Skip to main content
Full access
Articles
Published Online: 20 January 2017

Gaze-Contingent Music Reward Therapy for Social Anxiety Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Abstract

Objective:

Patients with social anxiety disorder exhibit increased attentional dwelling on social threats, providing a viable target for therapeutics. This randomized controlled trial examined the efficacy of a novel gaze-contingent music reward therapy for social anxiety disorder designed to reduce attention dwelling on threats.

Method:

Forty patients with social anxiety disorder were randomly assigned to eight sessions of either gaze-contingent music reward therapy, designed to divert patients’ gaze toward neutral stimuli rather than threat stimuli, or to a control condition. Clinician and self-report measures of social anxiety were acquired pretreatment, posttreatment, and at 3-month follow-up. Dwell time on socially threatening faces was assessed during the training sessions and at pre- and posttreatment.

Results:

Gaze-contingent music reward therapy yielded greater reductions of symptoms of social anxiety disorder than the control condition on both clinician-rated and self-reported measures. Therapeutic effects were maintained at follow-up. Gaze-contingent music reward therapy, but not the control condition, also reduced dwell time on threat, which partially mediated clinical effects. Finally, gaze-contingent music reward therapy, but not the control condition, also altered dwell time on socially threatening faces not used in training, reflecting near-transfer training generalization.

Conclusions:

This is the first randomized controlled trial to examine a gaze-contingent intervention in social anxiety disorder. The results demonstrate target engagement and clinical effects. This study sets the stage for larger randomized controlled trials and testing in other emotional disorders.
Attention bias modification refers to a novel group of treatments grounded in cognitive neuroscience targeting aberrant threat-related attention patterns in anxiety disorders (1, 2). Accumulating evidence finds moderate effects of reaction-time-based attention bias modification protocols for anxiety disorders (3). However, efficacy remains inconsistent across studies, possibly from a failure of some reaction-time-based protocols to effectively engage aberrant attentional processes (4, 5). Reaction-time measurements of attention bias typically possess poor psychometrics and capture only indirect effects of attention (611). These limitations exist because reaction-time biases reflect behaviors occurring at the end of a complex process, which unfolds dynamically from the point of threat detection (12, 13). Thus, reaction-time-based training fails to shape key aspects of attention allocation that are naturally deployed (1, 1316). Finally, reaction-time-based attention bias modification protocols utilize many monotonous trials, which are experienced by some patients as tedious, potentially reducing treatment engagement (17). Eye-tracking measures may provide better therapeutic targets (12, 13, 17). Socially anxious individuals tend to observe threats for longer time periods than nonanxious individuals (15, 1820), a pattern that manifests stably over time (15), and thus can provide a viable target for treatment.
The present randomized control trial tests the efficacy and associated mechanism of a novel eye-tracking-based attention bias modification treatment for social anxiety disorder, targeting enhanced dwelling time on socially threatening faces in social anxiety disorder (15). Patients were randomly assigned either to gaze-contingent music reward therapy (the experimental group), designed to divert attention toward neutral over threatening faces, or to a control condition with no feedback on viewing patterns (the control group). This randomized controlled trial tests the hypothesis that compared with those in the control group, those receiving gaze-contingent music reward therapy will generate more robust, lasting reductions in social anxiety disorder symptoms monitored over a 4-month period and experience greater reduction in time spent dwelling on threat. We also hypothesized that reductions in dwell time on threat will partially mediate the association between treatment group and reductions in social anxiety disorder symptoms.

