Skip to main content
Full access
Editorials
Published Date: 1 February 2025

A Fresh Look at the Allostasis Theory of Addiction

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
Addictive disorders continue to be a major cause of death and disability worldwide, and substance use collectively accounts for 5.5% of global disease burden (1). The immense suffering of patients, families, and others affected by these problems cannot easily be quantified. Decades of scientific efforts and billions of dollars in research funding have only made a small dent in these tragic numbers.
People with clinically significant substance use problems show a motivational reprioritization (2) and as a result systematically forego healthy rewards in favor of drugs. They also continue to use drugs despite knowledge of serious, often fatal negative consequences (3). This would seem to indicate a significantly disordered function of brain circuitry subserving motivation and decision-making. However, our mechanistic understanding of why and how this happens in some people remains very limited (4). Obtaining scientific answers to these questions is critical for at least two reasons: to counter the stigma and social marginalization caused by the widespread perception of addiction as simply a character flaw, and to develop more effective, mechanism-based treatments (4, 5).
There once seemed to be a prospect for simple answers. Mesolimbic dopamine (DA) was established to play a key role for motivated behavior and reward, addictive substances were found to excessively activate this system, and the pursuit of reward mediated by DA was thought to be at the core of addiction (6). While an important starting point, this clearly could not be the whole story. For one thing, if it were, we would all become addicted. Things clearly must be more complex. There has since not been a lack of influential theories to account for the complexity of addiction (3, 7, 8). These theories, based on elegant preclinical data, have sparked lively debates in the field but are not necessarily mutually exclusive. The greater challenge is that their clinical relevance and ability to guide treatment development has remained unclear.
Among contemporary addiction theories, an affective allostasis conceptual framework has gained increasing influence. It emphasizes as a key phenomenon the emergence of negative affect, or “hyperkatifeia,” with development of addiction. This is thought to reflect a pathological recruitment of brain “anti-reward,” or stress- and aversion systems, driven by both acute and protracted withdrawal states. As these kick in, the thinking is that they cause a progressive shift of motivation, from positively to negatively reinforced drug seeking and taking, or from “reward” to “relief” seeking (7). This theory aligns with my clinical experience and has for decades guided my own research (9). But what if the stigma of addiction rears its head here as well and filters our clinical experience? Perhaps prevailing, moralizing attitudes they are met with make it easier for patients to talk about underlying traumatic experiences, negative emotions, and adverse consequences of drinking, rather than the highs that may still be fueling the desire to drink?
In this issue, King and colleagues (10) report provocative findings that would seem to prompt a need to take a fresh look at the allostasis theory, at least as it applies to alcohol use disorder (AUD). The investigators recruited people with or and without AUD. In both groups, they sampled high-resolution ecological momentary assessment (HR-EMA) data “in the wild” during a 3-hour monitoring period of an alcohol drinking episode and, on another occasion, a non-alcohol episode of the same duration. This allowed them to obtain a fine-grained characterization of subjective effects associated with alcohol taking. The specific subjective effects assessed were stimulation, liking, wanting, sedation, and negative affect.
The allostasis theory of addiction produces a clear, testable prediction for this experiment. In people with AUD, the pleasurable subjective effects of alcohol, such as stimulation, liking, and wanting should be attenuated compared with people without AUD. Conversely, the ability of alcohol intake to result in a reduction in negative affect should be increased. In fact, the study found exactly the opposite. Positive alcohol effects were greater in people with AUD. And while alcohol did reduce negative affect, this effect was smaller in magnitude and did not differ between the groups.
AUD is a highly heterogeneous condition, with the pattern of comorbidity being a clinically important aspect of heterogeneity. Comorbidity with internalizing disorders characterized by negative affect, such as depression and anxiety, is common in patients with AUD (11). If anywhere, reduction of negative affect by alcohol intake should presumably be most likely in that population. King and colleagues therefore targeted for recruitment participants with and without depression so that both their AUD and non-AUD groups were roughly balanced for this comorbidity. However, focusing on AUD participants with depression did not change the results. Positive alcohol effects still dominated in people with AUD and were greater than in people without it. Reduction of negative affect was small and did not differ between people with and without AUD.
But maybe an allostatic shift only occurs once AUD has reached a certain severity? To address this, King and colleagues compared the subjective effects of alcohol between participants with moderate-to-severe versus mild AUD. If anything, the positive subjective effects of alcohol were more pronounced in the more severe group, in which participants reported higher stimulation and wanting, although liking was similar between groups. Once again, the decrease in negative affect was small and did not differentiate people with mild AUD from those with the moderate-to-severe form of this condition.
These findings complement results coming from another important line of research by King and colleagues. For more than a decade, this group has followed a unique cohort. As young adults, participants entering this study were challenged with alcohol under controlled laboratory conditions, allowing subjective effects to be examined. Over the following years, participants were repeatedly reassessed, with very few of them being lost to follow-up. In this unique cohort, initial positive response to alcohol was the strongest predictor of the risk for subsequently developing AUD. And when the alcohol challenge was repeated, this positive alcohol response increased, rather than being attenuated over time, in those participants who developed AUD (12).
These studies are thoughtfully and elegantly designed. They are very well executed. The data are unusually clear. Collectively, these two lines of work would seem to pose a serious challenge for those of us whose work is guided by the allostasis theory of addiction. Nevertheless, I believe that important questions remain before it is possible to adjudicate this case.
Perhaps foremost among outstanding questions is whether the results of the present study will generalize to clinical AUD populations. The people with AUD that the authors studied were non-treatment seeking and had no current desire to stop drinking. This is a population that is likely to differ in important ways from people who seek treatment (13). Relatedly, almost half of the studied population had mild AUD. It is unclear to what extent this diagnostic category shares underlying mechanisms with what was described in the 1930s as “alcoholism” by patients themselves in the Big Book of Alcoholics Anonymous (14), as “alcohol dependence” half a century later by scientific pioneers of the alcohol field (15), or by any diagnostic system that has followed. At the low levels of severity that correspond to mild AUD, spontaneous remission is common, and the presence of a clinically relevant disorder can be questioned (4). The other important question that remains is of course to what extent the present findings will generalize to other addictive disorders.
In summary, King and colleagues present robust, provocative, and potentially very important findings in people with AUD. Their findings would seem to fundamentally challenge one of the most widely held contemporary theories of addiction that postulates affective homeostasis and a shift toward negatively reinforced pursuit of drugs to be central phenomena of addiction. Those of us who are inclined to believe that the allostasis theory captures clinically and neurobiologically important phenomena have some thinking to do.
For now, however, it remains entirely possible that the present findings are in fact not inconsistent with the allostasis theory of addiction. Instead, the implication may be that the neurobiological and motivational phenomena this theory rests on emerge relatively late in the development of addiction as a result of the onslaught of heavy drinking over many years to decades. Of course, even at this level of severity, treatment is sadly infrequent due to stigma, socioeconomic factors, and other reasons. But among people who seek treatment, the emergence of affective allostasis may well be a key factor that both maintains alcohol use and also leads patients to ultimately seek treatment. If this interpretation is correct, then the phenomena described by the allostasis theory, while not easily detected in non-treatment seeking populations, may still be critically important in patients seeking treatment for their addiction.

