Skip to main content
Full access
Influential Publications
Published Online: 1 July 2013

A Meta-Analytic Review of Prolonged Exposure for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Abstract

Two decades of research demonstrate the efficacy of exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The efficacy of prolonged exposure (PE), a specific exposure therapy program for PTSD that has been disseminated throughout the world, has been established in many controlled studies using different trauma populations. However, a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of PE for PTSD has not been conducted to date. The purpose of the current paper is to estimate the overall efficacy of PE for PTSD relative to adequate controls. We included all published randomized controlled trials of PE vs. control (wait-list or psychological placebo) for the treatment of PTSD in adolescents or adults. Treatments were classified as PE if they included multiple sessions of imaginal and in vivo exposure and were based on the manualized treatment developed by Foa, Rothbaum, Riggs, and Murdock (1991). Thirteen studies with a total sample size of 675 participants met the final inclusion criteria. The primary analyses showed a large effect for PE versus control on both primary (Hedges’s g = 1.08) and secondary (Hedges’s g = 0.77) outcome measures. Analyses also revealed medium to large effect sizes for PE at follow-up, both for primary (Hedges’s g = 0.68) and secondary (Hedges’s g = 0.41) outcome measures. There was no significant difference between PE and other active treatments (CPT, EMDR, CT, and SIT). Effect sizes were not moderated by time since trauma, publication year, dose, study quality, or type of trauma. The average PE-treated patient fared better than 86% of patients in control conditions at post-treatment on PTSD measures. PE is a highly effective treatment for PTSD, resulting in substantial treatment gains that are maintained over time.
(Reprinted with permission from Clinical Psychology Review 2010; 30:635–641)
It is estimated that most Americans (81.7%) will be exposed to a trauma during their lifetime (Sledjeski, Speisman, & Dierker, 2008) and 6.8% will meet criteria for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) at some point in their lives (Kessler et al., 2005). It has been estimated that anxiety disorders account for one third of all mental health care costs in the United States and PTSD is the most costly anxiety disorder (Greenberg et al., 1999). Compared to individuals without PTSD or with other disorders, those with PTSD are more likely to have other current or past psychiatric diagnoses, with lifetime psychiatric comorbidity rates on the order of 80% (Fairbank, Ebert, & Caddell, 2001). Further, PTSD is associated with disruptions in work, social functioning, and physical health (Alonso et al., 2004; Galovski & Lyons, 2004; Smith, Schnurr, & Rosenheck, 2005; Zatzick et al., 1997).
Fortunately, many studies have documented the efficacy of pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). A meta-analysis of pharmacotherapy for PTSD showed that more patients responded to medication (59.1%) compared to placebo (38.5%) (Stein, Ipser, & Seedat, 2006). However, the best evidence and treatment guidelines suggest trauma focused psychotherapy is more effective and should be considered a first line treatment for PTSD (Foa, Keane, Friedman, & Cohen, 2009; NICE, 2005; Penava, Otto, Pollack, & Rosenbaum, 1996). Existing meta-analyses of psychotherapy trials have provided estimates of the effect sizes of psychotherapy on trauma-related and other symptoms. For example, Sherman examined whether psychological treatments for PTSD (e.g. CBT, EMDR, and Dynamic therapy; 17 studies) were effective (Sherman, 1998). The results showed a moderate effect of treatment on improvement in PTSD symptoms (d = 0.52). However, this study did not examine the relative efficacy of psychological treatments compared to control groups. Also, Van Etten and Taylor (1998) examined the efficacy of all treatments for PTSD (61 studies including pharmacotherapy, psychotherapy, and control conditions). Overall, psychological and pharmacological treatments outperformed controls. In addition, eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) and cognitive behavioral therapy were identified as more effective than other psychological treatments. A meta-analysis by Bradley, Greene, Russ, Dutra, and Westen (2005) also supported the efficacy of psychotherapy for PTSD with data from 26 controlled trials across multiple treatments.
More recent meta-analyses have focused on the efficacy of specific psychological treatments for PTSD. Seidler and Wagner (2006) compared seven studies of “trauma-focused” cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) to EMDR. The results showed that both treatments were effective with no significant differences between treatments. In an analysis of 33 studies, Bisson and Andrew (2007) demonstrated that “trauma-focused” CBT, EMDR, stress management and group “trauma-focused” CBT were more effective than “non-trauma focused” treatments at reducing PTSD symptoms. In a similar study, Bisson et al. (2007) analyzed 38 studies on “trauma-focused” CBT, EMDR, stress management, and group CBT. Results indicated that “trauma-focused” CBT and EMDR were more efficacious than waitlist/control on most outcome