Method

Participants

Progress through the study stages is summarized in the CONSORT diagram in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article. Participants were 40 treatment-seeking patients (mean age=33.83 years, SD=10.80; 20 males). Inclusion criteria were as follows: a primary diagnosis of social anxiety disorder (i.e., social anxiety disorder being the main source of behavioral and emotional dysfunction); 18–60 years of age; and normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Exclusion criteria were as follows: any history or present diagnosis of psychosis; a high risk for violence to self or others; a present diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, bipolar disorder, or tic disorder; epilepsy or brain injury; use of medication other than selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs); any concurrent psychotherapy; drug or alcohol misuse; and eye-tracking calibration difficulties.
A number of participants had comorbidities: 11 had had a mild depressive episode (seven in the experimental group), nine had dysthymia (four in the experimental group), 16 had generalized anxiety disorder (seven in the experimental group), six had panic disorder (two in the experimental group), and four had agoraphobia (two in the experimental group). Nine participants (five in the experimental group) were taking a stable dosage of SSRIs that had begun at least 3 months prior to the beginning of the study. SSRI dosage was kept stable throughout the study. Participants were randomly assigned to either gaze-contingent music reward therapy (N=20) or to the control condition (N=20). The two groups did not significantly differ in age, education, and symptom severity at baseline, and they had the same male-to-female ratio (50% male). All participants were naive to eye-tracking procedures. All participants continued participation until the end of treatment, and three from the control group declined participation in the follow-up. The study was approved by the local institutional review board, and participants provided written informed consent.

Clinical Status

Potential participants who contacted our clinic in search of treatment were screened over the telephone for social anxiety symptoms using the Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN) (21). Those with SPIN scores ≥30 (indicating probable social anxiety disorder) were invited for a full clinical assessment. Clinical interviews were conducted by an independent evaluator, a clinical psychologist trained to 85% reliability with a senior psychologist. The independent evaluator was blind to group assignment and all aspects of treatment. Weekly sessions were conducted to monitor and review diagnostic decisions.
Primary and comorbid diagnoses were ascertained using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (22) and were further established using the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS) (23), with a cutoff score ≥50 as an inclusion criterion. This LSAS cutoff score represents an optimal balance between specificity and sensitivity for diagnosis of social anxiety disorder (24, 25).
The primary outcome was severity of social anxiety measured using the total score of the clinician-administered LSAS (23). Cronbach’s alpha in our sample was 0.86 at pretreatment, 0.87 at posttreatment, and 0.86 at follow-up. The secondary outcome was self-reported social anxiety using the SPIN total score (21). Cronbach’s alpha in this sample was 0.86 at pretreatment, 0.90 at posttreatment, and 0.89 at follow-up.

Attention Allocation to Threat: Gaze-Tracking Assessment

Attention allocation to threat was assessed with an established eye-tracking task (15) using a remote high-speed eye tracker (SensoMotoric Instruments, Teltow, Germany). Each trial presented a 4×4 matrix of 16 faces (26), half with disgusted and half with neutral facial expressions (Figure 1). Each face appeared randomly at any position on the matrix. The following parameters were followed: each actor appeared only once in a matrix; each matrix contained eight male and eight female faces; half of the faces showed a disgusted expression, and half showed a neutral expression; and the four inner faces were always two disgusted and two neutral expressions.
FIGURE 1. An Example of a Single Matrix of Disgusted and Neutral Faces in a Study of Gaze-Contingent Music Reward Therapy for Social Anxiety Disordera
a The eight disgusted faces comprise the threatening area of interest, while the eight neutral faces comprise the neutral area of interest.
Each trial began with a fixation cross shown until a fixation of 1,000 ms was recorded, verifying that a trial began only when a participant’s gaze was fixated at the center of the matrix. Each matrix was presented for 6,000 ms, followed by an intertrial interval of 2,000 ms until the next fixation cross appeared. Participants were instructed to look freely at each matrix in any way they chose until it disappeared. For further details of the gaze-tracking assessment, see the online data supplement. Cronbach’s alpha in this sample was 0.94 at pretreatment (for the full sample) and was 0.95 and 0.93 at posttreatment for the experimental and control groups, respectively.

Gaze-Contingent Music Reward Therapy and Control Groups

The treatment task was a modified version of the assessment task described above designed to divert patients’ attention toward neutral faces and away from threatening faces presented in the matrices. At the beginning of each treatment session, patients selected a 12-minute music track they wanted to listen to during the session. Music tracks were selected from an extensive menu reflecting the most popular musicians according to published rating charts. Each treatment session began with eye-tracking calibration followed by 30 face matrices, shown for 24 seconds each, with no intertrial intervals. Each face appeared 15 times per session. Patients in the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group heard their selected music play only when fixating on one of the neutral faces in a matrix (the neutral area of interest). When fixating on one of the disgusted faces (the threat area of interest), the music stopped. Patients in the control group heard the music of their choice throughout the session without interruptions (i.e., the music was noncontingent upon their gaze). The treatment tasks ran E-Prime, version 2 (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh).