References

1.
GBD 2013 DALYs and HALE Collaborators; Murray CJ, Barber RM, et al: Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 306 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 188 countries, 1990–2013: quantifying the epidemiological transition. Lancet 2015; 386:2145–2191
2.
Millan EZ, McNally GP: Reprioritizing motivations in addiction. Science 2024; 384:271
3.
Lüscher C, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ: The transition to compulsion in addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2020; 21:247–263
4.
Heilig M, MacKillop J, Martinez D, et al: Addiction as a brain disease revised: why it still matters, and the need for consilience. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021; 46:1715–1723
5.
Heilig M, Epstein DH, Nader MA, et al: Time to connect: bringing social context into addiction neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2016; 17:592–599
6.
Wise RA, Bozarth MA: A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. Psychol Rev 1987; 94:469–492
7.
Koob GF: Drug addiction: hyperkatifeia/negative reinforcement as a framework for medications development. Pharmacol Rev 2021; 73:163–201
8.
Robinson TE, Berridge KC: Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 2003; 54:25–53
9.
Heilig M, Thorsell A, Sommer WH, et al: Translating the neuroscience of alcoholism into clinical treatments: from blocking the buzz to curing the blues. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010; 35:334–344
10.
King AC, Fischer AM, Cursio JF, et al: Real-time assessment of alcohol reward, stimulation, and negative affect in individuals with and without alcohol use disorder and depressive disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2025; 182:187–197
11.
Castillo-Carniglia A, Keyes KM, Hasin DS, et al: Psychiatric comorbidities in alcohol use disorder. Lancet Psychiatry 2019; 6:1068–1080
12.
King A, Vena A, Hasin DS, et al: Subjective responses to alcohol in the development and maintenance of alcohol use disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2021; 178:560–571
13.
Fein G, Landman B: Treated and treatment-naive alcoholics come from different populations. Alcohol 2005; 36:19–26
14.
Alcoholics Anonymous: The Story of How Many Thousands of Men and Women Have Recovered From Alcoholism, 4th ed. New York, NY, Alcoholics Anonymous World Services, 2001
15.
Edwards G, Gross MM: Alcohol dependence: provisional description of a clinical syndrome. Br Med J 1976; 1:1058–1061

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 142 - 144

History

Accepted: 20 November 2024
Published online: 1 February 2025
Published in print: February 01, 2025

Keywords

  1. Addiction Psychiatry
  2. Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders
  3. Alcohol

Authors

Details

Markus Heilig, M.D., Ph.D. markus.heilig@liu.se
Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Heilig (markus.heilig@liu.se).

Competing Interests

Dr. Heilig reports receiving research funding or consulting fees in the past 5 years from Aelis Farma, Brainsway Technologies, Camurus, Indivior, Janssen, Molteni, Nordic Drugs, and Pfizer.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing PsychiatryOnline@psych.org or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Figures

Tables

Media

Share

Share

Share article link

Share