measures; however, the evidence for EMDR was not as strong. In contrast, Benish, Imel, and Wampold (2008) found no significant differences among active psychological treatments for PTSD (15 studies); Benish et al. interpreted this lack of treatment differences as evidence that “specific ingredients may not be critical for the treatment of PTSD” (p. 754). However, their meta-analysis did not include comparisons between active treatments and those comprising only nonspecific factors (e.g., therapeutic alliance). Furthermore, the authors stated that “although prolonged exposure has been selected … as a model treatment for dissemination … the meta-analytic findings do not suggest that any particular therapy is superior to another” (p. 755). However, the inclusion of comparisons between very similar, effective treatments (e.g., prolonged exposure vs. prolonged exposure plus cognitive restructuring) would not be expected to reveal differences, and therefore would obscure overall treatment differences.
Previous meta-analyses have also collapsed “trauma-focused” therapies together such that any significant outcome differences among treatments are obscured. Because the procedures of these treatments vary, the ways in which trauma is addressed also differ; thus, the effects of these treatments may differ as well. The present study investigates the efficacy of one specific type of this therapy, prolonged exposure (PE). PE is a manualized treatment package that consists of 9 to 12 90-min sessions. Sessions one and two include information gathering and psychoeducation. The later sessions include repeated imaginal exposure to the index trauma and assignment of in-vivo exposure homework to avoided trauma cues.
PE was chosen as the focus of this analysis for several reasons. First, the American Psychological Association’s Division 12, which addresses empirically-supported psychological treatments, concluded that PE has “strong research support,” making it a “well-established” treatment for PTSD. In addition, several new randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have examined the efficacy of prolonged exposure (Gilboa-Schechtman et al., unpublished manuscript; Nacasch et al., under review; Schnurr et al., 2007). These new RCTs also provide follow-up data that allow for a more thorough evaluation of the maintenance of PE’s effect. Further, PE was chosen for national dissemination by major healthcare administrations due to its efficacy. For example, the Veterans’ Administration Office of Mental Health Services recently funded a “national rollout” to disseminate of PE into VA hospitals as a treatment of choice for veterans suffering from PTSD (Nemeroff et al., 2006). Likewise, the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) recommended PE as a model treatment for nationwide use (Nemeroff et al., 2006). Despite the evidence supporting this treatment, no meta-analyses have been conducted to examine the unique contribution of PE. Therefore, the purpose of this meta-analysis is to provide an up-to-date estimate of treatment efficacy for PTSD by combining multiple randomized controlled trials of PE. In addition, this meta-analysis addresses some of the issues raised by Benish et al. (2008) above by including conditions that control for non-specific factors. Outcome variables were classified into two categories: primary (PTSD symptom severity) and secondary (general subjective distress; Table 1).
Table 1. Measures for Coding Analyses
DomainMeasure
Primary outcome measures
CAPS, MPSS-SR, PCL, PSS-I, PSS-SR, PTSD severity, SIP, SI-PTSD
Secondary outcome measuresBDI, HADS-A, HADS-D, IES, QOLI, SAS (G,S,W), STAI, STAI-S, STAI-T
Note. BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; CAPS = Clinician Administered PTSD Scale; HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; IES = Impact of Events Scale; MPSS-SR = Modified PTSD Symptom Scale-Self Report; PCL = PTSD Checklist; PSS-I = PTSD Symptom Scale-Interview; PSS-SR = PTSD Symptom Scale-Self Report; QOLI = Quality of Life Inventory; SAS (G,S,W) = Social Adjustment Scale (Global, Social, Work); SIP = Structured Interview for PTSD; SI-PTSD = Davidson’s Structured Interview for PTSD; STAI = State Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAI-S = State subscale of State Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAI-T = Trait subscale of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory.
We derived several hypotheses from the existing literature. First, we expected that PE would outperform control conditions on primary outcome measures (Hypothesis 1) and secondary outcome measures (Hypothesis 2) at post-treatment. In addition, we predicted that waitlist controlled trials would produce larger effect sizes than psychological placebo controlled trials at post-treatment (Hypothesis 3). We did not expect PE to outperform other active treatment conditions (e.g. EMDR, CPT; Hypothesis 4). At follow-up, we expected that PE would outperform control conditions on primary outcome measures (Hypothesis 5) and secondary outcome measures (Hypothesis 6). Finally, in follow-up analyses we examined the potential moderating effect of publication year and dose (number of sessions) on effect size. Publication year was included as a potential moderator to examine whether effect sizes are changing over time (due to, e.g., refinements are rendering the treatments more efficacious).