Apparatus and Eye-Tracking Measures

Gaze data were recorded using a RED500 system and were analyzed with BeGaze software (SensoMotoric Instruments, Teltow, Germany). Operating distance to the eye-tracking monitor was 70 cm. The stimuli were presented on a 22-inch Dell P2213 monitor (screen resolution 1680×1050). The sampling rate was 500 Hz. For each matrix, two areas of interest were defined: the eight faces displaying an expression of disgust (the threatening area of interest), and the eight faces displaying a neutral expression (the neutral area of interest). Total dwell time in milliseconds for each area of interest in each matrix was recorded, and the proportion of dwell time on the threatening area of interest relative to the total dwell time on both areas of interest in each matrix was calculated. This calculation reflected the proportion of time that the gaze was on threatening stimuli out of the total time the faces on each matrix were observed. An overall index of the average percentage of time spent dwelling on threatening stimuli was computed across the presented matrices (60 matrices in the assessment task, and 30 in the training task).

General Procedure

Study design was a parallel-group randomized controlled trial, with two groups (the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group and the control group) and three assessment points (pretreatment, posttreatment, and 3-month follow-up). Participants were clinically assessed at the three time points using structured clinician-rated measures and self-report questionnaires. Attention allocation patterns were assessed at pre- and posttreatment and across the training sessions. Data collection was carried out between January 2015 and July 2016.
Consenting participants underwent the clinical assessment at pretreatment. They were informed that the purpose of the study was to evaluate the efficacy of a novel eye-tracking-based treatment for social anxiety disorder. Those meeting the inclusion criteria completed the attention allocation assessment task in a subsequent session the following week. Treatment consisted of eight 20-minute sessions, twice a week across 4 weeks. Posttreatment assessment was conducted 1 week after the last training session and included the same measures and tasks used in the pretreatment assessment. Participants were clinically reassessed again at a 3-month follow-up. At this point, participants in the control group were given the opportunity to receive gaze-contingent music reward therapy.

Data Analysis

Independent sample t tests were used to compare between-group descriptive characteristics at pretreatment. Treatment effects were tested using the generalized estimating equations approach (27, 28), as recommended for randomized controlled trials (29). The generalized estimating equations approach accounts for correlated repeated-measures analysis and accommodates missing data under the missing-at-random assumption by computing estimated marginal means. Thus, this approach serves as an intention-to-treat analysis strategy, which includes data from all randomized participants who provided at least one data point. To represent within-subject dependencies in the models, we specified an unstructured correlation matrix. Overall effects of the experimental intervention relative to the control condition on clinician-rated (LSAS total score, as well as the fear and the avoidance subscale scores) and self-reported (SPIN score) social anxiety symptoms were estimated using models containing the main effects of group (the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group and the control group), time (pretreatment, posttreatment, follow-up), and their interaction. We first applied a full factorial model across the three time points. Follow-up analyses modeled symptom change from pre- to posttreatment. Long-term maintenance effects modeled symptom change from posttreatment to follow-up. Time-by-group interaction terms were used to test the treatment effect hypothesis of greater decrease in social anxiety disorder symptoms over time for the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group relative to the control group. Chi-square tests were used to compare groups on clinically significant change.
Group effects on attention allocation were analyzed using repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the percentage of dwell time on threat during the treatment task in sessions one through eight. The eight sessions served as a within-subject factor, and treatment group (the experimental and control groups) served as a between-subjects variable. An independent sample t test was also used to compare the two groups on the amount of reduction in the percentage of dwell time on threat from session one to session eight, calculated as the percentage of dwell time on threat in session one minus the same in session eight. To test for possible group differences in the percentage of dwell time on threat at pretreatment, we compared group performance using independent sample t tests on the pretreatment assessment task and on the first five matrices of session one.
To examine generalization of training through near transfer to novel faces, repeated-measures ANOVA modeled the percentage of dwell time on threat in the assessment task. Time (pretreatment and posttreatment) served as a within-subject factor, and treatment group (the experimental and control groups) served as a between-subjects variable. Follow-up analyses included separate contrasts for the pre- and posttreatment assessments. All statistical tests were two-sided, using alpha ≤0.05. Effect sizes are reported using η2p and Cohen’s d when appropriate.
Finally, to assess whether reduction in dwell time on threat (the time in session eight minus the time in session one) served as a mediator of treatment effects as measured by the LSAS and the SPIN, we applied a mediation analysis procedure (30), model 4, using the PROCESS macro in SPSS (SPSS, Chicago). This procedure estimates indirect effects in both unmoderated and moderated mediation models (31), providing bootstrap confidence intervals for the mediated effects. We applied 1,000 bootstrap samples. The mediator variables are considered significant if the lower and upper bounds of the confidence interval do not include zero (31).

