Skip to main content
Full access
Clinical Case Conference
Published Online: 1 September 2019

Clinicians and Cognitive Bias: A Case of Frontotemporal Dementia Misdiagnosed as Conversion Disorder

“Ms. A,” a 47-year-old woman with a 2.5-year history of worsening symptoms of depression, anxiety, impaired speech, bradykinesia, and incoordination, was referred to a tertiary medical center for further evaluation and treatment of conversion disorder complicated by depression and anxiety. Symptom onset followed the suicide of her college-age son, and her symptoms eventually rendered her nearly mute and unable to perform activities of daily living. Previous psychiatric and neurological evaluations had been conducted approximately 6 months before her referral and were inconclusive. Neuropsychological testing revealed abnormalities in memory and speech. These findings were interpreted as effort dependent and labeled as possible conversion disorder (also known as functional neurological disorder). CSF studies were reported to be normal. A brain MRI report described possible cerebral atrophy.
The patient’s medical history included asthma and hyperlipidemia. Her only prior psychiatric history was mild depression approximately 8 years earlier, for which she was treated with several selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Ms. A had been raised by her adoptive mother and her biological father, and there was no history of abuse or trauma. After completing an associate’s degree, she was employed until the presenting illness rendered her disabled. She was married and had two children. Family history of psychiatric illness included suicides in her son and a grandfather.
Ms. A was admitted to the psychiatric hospital for diagnostic clarification and treatment. The working diagnosis on admission was severe major depression with catatonia, although concern was also noted for an underlying neurological illness, and her prior diagnosis of conversion disorder was retained. Trials of benzodiazepines, various antidepressants and antipsychotics, and an acute course of ECT were ineffective. A neurological evaluation was performed concurrently with her psychiatric care, and the patient was noted to be mildly hyperreflexic throughout and to have equivocal Babinski signs bilaterally. Brain MRI, sleep and wake EEG, and serum and CSF autoimmune encephalopathy panels were obtained, and additional exhaustive testing for endocrinologic, immunologic, and infectious etiologies were conducted. The brain MRI was interpreted as normal and unchanged from the prior study. EEG showed some left temporal slowing. CSF studies included slightly elevated levels of protein (50 mg/dL [normal, 0–35 mg/dL]) and neuron-specific enolase (17 ng/mL [normal, <15 ng/mL]). The latter finding can indicate neuronal destruction (1). West Nile virus assays were equivocal. At this point, the assessments did not indicate a specific medical or neurological disorder to explain the patient’s symptoms, and the diagnosis of conversion disorder continued.
After 2 months in hospital, Ms. A was noted on neurological examination to have frontal lobe release signs, specifically the palmomental reflex bilaterally, as well as worsening hyperreflexia. An electromyogram provided evidence of a chronic, active, predominantly axonal neurogenic process affecting the right C8/T1-, L5-, and S1-innervated muscles. A reevaluation of the brain MRI studies suggested that there was in fact evidence of cortical atrophy inappropriate for the patient’s age, which led to reconsideration of an underlying neurodegenerative disorder. Frontotemporal dementia was then considered as the cause for her illness. Genetic testing was performed, but results were not available until after the patient had been discharged from the hospital to a dementia care facility, where she died several weeks later. Genetic testing results later revealed a C9orf72 genetic mutation, consistent with familial frontotemporal dementia.
Every specialty of medicine is concerned with misdiagnosis. Recent developments in behavioral economics offer a helpful perspective on nonrational influences or cognitive biases that intrude on important decision making. The case presented here highlights how cognitive bias can affect both diagnosis and treatment of patients.
In hindsight, the slow, progressive course of the patient’s illness corresponded with what is known about the natural history of frontotemporal dementia associated with a C9orf72 genetic mutation. It is often associated with psychiatric symptoms and very subtle (or even absent) brain atrophy on MRI, which can make the clinical picture confusing (2). Several times during the 3-month hospital course, different approaches and diagnostic considerations might have led to an earlier correct diagnosis but would not have changed the outcome. These missed opportunities include 1) obtaining details of all prior assessments, including detailed reports, not just summaries, of prior neuropsychological testing; 2) pausing and reconsidering diagnoses when any abnormality was noted, including the mild CSF changes along with EEG changes; and 3) acknowledging earlier that the patient’s hospital course was not what one would see with conversion disorder, with or without a comorbid major depressive disorder. In particular, the clinical findings of the case did not present positive evidence of incompatibility between the patient’s symptoms and a recognized medical or neurological condition, which is a necessary criterion in DSM-5 for diagnosing conversion disorder. For example, we misattributed the significance of Ms. A’s speech peculiarities. Her speech while under our care was always sparse in production; had it been decidedly variable across testing occasions, it would have been stronger evidence in favor of a conversion diagnosis (3).
In 1965, Slater (4) estimated a misdiagnosis rate of 33% for “hysteria” (later renamed conversion disorder). His conclusion was to denounce the diagnosis itself, writing that “the diagnosis of ‘hysteria’ is a disguise for ignorance and a fertile source of clinical error. It is, in fact, not only a delusion but also a snare” (4). In contemporary research, the misdiagnosis rate is estimated to be much lower, approximately 4%, which is similar to rates for other psychiatric and neurological disorders (5). However, diagnostic uncertainty and risk of error remain, as evidenced by reports of cases of neurological or medical illness mistakenly called conversion disorder (613) and cases of conversion disorder mistakenly called a medical or neurological illness (14, 15), as well as combinations of psychiatric and neurological disorders in the same patient (16).
Many hypotheses have been proposed to explain conversion disorder misdiagnoses. Among them are the practical clinical challenges of applying the diagnostic criteria for conversion disorder (8, 17), the “subtle interplay” of biological and psychological models of neuropsychiatric illness (6), limited access to advanced imaging (11), current customs of neurological evaluation of functional neurological symptom disorders (9, 18, 19), and a limited neurobiological understanding of functional neurological disorders (7, 17). The contribution of cognitive biases leading to misdiagnosis of conversion disorder has been briefly noted in several reports (7, 11), although without substantial elaboration.