1. Method

1.1. Study Selection

We selected well-controlled randomized trials of PE for PTSD using a comprehensive search strategy. We searched the following databases: PsycINFO (1840 to March 2009), MEDLINE (1966 to March 2009), Scopus (1869 to March 2009), and the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials up to March 2009. The searches included the following terms: “prolonged exposure,” “cognitive behavioral,” “clinical trial,” “random,” “randomly,” “randomize,” “randomise,” “randomized,” or “randomised” alone and in combination with “posttraumatic,” “post-traumatic,” “posttraumatic stress disorder,” “post-traumatic stress disorder,” or “PTSD”. These words were searched as key words, title, abstract, and Medical Subject Headings. Also, we examined citation maps and used the “cited by” search tools. These findings were cross-referenced with references from reviews. In addition, we contacted authors of PE randomized trials for emerging publications. These initial search strategies produced 127 potential articles. Examination of the abstracts identified 23 relevant articles. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) participants who met full DSM-III-R, DSM-IV, or DSM-IV-TR criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder; (b) random assignment; (c) adequate control condition (psychological placebo or wait-list control); (d) adult or adolescent participants; and (e) more than one session of PE during the acute phase of treatment. Treatments were classified as PE if they included both imaginal and in vivo exposure. Exposures were classified as in vivo if they involved confronting trauma cues in real life. All included studies used exposure therapy that was based on the manualized treatment developed by Foa et al. (1991). Exclusion criteria were: (a) single case studies; (b) studies focused on acute stress disorder; and (c) studies with insufficient data, unless study authors were able to provide such data. Of the 23 identified studies, ten were excluded. Four studies had inadequate trials of PE based on the above definition (Bryant et al., 2003; Cloitre, Koenen, Cohen, & Han, 2002; Glynn et al., 1999; Ironson, Freund, Strauss, & Williams, 2002). Four studies did not have an adequate control condition (Bryant et al., 2008; Ironson et al., 2002; Lee, Gavriel, Drummond, Richards, & Greenwald, 2002; Paunovic & Öst, 2001); in one study medication was the only treatment provided during the acute phase of treatment (Rothbaum et al., 2006); and in one study over one third of the participants did not meet full DSM-IV-TR criteria for PTSD (Difede et al., 2007). Thirteen studies with a total sample size of 658 participants met the final inclusion criteria and were included in the meta-analysis.

1.2. Software

All analyses were completed with Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Borenstein & Rothstein, 1999). Comprehensive Meta-Analysis is a program funded by the National Institutes of Health’s SBIR program.

1.3. Procedure

Data on the following variables were collected: treatment conditions, treatment dose (number of sessions and total hours), number of participants, and year of publication. Dependent variables were classified into categories including primary (PTSD/domain-specific subjective distress) and secondary (general subjective distress: Table 1). In some of the included comparisons, PE was modified and combined with other treatments such as cognitive restructuring (Foa et al., 1999,2005; Marks, Lovell, Noshirvani, Livanou, & Thrasher, 1998; Power et al., 2002); results will be reported with and without these comparisons included.
Control conditions were classified into three categories: active treatment, psychological placebo or wait-list. Active treatment was defined as those treatments that were purported to contain specific ingredients to alleviate PTSD symptoms. Treatments that were categorized as psychological placebo included: supportive counseling (SC), relaxation (R), Present Centered Therapy (PCT), Time Limited Psychodynamic Therapy (TLDP), and treatment as usual (TAU). Wait-list (WL) was defined as a control condition in which participants did not receive any treatment for PTSD symptoms for a specified amount of time. Six of thirteen studies utilized psychological placebos as the control condition, 5 of the 13 studies had wait-list as the control condition and 2 of the 13 studies included both a psychological placebo condition and a wait-list condition (see Table 2).
Table 2. Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis
StudyConditionsN# of SessionsTotal hoursPrimary outcome measuresSecondary outcome measures
Asukai (in press)
PE +Psych PL, Psych PL
24
8-15
12-22.5
CAPS
CES-D, GHQ-28, IES
Foa et al. (1991)
SIT, PE, Psych PL, WL
45
9
13.5
PTSD severity measure
BDI, STAI
Foa et al. (1999)
PE, SIT, PE + SIT, WL
79
9
14.5
PSS-I
BDI, SAS, STAI
Foa et al. (2005)
PE, PE + CR, WL
121
8–12
12–24
PSS-I
BDI, SAS
Gilboa-Shechtman et al. (unpublished manuscript)
PE, Psych PL
30
6–19
6–28.5
CPSS
BDI, CGAS
Marks et al. (1998)
PE, CR, PE + CR, Psych PL
77
10
15–17.5
CAPS, PSS-SR
Work/social interference
McDonagh et al. (2005)
PE, PCT, WL
57
14
24.5
CAPS
BDI, QOLI, STAI
Nacasch et al. (under review)
PE, Psych PL
26
9–15
13.5–30
PSS-I
BDI, STAI
Power et al. (2002)
EMDR, PE + CR, WL
72
10
15
CAPS, SI-PTSD
BDI, HADS-A, HADS-D, HAM-A,
Resick, Nishith, Weaver, Astin, and Feuer(2002)
CPT, PE, WL
121
9
13
CAPS, PSS-SR
BDI
Rothbaum, Astin, and Marsteller (2005)
PE, EMDR, WL
60
9
13.5
CAPS, PSS-SR
BDI, IES, STAI
Schnurr et al. (2007)
PE, Psych PL
201
10
15
CAPS, PCL
BDI, QOLI, STAI
Taylor et al. (2003)
PE, EMDR, Psych PL
45
8
12
CAPS, PSS-SR
BDI
Note. BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; CAPS = Clinician Administered PTSD Scale; HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; IES = Impact of Events Scale; MPSS-SR = Modified PTSD Symptom Scale-Self Report; PCL = PTSD Checklist; PSS-I = PTSD Symptom Scale-Interview; PSS-SR = PTSD Symptom Scale-Self Report; QOLI = Quality of Life Inventory; SAS (G,S,W) = Social Adjustment Scale (Global, Social, Work); SIP = Structured Interview for PTSD; SI-PTSD = Davidson’s Structured Interview for PTSD; STAI = State Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAI-S = State subscale of State Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAI-T = Trait subscale of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory.