Results

Preliminary Analyses

The experimental and control groups did not differ significantly in age, education, self- and clinician-reported social anxiety disorder symptoms, and percentage of dwell time on socially threatening faces at pretreatment (Table 1). Additional analyses compared the groups on all dependent variables between facial sets A and B, but there were no significant differences between the two sets (see the data supplement for further description of the study process that used two sets of faces as stimuli in sets A and B). Therefore, in all relevant analyses the two sets were collapsed.
TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics, Social Anxiety Symptoms, and Attention Allocation Patterns by Group at Pretreatment, Posttreatment, and Follow-Up in a Study of Gaze-Contingent Music Reward Therapy for Social Anxiety Disordera
VariableGaze-Contingent Music Reward Therapy GroupControl Group
 MeanSDMeanSD
Age (years)33.909.9133.7711.89
Years of education14.423.4213.802.54
LSAS score at pretreatment78.0513.7474.1520.11
LSAS score at posttreatment49.6016.7866.7026.45
LSAS score at follow-up50.2515.6458.8427.35
SPIN score at pretreatment47.757.2844.3010.04
SPIN score at posttreatment31.707.1740.5012.22
SPIN score at follow-up27.008.2233.9014.32
Dwell time (pretreatment %)48.547.7546.869.34
Dwell time (posttreatment %)37.9011.5346.324.83
a
Dwell time refers to the percentage of time spent dwelling on the threatening area of interest during the assessment task. LSAS=Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; SPIN=Social Phobia Inventory.

Change in Social Anxiety Symptoms Following Treatment: Primary Outcome (LSAS)

Figure 2A presents results of the generalized estimating equations model for the LSAS scores. A main effect of time (Wald χ2=75.92, p<0.0001) was subsumed under a significant time-by-group interaction effect (Wald χ2=18.13, p<0.0001). Follow-up analyses indicated significant reduction in LSAS scores from pre- to posttreatment in both groups (for the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group, p<0.0001, d=1.86; and for the control group, p<0.05, d=1.06). However, reduction in social anxiety disorder severity was significantly greater in the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group than in the control group (Wald χ2=18.04, p<0.0001). This difference reflected a larger change in the LSAS score (mean=21 points) for those in the experimental group relative to those in the control group (d=1.31). Modeling change in symptoms from posttreatment to 3-month follow-up revealed a significant difference in symptom reduction between groups (Wald χ2=4.77, p<0.03), with no significant change in symptoms in the experimental group, while in the control group, symptoms continued to decrease (p=0.021). This difference reflected a larger change in the LSAS score (mean=8.51 points) for the control group relative to the experimental group (d=1.09). A similar result pattern was observed for the fear and the avoidance LSAS subscales when analyzed separately (see the online data supplement).
FIGURE 2. Social Anxiety Scores by Group and by Time in a Study of Gaze-Contingent Music Reward Therapy (GC-MRT) for Social Anxiety Disordera
a Social anxiety scores are shown by group (the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group, N=20; and the control group, N=20) and by time (pretreatment, posttreatment, and 3-month follow-up). The social anxiety scores are measured using the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS), as shown in panel A, and using the Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN), as shown in panel B.

