidate the impact of other factors that are also frequently associated with poverty. We did not find that our results were influenced by maternal support or life events (see the online data supplement), but future work would benefit from including assessments of factors such as nutrition, school quality, and other indicators of enriched versus impoverished environments known to be associated with low income-to-needs ratio.
In summary, this study provides novel data suggesting that poverty influences functional brain connectivity in regions thought to be critical for emotion regulation, and that these changes in connectivity are a mediating factor by which poverty is associated with subsequent negative emotional and mental health outcome. These data add to the growing awareness of the immense public health crisis represented by the huge number of children growing up in poverty and the likely long-lasting impact this experience has on brain development and on negative mood and depression.

Footnote

Dr. Barch has served as a consultant for Amgen, Pfizer, Takeda, and Roche and has a contract to analyze imaging data for Pfizer. Dr. Luby has received royalties from Guilford Press. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2015.15081014.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Carneiro PM, Heckman JJ: Human capital policy, in Inequality in America: What Role for Human Capital Policies? Edited by Heckman JJ, Krueger AB, Friedman BM. Cambridge, Mass, MIT Press, 2003
2.
Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ: The effects of poverty on children. The Future of Children: Children and Poverty 1997; 7:55–71
3.
Freedman D, Woods GW: Neighborhood effects, mental illness, and criminal behavior: a review. J Politics Law 2013; 6:1–16
4.
Leung JT, Shek DT: Poverty and adolescent developmental outcomes: a critical review. Int J Adolesc Med Health 2011; 23:109–114
5.
Perkins SC, Finegood ED, Swain JE: Poverty and language development: roles of parenting and stress. Innov Clin Neurosci 2013; 10:10–19
6.
Rauch SA, Lanphear BP: Prevention of disability in children: elevating the role of environment. The Future of Children: Children and Poverty 2012; 22:193–217
7.
Raver CC: Low-income children’s self-regulation in the classroom: scientific inquiry for social change. Am Psychol 2012; 67:681–689
8.
Yoshikawa H, Aber JL, Beardslee WR: The effects of poverty on the mental, emotional, and behavioral health of children and youth: implications for prevention. Am Psychol 2012; 67:272–284
9.
Roosa MW, Jones S, Tein JY, et al: Prevention science and neighborhood influences on low-income children’s development: theoretical and methodological issues. Am J Community Psychol 2003; 31:55–72
10.
Brito NH, Noble KG: Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Front Neurosci 2014; 8:276
11.
Luby J, Belden A, Botteron K, et al: The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr 2013; 167:1135–1142
12.
Hanson JL, Nacewicz BM, Sutterer MJ, et al: Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 77:314–323
13.
Jednoróg K, Altarelli I, Monzalvo K, et al: The influence of socioeconomic status on children’s brain structure. PLoS One 2012; 7:e42486
14.
Hanson JL, Chandra A, Wolfe BL, et al: Association between income and the hippocampus. PLoS One 2011; 6:e18712
15.
Noble KG, Grieve SM, Korgaonkar MS, et al: Hippocampal volume varies with educational attainment across the life-span. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:307
16.
Noble KG, Houston SM, Kan E, et al: Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Dev Sci 2012; 15:516–527
17.
Butterworth P, Cherbuin N, Sachdev P, et al: The association between financial hardship and amygdala and hippocampal volumes: results from the PATH through life project. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2012; 7:548–556
18.
Piras F, Cherubini A, Caltagirone C, et al: Education mediates microstructural changes in bilateral hippocampus. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; 32:282–289
19.
Staff RT, Murray AD, Ahearn TS, et al: Childhood socioeconomic status and adult brain size: childhood socioeconomic status influences adult hippocampal size. Ann Neurol 2012; 71:653–660
20.
van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH: Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci 2000; 1:191–198
21.
Hirase H, Shinohara Y: Transformation of cortical and hippocampal neural circuit by environmental enrichment. Neuroscience 2014; 280:282–298
22.
Okuda H, Tatsumi K, Makinodan M, et al: Environmental enrichment stimulates progenitor cell proliferation in the amygdala. J Neurosci Res 2009; 87:3546–3553
23.
Eiland L, Ramroop J, Hill MN, et al: Chronic juvenile stress produces corticolimbic dendritic architectural remodeling and modulates emotional behavior in male and female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012; 37:39–47
24.
Vyas A, Mitra R, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, et al: Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci 2002; 22:6810–6818
25.
Tottenham N, Hare TA, Quinn BT, et al: Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev Sci 2010; 13:46–61
26.
Roy AK, Shehzad Z, Margulies DS, et al: Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 2009; 45:614–626
27.
Marquez de la Plata CD, Garces J, Shokri Kojori E, et al: Deficits in functional connectivity of hippocampal and frontal lobe circuits after traumatic axonal injury. Arch Neurol 2011; 68:74–84
28.
Cao X, Liu Z, Xu C, et al: Disrupted resting-state functional connectivity of the hippocampus in medication-naive patients with major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord 2012; 141:194–203
29.
Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, et al: Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 2014; 24:2981–2990
30.
Diekhof EK, Geier K, Falkai P, et al: Fear is only as deep as the mind allows: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on the regulation of negative affect. Neuroimage 2011; 58:275–285
31.
Burghy CA, Stodola DE, Ruttle PL, et al: Developmental pathways to amygdala-prefrontal function and internalizing symptoms in adolescence. Nat Neurosci 2012; 15:1736–1741
32.
Fan Y, Herrera-Melendez AL, Pestke K, et al: Early life stress modulates amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity: implications for oxytocin effects. Hum Brain Mapp 2014; 35:5328–5339