1.4. Study Quality Ratings

The quality of the included studies was then rated using a modified version of Jadad et al.’s quality assessment guidelines (Jadad et al., 1996). Jadad et al. included three criteria for the quality assessment: 1) random assignment to condition; 2) double-blinding; and 3) description of withdrawals and dropouts. Because this meta-analysis reviews a psychological treatment, the second criterion cannot be applied here because patients cannot remain blind to their treatment condition; therefore we modified this criterion to require that the evaluators were blind to the therapeutic condition (i.e., single blind).

1.5. Effect Size Calculation

Between-group effect sizes for each study were computed using Hedges’s g (Rosenthal, 1991). Studies with multiple outcomes were categorized as above (see Table 1) and then combined within each domain. When the necessary data were available, Hedges’s g was calculated directly using the following formula: where is the mean of the treatment group, is the mean of the comparison group, and SP is the pooled standard deviation. If these data were not provided, Hedges’s g was estimated using conversion equations for significance tests (e.g., t, F) (Rosenthal, 1991). All effect sizes were corrected for small sample sizes according to Hedges and Olkin (1985). Therefore, a smaller sample size reduces the estimated effect size helping control for different sample sizes across studies. These controlled effect sizes may then be interpreted conservatively with Cohen’s convention of small (0.2), medium (0.5), and large (0.8) effects (Cohen, 1988). Hedges’s g also may be computed directly from Cohen’s d with the following formula: . When there were multiple outcomes per domain they were combined according to Borenstein, Hedges, and Rothstein (2007). The overall mean effect size for all of the studies combined was computed using the following formula: where wj is the weight for each study and gj is the effect size for each study. Effect sizes were calculated with random effects models. The random effects analysis estimates the overall effect size assuming the studies included are only a sample of the entire population of studies and/or when the studies are heterogeneous.

2. Results

Hypothesis 1.

PE will outperform the control conditions on primary outcome measures at post-treatment.
This analysis included 13 studies with 675 participants. Consistent with prediction, prolonged exposure outperformed control conditions on primary outcome measures at posttreatment showing a large effect size (Hedges’s g = 1.08 [SE = 0.20, 95% CI: 0.69 to 1.46, p < 0.001)]). Thus, the average participant receiving prolonged exposure fared better than 86% of the control participants at posttreatment on primary outcome measures (Fig. 1).
Figure 1. Forest Plot of Prolonged Exposure vs. Control Effect Sizes (Hedges’s g) at Posttreatment on Primary Outcome Measures

Hypothesis 2.