Clinically Significant Change

We compared the number of participants who achieved clinically significant change (32) from pre- to posttreatment between the two treatment groups. Determination of clinically significant change reflected the test-retest reliability data from Baker et al. (33) and pretreatment LSAS scores from the authors’ data pool in previous clinical trials (N=87 subjects). A posttreatment cutoff score of 44.09 was determined as reflecting clinically significant change. Fourteen patients (70%) in the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group achieved clinically significant change posttreatment, compared with six patients (30%) in the control group (χ2=6.40, p=0.01). Analysis of clinically significant change at follow-up showed that 15 patients (75%) in the experimental group achieved clinically significant change, compared with six patients (30%) in the control group (χ2=8.12, p=0.004).

Change in Social Anxiety Symptoms Following Treatment: Secondary Outcome (SPIN)

Figure 2B presents results from the generalized estimating equations model for self-reported SPIN scores. This analysis revealed a main effect of time (Wald χ2=81.82, p<0.0001) that was subsumed under a group-by-time interaction (Wald χ2=16.13, p<0.0001). Follow-up analyses indicated a significant decline in SPIN scores from pre- to posttreatment in the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group (p<0.0001, d=1.76) but not in the control group. Reduction in social anxiety disorder severity from pre- to posttreatment was significantly larger in the experimental group relative to the control group (Wald χ2=16.12, p<0.0001). This difference reflected a larger change in the SPIN score (mean=12.29 points) for those in the experimental group relative to those in the control group (d=1.24). For change in symptoms from posttreatment to follow-up, only a main effect of time emerged (Wald χ2=23.17, p<0.0001), with both groups showing further improvement (in the experimental group, p<0.0001, d=1.23; and in the control group, p<0.001, d=1.09). The time-by-group interaction was nonsignificant.

Treatment-Related Change in Dwell Time on Threat

The percentage of dwell time on threat by group and by session is shown in Figure 3A. A comparable performance in the experimental group (mean=41.38, SD=11.77) and in the control group (mean=44.92, SD=8.19) was observed for average dwell time on threat in the first five matrices of session one (t=1.10, df=38, p=0.27). Analyses across sessions revealed significant main effects of group (F=20.16, df=1, 38, p<0.001, η2p=0.35) and session (F=3.42, df=7, 266, p<0.001, η2p=0.08) that were subsumed under a significant group-by-session interaction (F=2.36, df=7, 266, p=0.02, η2p=0.05). The percentage of dwell time on a threat declined by 9% from session one to session eight in the experimental group, compared with a decline of 0.1% in the control group (t=2.70, df=38, p=0.01, d=0.86).
FIGURE 3. Percentage of Dwell Time on a Threat by Group and by Time in a Study of Gaze-Contingent Music Reward Therapy (GC-MRT) for Social Anxiety Disordera
a The percentage of dwell time on a threat by group (the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group, N=20; and the control group, N=20) is shown by treatment session (session one through session eight) in panel A and by assessment session (pretreatment and posttreatment) in panel B. Error bars indicate standard error.

Near Transfer: Treatment Effects on Novel Faces

The percentage of dwell time on threat by group (the experimental and control groups) and by time (pre- and posttreatment) is shown in Figure 3B. A main effect of time (F=7.52, df=1, 34, p<0.01, η2p=0.18) was qualified by a significant group-by-time interaction (F=4.63, df=1, 34, p=0.04, η2p=0.12). Follow-up contrasts revealed no significant group differences at pretreatment (F=0.05, df=1, 34, p=0.82, η2p< 0.01). At posttreatment, a significantly lower percentage of time spent dwelling on threat was noted in the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group relative to the control group (F=7.84, df=1, 34, p<0.01, η2p=0.19), with the control group (mean=46.32, SD=4.83) allocating more attention to threat compared with the experimental group (mean=37.90, SD=11.53).

Mediation Analyses

Reduction in the percentage of dwell time on threat from session one to session eight partially mediated the association between group and reduction in clinician-reported LSAS scores from pre- to posttreatment. The effect of group on reduction in dwell time on threat was significant (b=9.37, SE=3.49, p<0.05, 95% CI=0.02–0.16). The effect of reduction in threat dwell time on LSAS score reduction was not significant (b=34.02, SE=23.25, p=0.15), but the indirect effect of group on the reduction in the LSAS scores via the reduction in dwell time on threat from session one to session eight was significant (b=3.16, SE=1.87, p<0.05, 95% CI=0.27–7.98), reflecting a partial mediation effect. A similar analysis on self-reported SPIN scores from pre- to posttreatment did not reveal mediation via reduction in threat dwell time. The effect of reduction in threat dwell time on SPIN score reduction was not significant (b=10.31, SE=14.64, p=0.15), and although the direct effect of group on reduction of the SPIN scores was significant (b=11.29, SE=3.43, p<0.05, 95% CI=4.34–18.25), the indirect effect via reduction in dwell time on threat was not (b=0.96, SE=1.47, p>0.05).