Cognitive Heuristics

In 1974, Kahneman and Tversky (20) described a series of experiments that demonstrated consistent, predictable patterns of how humans simplify complex cognitive tasks such as assessing probabilities and making predictions. They called these patterns “cognitive heuristics,” which are mental shortcuts that allow people to make decisions efficiently. These shortcuts are considered adaptive features of human cognition that are generally highly effective, although they can also lead to systematic errors in judgment, called “biases.” In the medical literature, the study of cognitive heuristics and resultant biases has been a focus of growing interest (21, 22). We review pertinent cognitive heuristics originally described by Kahneman and Tversky—representativeness, availability, adjustment and anchoring—and consider how they may help explain the diagnostic error in this case.

Representativeness

Representativeness refers to estimating probability based on similarity. Errors arise when the idea that “A looks like B” overwhelms other important but more complex considerations, such as estimates of prior probability, sample size, or statistical power. Kahneman and Tversky illustrate this concept by showing how stereotypical personality traits influence predictions about professions (20). For example, when presented with a stereotypical personality sketch of a shy and fastidious person drawn from a sample of more lawyers than librarians, most people will predict that the person is a librarian, in effect prioritizing the professional stereotype over the sample distribution and violating the rules of prior probability. However, when asked the same question in the absence of a personality sketch, we use prior probability estimates correctly. Kahneman and Tversky also observed that people have greater confidence in their decisions as the degree of similarity increases, even when the stereotype itself is known to be “unreliable or outdated” (20).
With regard to this case, representativeness appears to have contributed to diagnostic error because of the historical association between conversion disorder, traumatic stressors, and unexplained neuropsychiatric symptoms. As recently as the 4th edition of DSM, the diagnostic criteria for conversion disorder included a temporal relationship with a traumatic stressor (23). The fact that the patient’s unexplained symptoms appeared after her son’s suicide rendered them representative of an antiquated but common understanding of conversion disorder and likely also conferred a high degree of confidence in the diagnosis despite recent changes to the diagnostic criteria intended to address such errors. DSM-5 removed the criterion requiring association with a traumatic stressor, relegating it to the accompanying text because of concern that it had “not been shown to be either diagnostically reliable or predictive of outcome” (24, 25). DSM-5 also added the new criterion requiring positive evidence of symptoms incompatible with a known medical illness—not just the absence of an alternative diagnosis.