PE will outperform the control conditions on secondary outcome measures at post-treatment.
This analysis included 13 studies with 666 participants. Consistent with prediction, prolonged exposure outperformed control conditions on secondary outcome measures at posttreatment showing a large effect size (Hedges’s g = 0.77 [SE = 0.12, 95% CI: 0.53 to 1.01, p < 0.001]). Thus, the average participant receiving prolonged exposure fared better than 79% of the control participants at posttreatment on secondary outcome measures.

Hypothesis 3.

PE versus wait-list will produce a larger effect than PE versus psychological placebo at post-treatment on primary outcome measures.
This analysis included 13 studies with 675 participants. Consistent with prediction, the overall effect size of PE compared to wait-list (Hedges’s g = 1.51 [SE = 0.20, 95% CI: 1.12 to 1.90]) was significantly greater than the overall effect size of PE compared to psychological placebo (Hedges’s g = 0.65 [SE = 0.19, 95% CI: 0.29 to 1.01]) at post-treatment (p = .001).

Hypothesis 4.

PE will not significantly outperform other active treatments on primary outcome measures.
This analysis included 6 studies with 262 participants in PE, EMDR, CPT, CT, and SIT. Consistent with prediction, there was not a significant overall difference between PE and active treatment conditions on primary outcome measures at posttreatment (Hedges’s g = −0.07 [SE = 0.18, 95% CI: −0.42 to 0.28, p = .69]).

Hypothesis 5.

PE will outperform the control conditions on primary outcome measures at follow-up
This analysis included 7 studies with 348 participants. Follow-up assessments ranged between one and 12 months. Consistent with prediction, PE outperformed control conditions at follow-up on primary outcome measures showing a medium to large effect size (Hedges’s g = 0.68 [SE = 0.21, 95% CI: 0.27 to 1.10, p = .001]). Thus, the average participant receiving prolonged exposure fared better than 76% of the control participants at follow-up on primary outcome measures (Fig. 2).
Figure 2. Forest Plot of Prolonged Exposure vs. Control Effect Sizes (Hedges’s g) at Follow-Up on Primary Outcome Measures

Hypothesis 6.

PE will outperform the control conditions on secondary outcome measures at follow-up.
This analysis included 7 studies with 368 participants. Consistent with prediction, PE outperformed control conditions at follow-up on secondary outcome measures showing a medium effect size (Hedges’s g = 0.41 [SE = 0.19, 95% CI: 0.03 to 0.78, p = .03)]). Thus, the average participant receiving prolonged exposure fared better than 66% of the control participants at follow-up on secondary outcome measures.

3. Validity of Meta-Analytic Results

3.1. Heterogeneity

A heterogeneity analysis was conducted to test the assumption that the effect sizes were from a homogeneous sample (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). For this analysis all 13 studies were included with all time points (post-treatment and follow-up) on primary outcome measures. The test was significant, Q(12) = 59.90, p<0.001, suggesting that the random effects analyses were most appropriate for this study.

3.2. Moderators: Effect Size as a Function of Publication Year, Dose, Type of Trauma, and Time Since Trauma

The following analyses were completed using unrestricted maximum likelihood meta regressions. There was no significant relationship between effect size and time since trauma (β = 0.55, p = 0.61), publication year (β = −0.00, p = 0.86), dose (β = −0.01, p = 0.71), or study quality (β = −0.30, p = 0.07). There was also no significant difference in effect sizes across types of trauma (combat/ terror, childhood sexual abuse, rape, mixed: p = 0.14).

3.3. Publication Bias: “The File Drawer Problem”

Several authors suggest there may be a potential discrepancy between the number of published trials and the total number that are completed (Bakan, 1967; McNemar, 1960; Smart, 1964; Sterling, 1959). Therefore, any meta-analysis of published studies may be missing non-significant studies and therefore overestimate the overall effect size. Rosenthal and others have called this confound “The File Drawer Problem” (Rosenthal, 1991). A conservative method of addressing this problem is to assume that the effect sizes of all current or future unpublished studies are equal to 0 and compute the number of such studies it would require to reduce the overall effect size to a non-significant level (Rosenthal & Rubin, 1988). This value may be referred to as the “fail-safe N”.
Rosenthal suggested the following equation to compute a fail-safe where K is the number of studies in the meta-analysis and is the mean Z obtained from the K studies (Rosenthal, 1991). Rosenthal also suggested that findings may be considered robust if the required number of studies (X) to reduce the overall effect size to a non-significant level exceeded 5 K + 10 which in this study would be 75 (Rosenthal, 1991). Analyses revealed that it would require more than 446 current or future unpublished studies with an effect size of 0 to bring the overall effect size of the primary analyses within the non-significant range, suggesting that the findings in this meta-analysis are robust.