Discussion

This randomized controlled trial examined the efficacy of a novel gaze-contingent music reward therapy for patients with social anxiety disorder. To our knowledge, it is the first study to apply gaze-contingent reward feedback therapy in a clinically anxious population. Results indicate that gaze-contingent music reward therapy was significantly more effective than a control condition in reducing both clinician-rated and self-reported social anxiety disorder symptoms posttreatment. Moreover, overall symptom reduction reflected reductions in experienced social fear and avoidance behaviors, as reflected in the LSAS subscales. The effects of gaze-contingent music reward therapy were maintained at 3-month follow-up, although patients in the control condition did tend to improve during this period. Findings also indicate effective target engagement in the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group, a near transfer of this target engagement effect, and partial mediation of clinical effects by effects on target engagement. However, this partial mediation was not significant for self-reported social anxiety.
While gaze-contingent music reward therapy is based on the principles of attention bias modification, this novel treatment involves several unique features. First, training targets a behavior (dwell time on threat in a free-viewing paradigm, with acceptable psychometrics [15]) that has been missing from most reaction-time-based attention bias modifications (6, 7, 911). Second, unlike most forms of attention bias modification, the stimulus array in gaze-contingent music reward therapy contains 16 faces per matrix, thereby increasing the requirement for continuous allocation of attention away from negative stimuli. This design also increases ecological validity relative to attention bias modification tasks containing smaller stimulus arrays (8, 13, 17). Third, gaze-contingent music reward therapy targets eye gaze, which reflects dynamic allocation of attention to stimuli, unlike most forms of attention bias modification, which target reaction-time-based biases, a less dynamic measure occurring at the end of complex information processing progressions (12, 13). Finally, the use of music in gaze-contingent music reward therapy and in a control situation may increase patients’ engagement. Of note, the present study maintained 100% of patients at posttreatment, which is unusual. This may address concerns expressed about poor engagement in other forms of attention bias modification (8, 17).
The findings of change in dwell time on threat from pre- to posttreatment are in accord with previous proof-of-principle studies that demonstrated the potential of gaze-contingent attention bias modification procedures in modifying attentional processes (13, 17). The present results extend those of previous studies by examining a clinical population of treatment-seeking patients with social anxiety disorder as opposed to examining dysphoric mood reactivity among samples of nonselected students (13, 17).
Although results indicated a lower LSAS score in the experimental group relative to the control group at posttreatment, there was also a significant reduction in symptoms among those in the control group at posttreatment and at follow-up. This result might reflect nonspecific placebo effects, as the two treatment situations were equivalent with regard to number of sessions, session length, intervention modality, and the amount and nature of interaction with research staff (34). Previous research has documented positive clinical effects of well-designed placebo conditions in clinical trials (35). Alternatively, symptom reduction in the control group could be related to exposure to threatening faces in the context of positive valence induced by the music reward (36). Previous fear conditioning, fear extinction, and fear exposure therapy research in anxiety has raised the possibility that reducing the negative valence of a feared stimulus, and increasing positive affect prior to and during exposure, may increase the beneficial outcome of exposure in anxiety (36, 37). Future research could examine this possibility by using a different control condition (e.g., yoked music feedback between participants of the different groups, thus controlling for the difference between interrupted and continuous music reward applied in the present study). Alternatively, it is possible to test the effects of music reward without exposure to threat by applying matrices with neutral faces only.
While this study indicates promising efficacy of gaze-contingent music reward therapy, some general limitations of this method deserve notice. First, the use of eye tracking restricts treatment for those who have eye-tracking calibration difficulties. In the present sample, calibration was not achieved in three patients, and others were not invited because of eyesight issues that would have prevented successful calibration. Second, although eye-tracking technology is advancing rapidly, high-quality eye-tracking systems are still quite costly, which may restrict availability in clinics. In a related vein, future studies could directly compare the advantages and cost-effectiveness of attention bias modifications based on eye tracking with those based on traditional reaction time (17), as well as with those of cognitive-behavioral therapy.
There are also limitations to note with the present study overall. First, attention allocation patterns were not measured at follow-up. Therefore, it remains unclear whether the observed reductions in dwell time on threat at posttreatment were sustained. Second, we did not examine the possible influence of explicit knowledge of the training rule by patients in the gaze-contingent music reward therapy group. Such explicit knowledge might have affected treatment outcomes among participants in this group. Future research could examine this issue by explicitly informing patients of the embedded music contingency and test the effect of such explicit knowledge on treatment outcome (3840). Finally, the present study included only threatening and neutral facial expressions in assessment and training. Future studies could use positive as well as other negative facial expressions to further elucidate the specificity of emotion expression to therapeutic effects.
In conclusion, this randomized controlled trial is the first to examine a newly developed attention bias modification based on gaze-contingent feedback targeting a previously identified bias in social anxiety disorder: increased dwell time on socially threatening stimuli (15). Gaze-contingent music reward therapy, comprising eight 12-minute sessions of gaze-contingent music reward feedback, was able to successfully rectify this biased gaze process. Moreover, this therapy achieved reduction in dwell time on threat and led to a significant reduction in social anxiety disorder symptoms following treatment. Additional research is needed to confirm these findings and to possibly extend them to other anxiety and affective disorders.