Availability

The availability heuristic refers to assessing the probability of an event on the basis of the ease with which the event can be brought to mind. This can be mediated by frequency (in medicine, known colloquially as “common things are common”) but also by other factors, including the salience of a concept or narrative. For example, Kahneman and Tversky (20) showed subjects lists of names with equal gender distribution but with different ratios of famous men to famous women. When the subjects were asked whether there were more male or female names on the list, the answers corresponded to the gender proportion of famous names: when more famous men made the list, it appeared that there were more male names; when more famous women made the list, it appeared that there were more female names.
In this case, the patient’s symptoms appeared to coincide with the tragic loss of a son to suicide. That particular historical detail was noted in nearly every evaluation during the course of the patient’s care, which stands in contrast to other milestones that were noted with less frequency and which arguably were just as important to the diagnostic evaluation, such as onset of depressive symptoms (5–8 years before Ms. A’s son died) and the point at which speech production became dramatically disturbed (1.5 years after her son died). In retrospect, it appears that the powerful cognitive “availability” of a child’s suicide may have overwhelmed the completeness of the differential diagnosis.

Adjustment and Anchoring

The central tenet of adjustment and anchoring is that the starting point influences the ultimate decision. Different starting points yield different end points, and errors are biased toward the starting point, not the endpoint (20). An example of a cognitive bias stemming from this heuristic is “premature closure,” which refers to when physicians insufficiently consider alternative diagnoses once a provisional diagnosis is made. In the medical literature on diagnostic error, premature closure is among the most common biases leading to misdiagnosis (26).
In this case, the treating physicians were tasked with not only arriving at the right diagnosis but also rejecting an incorrect one. Diagnostic anchoring contributed from the outset in this case, starting with the patient’s initial neuropsychological testing, where speech and memory deficits were misinterpreted as stemming from functional impairment associated with the patient’s son’s death. A pattern of insufficient adjustment ensued, and despite extensive further evaluations, the correct diagnosis remained elusive while the initial incorrect diagnosis remained in place.

Implications For Clinical Practice

To improve the diagnostic accuracy of “functional” neurological disorders, Friedman and LaFrance (19) propose that we elevate functional neurological illness to the same level as other neurological disorders, list them in the differential diagnosis, enumerate pertinent positives and negatives, and rule in the diagnosis with evidence, not just as a diagnosis of exclusion, as was also proposed in DSM-5. In the literature on the science of decision making, this approach is described as cognitive de-biasing. Kahneman and Tversky (20), however, suggest that de-biasing efforts are insufficient, and that we should also incorporate familiarity with cognitive heuristics and biases into our judgments. In other words, the question to ask as we consider our diagnostic decisions is not only “How do we avoid cognitive shortcuts that cause errors?” but also “What cognitive shortcuts might help us prevent errors?”
With those lessons in mind, we propose that recent changes to the diagnosis of conversion disorder be viewed as not just a technical list of diagnostic criteria, but also a “choice architecture” (27) for diagnostic decisions. In a case of suspected conversion disorder, consideration should start with identifying evidence of incompatibility between a specific symptom and recognized neurological or medical conditions (criterion B of the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria).
Other aspects of conversion disorder phenomenology can be helpful in further characterizing specific patterns of psychopathology but also confer an elevated risk of diagnostic error when used as a starting point, in part because of biases stemming from representativeness, anchoring, and availability.

Conclusions

The case presented here—a 47-year-old woman with a 2-year history of progressive, debilitating neuropsychiatric symptoms after her son’s suicide, initially misdiagnosed with conversion disorder but later found to have frontotemporal dementia—highlights the importance of identifying positive evidence of incompatibility between a symptom and a recognized neurological or medical illness when diagnosing conversion disorder. The study of cognitive heuristics may be used to help understand and prevent diagnostic errors by identifying and critically assessing mental shortcuts that can lead to misdiagnosis and delay appropriate treatment decisions.