4. Discussion

4.1. Major Findings

This meta-analysis of 13 (N = 658) randomized controlled PE trials generally supported the hypotheses. As predicted, PE performed significantly better than control conditions on measures of PTSD both at post-treatment (g = 1.08) as well as at follow-up (g = 0.68). Similarly, PE treatment was associated with significantly better outcomes on secondary outcome measures, both at post-treatment (g = 0.77) and at follow-up (g = 0.41). These effect sizes compare quite favorably to those of other empirically supported psychotherapies; for example, cognitive therapy for depression has a reported effect size at post-treatment of d = 0.82 versus wait-list or placebo (Gloaguen, Cottraux, Cucherat, & Blackburn, 1998). In addition, the large effect sizes associated with PE in the present analyses are in contrast to the relatively smaller effects seen when “psychotherapeutic treatments” were lumped together (d = 0.52 post-treatment, d = 0.64 at follow-up) (Sherman, 1998). The current effect sizes were very robust and clearly not attributable to publication bias, given that it would require more than 400 PE trials with effect size equal to 0 to render the current results nonsignificant (Rosenthal, 1991). Taken together these results demonstrate that PE is a highly effective treatment for PTSD that confers lasting benefits across a wide range of outcomes.
Consistent with previous meta-analyses, there was no significant difference between PE and other active treatments (CPT, EMDR, CT, and SIT). This meta-analysis cannot answer the question of why these different treatments show similar efficacy. It is possible that these treatments work through separate mechanisms that are equally efficacious. Alternatively, it may be that the application of exposure routinely employed in all of these treatments is responsible for improvement. Future research may help further answer this question. Consistent with our hypothesis, the effect size for PE versus wait-list (g = 1.06) was significantly greater than the effect size for PE versus psychological placebo (g = 0.65). This is consistent with previous meta-analyses of psychological approaches (e.g. Feske & Chambless, 1995). There was significant effect size heterogeneity among the 13 studies included in the meta-analysis, indicating greater variability in effect sizes across studies than would be expected from sampling error alone (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). There was no significant relationship between effect size and dose (number of sessions). This finding could be due to restricted range (8 to 17 sessions) or flexible dosing (4 studies included flexible dosing). However, when the four studies with a flexible dose were removed we also found no relationship between effect size and dose (β = − 0.09, p = 0.61). It is also encouraging to note that effect sizes were not moderated by time since trauma, publication year, study quality, or type of trauma.
Results of the current meta-analysis have meaningful implications for mental health professionals who treat patients with PTSD. A therapist can expect that his/her average PE treated patient will fare better than 86% of patients treated with supportive counseling and similar unstructured talk therapies. The present analyses are also of interest to patients with PTSD who are weighing their treatment options, as well as to policy makers responsible, for example, for setting training priorities.

4.2. Limitations

Several limitations deserve comment. First, the current analyses cannot definitively answer the question of the relative effectiveness of PE compared to other active psychological treatments for PTSD as there were an insufficient number of eligible studies comparing PE with CPT (one study), EMDR (three studies) or SIT (two studies), to obtain a stable estimate of these effect sizes. Nevertheless, we presented this analysis by combining the six comparison treatments. There also are too few dismantling studies to examine the relative efficacy of the individual components of PE (imaginal and in vivo exposure). To our knowledge there is only one study that included an imaginal exposure only condition and met the other inclusion criteria (Bryant et al., 2003). Although there was only one study, it is interesting to note that the controlled effect size for imaginal exposure only was similar in magnitude to our overall findings (g = 0.83).
Second, the current analysis did not test the effectiveness of PE compared to pharmacological treatments. However, existing results suggest that the effect of pharmacotherapy for PTSD is significantly less than the effects observed here for PE. For example, a meta-analysis of nine randomized placebo-controlled pharmacotherapy trials found an effect size of 0.41 for PTSD symptoms. Even the medication (the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor fluoxetine) with the highest effect size at 0.65 was not of the same magnitude as the result found for PE (Penava et al., 1996).
Third, several of the studies were carried out by the same research group (i.e., Foa et al.). This raises the question of whether findings are similar when this type of treatment is implemented by different groups of researchers. We chose to examine this question empirically. Analysis showed that effect sizes were not significantly different between Foa’s group and other researchers (p = 0.47). Unfortunately data were not available to further determine the effects of gender or chronicity.
Finally, the follow-up analyses included fewer studies (7 vs. 13) and patients (348 vs. 675 for primary outcome measures) than the posttreatment analyses because many of the studies did not include a follow-up assessment. It is crucial to know whether the substantial posttreatment gains are maintained over time, and therefore it is encouraging to see a substantial effect size at follow-up. However, analyses that include fewer patients result in a loss of statistical power and therefore a less reliable estimate of the treatment effect size. Hopefully, future studies will include follow-up assessments such that subsequent meta-analyses can confirm the long-term effectiveness of PE for PTSD.