Footnotes

Supported in part by the United States—Israel Binational Science Foundation (grant number 2013349). The funding agency had no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Clinicaltrials.gov identifier: NCT02590770.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2016.16080894.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Bar-Haim Y: Research review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2010; 51:859–870
2.
MacLeod C, Mathews A: Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2012; 8:189–217
3.
Linetzky M, Pergamin-Hight L, Pine DS, et al: Quantitative evaluation of the clinical efficacy of attention bias modification treatment for anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety 2015; 32:383–391
4.
Heeren A, Lievens L, Philippot P: How does attention training work in social phobia: disengagement from threat or re-engagement to non-threat? J Anxiety Disord 2011; 25:1108–1115
5.
Heeren A, Mogoaşe C, Philippot P, et al: Attention bias modification for social anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2015; 40:76–90
6.
Waechter S, Stolz JA: Trait anxiety, state anxiety, and attentional bias to threat: assessing the psychometric properties of response time measures. Cognit Ther Res 2015; 39:441–458
7.
Van Bockstaele B, Verschuere B, Tibboel H, et al: A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychol Bull 2014; 140:682–721
8.
Mogoaşe C, David D, Koster EHW: Clinical efficacy of attentional bias modification procedures: an updated meta-analysis. J Clin Psychol 2014; 70:1133–1157
9.
Schmukle SC: Unreliability of the dot probe task. Eur J Pers 2005; 19:595–605
10.
Staugaard SR: Reliability of two versions of the dot-probe task using photographic faces. Psychol Sci Q 2009; 51:339–350
11.
Waechter S, Nelson AL, Wright C, et al: Measuring attentional bias to threat: reliability of dot probe and eye movement indices. Cognit Ther Res 2014; 38:313–333
12.
Armstrong T, Olatunji BO: Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin Psychol Rev 2012; 32:704–723
13.
Price RB, Greven IM, Siegle GJ, et al: A novel attention training paradigm based on operant conditioning of eye gaze: preliminary findings. Emotion 2016; 16:110–116
14.
Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, et al: Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 2007; 133:1–24
15.
Lazarov A, Abend R, Bar-Haim Y: Social anxiety is related to increased dwell time on socially threatening faces. J Affect Disord 2016; 193:282–288
16.
Shechner T, Jarcho JM, Britton JC, et al: Attention bias of anxious youth during extended exposure of emotional face pairs: an eye-tracking study. Depress Anxiety 2013; 30:14–21
17.
Ferrari GRA, Möbius M, van Opdorp A, et al: Can’t look away: an eye-tracking based attentional disengagement training for depression. Cognit Ther Res 2016; 40:672–686
18.
Buckner JD, Maner JK, Schmidt NB: Difficulty disengaging attention from social threat in social anxiety. Cognit Ther Res 2010; 34:99–105
19.
Schofield CA, Johnson AL, Inhoff AW, et al: Social anxiety and difficulty disengaging threat: evidence from eye-tracking. Cogn Emotion 2012; 26:300–311
20.
Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Alpers GW, et al: Is eye to eye contact really threatening and avoided in social anxiety? An eye-tracking and psychophysiology study. J Anxiety Disord 2009; 23:93–103
21.
Connor KM, Davidson JRT, Churchill LE, et al: Psychometric properties of the Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN): new self-rating scale. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 176:379–386
22.
Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, et al: The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59(suppl 20):22–33
23.
Liebowitz MR: Social phobia. Mod Probl Pharmacopsychiatry 1987; 22:141–173
24.
Mennin DS, Fresco DM, Heimberg RG, et al: Screening for social anxiety disorder in the clinical setting: using the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. J Anxiety Disord 2002; 16:661–673