References

1.
Cooper EH: Neuron-specific enolase. Int J Biol Markers 1994; 9:205–210
2.
Devenney E, Hornberger M, Irish M, et al: Frontotemporal dementia associated with the C9ORF72 mutation: a unique clinical profile. JAMA Neurol 2014; 71:331–339
3.
Duffy JR: Functional speech disorders: clinical manifestations, diagnosis, and management. Handb Clin Neurol 2016; 139:379–388
4.
Slater E: Diagnosis of hysteria. BMJ 1965; 1:1395–1399
5.
Stone J, Smyth R, Carson A, et al: Systematic review of misdiagnosis of conversion symptoms and “hysteria”. BMJ 2005; 331:989
6.
Philbrick KL, Rummans TA, Duffy JR, et al: Primary progressive aphasia: an uncommon masquerader of psychiatric disorders. Psychosomatics 1994; 35:138–141
7.
Solvason HB, Harris B, Zeifert P, et al: Psychological versus biological clinical interpretation: a patient with prion disease. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:528–537
8.
Jones JB, Barklage NE: Conversion disorder: camouflage for brain lesions in two cases. Arch Intern Med 1990; 150:1343–1345
9.
van Meerkerk-Aanen PJ, de Vroege L, Khasho D, et al: La belle indifférence revisited: a case report on progressive supranuclear palsy misdiagnosed as conversion disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2017; 13:2057–2067
10.
Cathebras P, Koenig M: Spinal cord astrocytoma mistaken for conversion disorder. Psychosomatics 2005; 46:187–188
11.
Glick TH, Workman TP, Gaufberg SV: Suspected conversion disorder: foreseeable risks and avoidable errors. Acad Emerg Med 2000; 7:1272–1277
12.
Hakala M, Rinne J, Karlsson H: Onset of neuropsychiatric symptoms after psychological trauma may result in erroneous diagnostic bias. BMJ Case Rep 2010; 2010:bcr0820092166
13.
Li W, Brandon O, Smith DV, et al: A conversion disorder or a stroke? A proximal basilar artery thrombosis induced “locked-in” syndrome in a young Caucasian woman. BMJ Case Rep 2013; 2013:bcr2012007875
14.
Stone J, Reuber M, Carson A: Functional symptoms in neurology: mimics and chameleons. Pract Neurol 2013; 13:104–113
15.
Solomon AJ, Bourdette DN, Cross AH, et al: The contemporary spectrum of multiple sclerosis misdiagnosis: a multicenter study. Neurology 2016; 87:1393–1399
16.
Pareés I, Saifee TA, Kojovic M, et al: Functional (psychogenic) symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2013; 28:1622–1627
17.
Nicholson TRJ, Stone J, Kanaan RAA: Conversion disorder: a problematic diagnosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011; 82:1267–1273
18.
Hallett M: Psychogenic movement disorders: a crisis for neurology. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2006; 6:269–271
19.
Friedman JH, LaFrance WC Jr: Psychogenic disorders: the need to speak plainly. Arch Neurol 2010; 67:753–755
20.
Tversky A, Kahneman D: Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 1974; 185:1124–1131
21.
Avorn J: The psychology of clinical decision making: implications for medication use. N Engl J Med 2018; 378:689–691
22.
Croskerry P: From mindless to mindful practice: cognitive bias and clinical decision making. N Engl J Med 2013; 368:2445–2448
23.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR, 4th ed, text revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000
24.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th ed. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2013
25.
Stone J, LaFrance WC, Levenson J, et al: Issues for DSM-5: conversion disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:626–627
26.
Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R: Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165:1493–1499
27.
Thaler RH, Sunstein CR: Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven, Conn, Yale University Press, 2008

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 690 - 693
PubMed: 31474126

History

Received: 11 May 2018
Revision received: 6 September 2018
Revision received: 15 October 2018
Revision received: 3 December 2018
Accepted: 10 December 2018
Published online: 1 September 2019
Published in print: September 01, 2019

Keywords

  1. Conversion Disorder
  2. Cognitive Heuristics

Authors

Affiliations

Benjamin R. Braus, M.D.
The Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. (Braus, Rummans, Lapid, Morgan, Sampson, Handler); and the Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, San Diego (Dimsdale).
Teresa A. Rummans, M.D. [email protected]
The Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. (Braus, Rummans, Lapid, Morgan, Sampson, Handler); and the Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, San Diego (Dimsdale).
Maria I. Lapid, M.D.
The Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. (Braus, Rummans, Lapid, Morgan, Sampson, Handler); and the Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, San Diego (Dimsdale).
Robert J. Morgan, M.D., Ph.D.
The Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. (Braus, Rummans, Lapid, Morgan, Sampson, Handler); and the Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, San Diego (Dimsdale).
Shirlene M. Sampson, M.D., M.S.
The Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. (Braus, Rummans, Lapid, Morgan, Sampson, Handler); and the Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, San Diego (Dimsdale).
Elliot M. Handler, M.D.
The Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. (Braus, Rummans, Lapid, Morgan, Sampson, Handler); and the Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, San Diego (Dimsdale).
Joel E. Dimsdale, M.D.
The Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. (Braus, Rummans, Lapid, Morgan, Sampson, Handler); and the Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, San Diego (Dimsdale).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Rummans ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Dr. Dimsdale is a consulting editor for UpToDate, and he receives royalties from Yale University Press. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share