4.3. Summary and Conclusions

In sum, the results show that PE is highly effective in treating PTSD, and significantly more effective than inactive (waitlist) and active (psychological placebo) control conditions. In fact, the average PE treated patient fared better than 86% of patients in the control conditions. These findings are in marked contrast to the conclusions of Benish et al. (2008) and suggest that the efficacy of exposure treatments for PTSD cannot be attributed solely to non-specific factors. Consistent with prediction (and with the Benish et al. findings), there was no significant difference between PE and other active treatment conditions (CPT, EMDR, CT, and SIT). PE effectively treats both PTSD-specific distress as well as more general trauma-related distress (e.g., depressed mood), and the benefits are dramatic and enduring. The large effect sizes support the status of PE as the first line treatment-of-choice for PTSD.

References

Alonso J., Angermeyer M. C., Bernert S., Bruffaerts R., Brugha T. S., Bryson H., et al. (2004). Disability and quality of life impact of mental disorders in Europe: Results from the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD) project. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Supplement, 109(420), 38−46.
Asukai N. (in press). Comparison of prolonged exposure and treatment as usual for PTSD in Japan.
Bakan D. (1967). On method. San Francisco, CA: Josey-Bass.
Benish S. G., Imel Z. E., Wampold B. E. (2008). The relative efficacy of bona fide psychotherapies for treating post-traumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of direct comparisons. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(5), 746−758.
Bisson J., Andrew M. (2007). Psychological treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Review, Issue 3, Art. No.: CD003388.
Bisson J. I., Ehlers A., Matthews R., Pilling S., Richards D., Turner S. (2007). Psychological treatments for chronic post-traumatic stress disorder: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 97−104 FEB.
Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Rothstein H. (2007). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Borenstein M., Rothstein H. (1999). Comprehensive meta-analysis, a computer program for research synthesis. New Jersey: Biostat Inc.
Bradley R., Greene J., Russ E., Dutra L., Westen D. (2005). A multidimensional meta-analysis of psychotherapy for PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2), 214−227.
Bryant R. A., Moulds M. L., Guthrie R. M., Dang S. T., Mastrodomenico J., Nixon R. D. V., et al. (2008). A randomized controlled trial of exposure therapy and cognitive restructuring for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(4), 695−703.
Bryant R. A., Moulds M. L., Guthrie R. M., Dang S. T., Nixon R. D. V. (2003). Imaginal exposure alone and imaginal exposure with cognitive restructuring in treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(4), 706−712.
Cloitre M., Koenen K. C., Cohen L. R., Han H. (2002). Skills training in affective and interpersonal regulation followed by exposure: A phase-based treatment for PTSD related to childhood abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(5), 1067−1074.
Cohen J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Difede J., Malta L. S., Best S., Henn-Haase C., Metzler T., Bryant R., et al. (2007). A randomized controlled clinical treatment trial for World Trade Center attack-related PTSD in disaster workers. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195(10), 861 −865.
Fairbank J. A., Ebert L., Caddell J. M. (2001). Posttraumatic stress disorder. In, Sutker P. B., Adams H. E. (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology (pp. 183−209)., 3rd ed. New York: Kluwer.
Feske U., Chambless D. L. (1995). Cognitive behavioral versus exposure only treatment for social phobia: A meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy, 26, 695−720.
Foa E. B., Dancu C. V., Hembree E. A., Jaycox L. H., Meadows E. A., Street G. P. (1999). A comparison of exposure therapy, stress inoculation training, and their combination for reducing posttraumatic stress disorder in female assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 194−200.
Foa E. B., Hembree E. A., Cahill S. P., Rauch S. A., Riggs D. S., Feeny N. C., et al. (2005). Randomized trial of prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder with and without cognitive restructuring: ùtcome at academic and community clinics. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 73(5), 953−964.
Foa E. B., Keane T. M., Friedman M. J., Cohen J. A. (2009). Effective treatments for PTSD: Practice guidelines from the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. New York: The Guilford Press.
Foa E. B., Rothbaum B. O., Riggs D. S., Murdock T. B. (1991). Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims: A comparison between cognitive-behavioral procedures and counseling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(5), 715−723.
Galovski T., Lyons J. A. (2004). Psychological sequelae of combat violence: A review of the impact of PTSD on the veteran's family and possible interventions. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9(5), 477−501.