25.
Taylor CT, Bomyea J, Amir N: Attentional bias away from positive social information mediates the link between social anxiety and anxiety vulnerability to a social stressor. J Anxiety Disord 2010; 24:403–408
26.
Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A: The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF): CD-ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, 1998
27.
Zeger SL, Liang KY: Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics 1986; 42:121–130
28.
Zeger SL, Liang KY, Albert PS: Models for longitudinal data: a generalized estimating equation approach. Biometrics 1988; 44:1049–1060
29.
Vens M, Ziegler A: Generalized estimating equations and regression diagnostics for longitudinal controlled clinical trials: a case study. Comput Stat Data Anal 2012; 56:1231–1242
30.
Hayes AF, Preacher KJ: Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. Br J Math Stat Psychol 2014; 67:451–470
31.
Hayes AF: Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, Guilford, 2013
32.
Jacobson NS, Truax P: Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J Consult Clin Psychol 1991; 59:12–19
33.
Baker SL, Heinrichs N, Kim HJ, et al: The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale as a self-report instrument: a preliminary psychometric analysis. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40:701–715
34.
Baskin TW, Tierney SC, Minami T, et al: Establishing specificity in psychotherapy: a meta-analysis of structural equivalence of placebo controls. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003; 71:973–979
35.
Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC: Placebo treatment versus no treatment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2003
36.
Dour HJ, Brown LA, Craske MG: Positive valence reduces susceptibility to return of fear and enhances approach behavior. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2016; 50:277–282
37.
Gathercole SE, Alloway TP, Kirkwood HJ, et al: Attentional and executive function behaviors in children with poor working memory. Learn Individ Differ 2008; 18:214–223
38.
Grafton B, Mackintosh B, Vujic T, et al: When ignorance is bliss: explicit instruction and the efficacy of CBM-A for anxiety. Cognit Ther Res 2014; 38:172–188
39.
Krebs G, Hirsch CR, Mathews A: The effect of attention modification with explicit vs. minimal instructions on worry. Behav Res Ther 2010; 48:251–256
40.
Nishiguchi Y, Takano K, Tanno Y: Explicitly guided attentional bias modification promotes attentional disengagement from negative stimuli. Emotion 2015; 15:731–741

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 649 - 656
PubMed: 28103714

History

Received: 10 August 2016
Revision received: 27 October 2016
Accepted: 7 November 2016
Published online: 20 January 2017
Published in print: July 01, 2017

Keywords

  1. Cognitive Therapy
  2. Phobic Disorders
  3. Social Anxiety
  4. Eye Tracking
  5. Gaze-Contingency
  6. Attention Bias Modification
  7. Attention Allocation

Authors

Details

Amit Lazarov, Ph.D. [email protected]
From the School of Psychological Sciences and the Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel; and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, Md.
Daniel S. Pine, M.D.
From the School of Psychological Sciences and the Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel; and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, Md.
Yair Bar-Haim, Ph.D.
From the School of Psychological Sciences and the Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel; and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, Md.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Lazarov ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

United States — Israel Binational Science Foundation: grant number 2013349

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share