33.
Gee DG, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, et al: Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110:15638–15643
34.
Grant MM, Wood K, Sreenivasan K, et al: Influence of early life stress on intra- and extra-amygdaloid causal connectivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015; 40:1782–1793
35.
Luking KR, Repovs G, Belden AC, et al: Functional connectivity of the amygdala in early-childhood-onset depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011; 50:1027–1041
36.
Veer IM, Beckmann CF, van Tol MJ, et al: Whole brain resting-state analysis reveals decreased functional connectivity in major depression. Front Syst Neurosci 2010; 4:4
37.
Davey CG, Whittle S, Harrison BJ, et al: Functional brain-imaging correlates of negative affectivity and the onset of first-episode depression. Psychol Med 2015; 45:1001–1009
38.
Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, et al: Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197:378–385
39.
Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund PA, et al: Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication, I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:113–123
40.
Javanbakht A, King AP, Evans GW, et al: Childhood poverty predicts adult amygdala and frontal activity and connectivity in response to emotional faces. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:154
41.
McLoyd VC: Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. Am Psychol 1998; 53:185–204
42.
Egger H, Ascher B, Angold A: Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA): Version 1.1. Durham, NC, Center for Developmental Epidemiology, Duke University Medical Center, 1999
43.
Luby JL, Si X, Belden AC, et al: Preschool depression: homotypic continuity and course over 24 months. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:897–905
44.
Buckner RL, Head D, Parker J, et al: A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage 2004; 23:724–738
45.
Ojemann JG, Akbudak E, Snyder AZ, et al: Anatomic localization and quantitative analysis of gradient refocused echo-planar fMRI susceptibility artifacts. Neuroimage 1997; 6:156–167
46.
Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al: Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 2002; 33:341–355
47.
Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, et al: Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14:11–22
48.
Ollinger JM, Corbetta M, Shulman GL: Separating processes within a trial in event-related functional MRI. Neuroimage 2001; 13:218–229
49.
Pagliaccio D, Luby JL, Bogdan R, et al: Amygdala functional connectivity, HPA axis genetic variation, and life stress in children and relations to anxiety and emotion regulation. J Abnorm Psychol 2015; 124:817–833
50.
Preacher KJ, Hayes AF: Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods 2008; 40:879–891
51.
Hayes AF: Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, Guilford, 2013
52.
Noble KG, Houston SM, Brito NH, et al: Family income, parental education, and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat Neurosci 2015; 18:773–778
53.
Pagliaccio D, Luby JL, Bogdan R, et al: Stress-system genes and life stress predict cortisol levels and amygdala and hippocampal volumes in children. Neuropsychopharmacology 2014; 39:1245–1253
54.
Bergé D, Carmona S, Salgado P, et al: Limbic activity in antipsychotic naive first-episode psychotic subjects during facial emotion discrimination. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2014; 264:271–283
55.
Cerullo MA, Eliassen JC, Smith CT, et al: Bipolar I disorder and major depressive disorder show similar brain activation during depression. Bipolar Disord 2014; 16:703–712
56.
Kim GW, Yang JC, Jeong GW: Emotional effect on cognitive control in implicit memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia. Neuroreport 2015; 26:647–655
57.
Kong F, Hu S, Wang X, et al: Neural correlates of the happy life: the amplitude of spontaneous low frequency fluctuations predicts subjective well-being. Neuroimage 2015; 107:136–145
58.
Costello EJ, Compton SN, Keeler G, et al: Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: a natural experiment. JAMA 2003; 290:2023–2029
59.
Ozer EJ, Fernald LC, Manley JG, et al: Effects of a conditional cash transfer program on children’s behavior problems. Pediatrics 2009; 123:e630–e637
60.
Galea S, Ahern J, Nandi A, et al: Urban neighborhood poverty and the incidence of depression in a population-based cohort study. Ann Epidemiol 2007; 17:171–179
61.
Mehta K, Kramer H, Durazo-Arvizu R, et al: Depression in the US population during the time periods surrounding the great recession. J Clin Psychiatry 2015; 76:e499–e504
62.
Ozer EJ, Fernald LC, Weber A, et al: Does alleviating poverty affect mothers’ depressive symptoms? A quasi-experimental investigation of Mexico’s Oportunidades programme. Int J Epidemiol 2011; 40:1565–1576
63.
Simmons LA, Braun B, Charnigo R, et al: Depression and poverty among rural women: a relationship of social causation or social selection? J Rural Health 2008; 24:292–298

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 625 - 634
PubMed: 26771739

History

Received: 5 August 2015
Revision received: 13 October 2015
Revision received: 8 November 2015
Accepted: 20 November 2015
Published online: 15 January 2016
Published in print: June 01, 2016

Authors

Details

Deanna Barch, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
David Pagliaccio, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
Andy Belden, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
Michael P. Harms, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
Michael Gaffrey
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
Chad M. Sylvester, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
Rebecca Tillman, M.S.
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.
Joan Luby, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry; the Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology; the Department of Psychology; and the Program in Neuroscience, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Mo.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Barch ([email protected]).

Funding Information

National Institute of Mental Health10.13039/100000025: MH064769, MH090786
Supported by NIH grants R01 MH064769-06 and R01 MH090786.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share