Gilboa-Schechtman E., Foa E. B., Shafran N., Aderka I. M., Powers M. B., Rachamim L., Rosenbach L., Yadin E., Apter A. (unpublished manuscript). A pilot randomized controlled trial of prolonged exposure and time limited dynamic therapy for adolescent victims of single event traumas.
Gloaguen V., Cottraux J., Cucherat M., Blackburn I. -M. (1998). A meta-analysis of the effects of cognitive therapy in depressed patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 49(1), 59−72.
Glynn S. M., Eth S., Randolph E. T., Foy D. W., Urbaitis M., Boxer L., et al. (1999). A test of behavioral family therapy to augment exposure for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 243−251.
Greenberg P. E., Sisitsky T., Kessler R. C., Finkelstein S. N., Berndt E. R., Davidson J. R. T., et al. (1999). The economic burden of anxiety disorders in the 1990s. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60(7), 427−435.
Hedges L. V., Olkin I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Olrando, FL: Academic Press.
Ironson G., Freund B., Strauss J. L., Williams J. (2002). Comparison of two treatments for traumatic stress: A community-based study of EMDR and prolonged exposure. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58(1), 113−128.
Jadad A. R., Moore R. A., Carroll D., Jenkinson C., Reynolds D. J. M., Gavaghan D. J., et al. (1996). Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: Is blinding necessary? Controlled Clinical Trials, 17(1), 1 − 12.
Kessler R. C., Berglund P., Demler O., Jin R., Merikangas K. R., Walters E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593−602.
Lee C., Gavriel H., Drummond P., Richards J., Greenwald R. (2002). Treatment of PTSD: Stress inoculation training with prolonged exposure compared to EMDR. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58(9), 1071 −1089.
Marks I., Lovell K., Noshirvani H., Livanou M., Thrasher S. (1998). Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder by exposure and/or cognitive restructuring: A controlled study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(4), 317−325.
McDonagh A., McHugo G., Sengupta A., Demment C C., Schnurr P. P., Friedman M., et al. (2005). Randomized trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy for chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in adult female survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 515−524.
McNemar Q. (1960). At random: Sense and nonsense. American Psychologist, 15, 295−300.
Nacasch N., Foa E. B., Huppert J. D., Tzur D., Fostick L., Dinstein Y., et al. (under review). The efficacy of prolonged exposure therapy for combat and terror-related PTSD: A randomized control comparison with treatment as usual. The American Journal of Psychiatry.
Nemeroff C. B., Bremner J. D., Foa E. B., Mayberg H. S., North C S., Stein M. B. (2006). Posttraumatic stress disorder: A state-of-the-science review. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40(1), 1−21.
NICE (2005). Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): The management of PTSD in adults and children in primary and secondary care. London: National Institute for Clinical Excellence.
Paunovic N., Öst L. G. (2001). Cognitive-behavior therapy vs exposure therapy in the treatment of PTSD in refugees. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39(10), 1183−1197.
Penava S. J., Otto M. W., Pollack M. H., Rosenbaum J. F. (1996). Current status of pharmacotherapy for PTSD: An effect size analysis of controlled studies. Depression and Anxiety, 4(5), 240−242.
Power K., McGoldrick T., Brown K., Buchanan R., Sharp D., Swanson V., et al. (2002). A controlled comparison of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing versus exposure plus cognitive restructuring versus waiting list in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 9, 299−318.
Resick P. A., Nishith P., Weaver T. L., Astin M. C., Feuer C. A. (2002). A comparison of cognitive-processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape victims. J Consult Clin Psychol, 70(4), 867−879.
Rosenthal R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. London, UK: Sage.
Rosenthal R., Rubin D. B. (1988). Comment: Assumptions and procedures in the file drawer problem. Statistical Science, 3,120−125.
Rothbaum B. O., Astin M. C., Marsteller F. (2005). Prolonged exposure versus eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) for PTSD rape victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18(6), 607−616.
Rothbaum B. O., Cahill S. P., Foa E. B., Davidson J. R. T., Compton J., Connor K. M., et al. (2006). Augmentation of sertraline with prolonged exposure in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19(5), 625−638.
Schnurr P. P., Friedman M. J., Engel C. C., Foa E. B., Shea M. T., Chow B. K., et al. (2007). Cognitive behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 297(8), 820−830.
Seidler G. H., Wagner F. E. (2006). Comparing the efficacy of EMDR and trauma-focused cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment of PTSD: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Medicine, 36(11), 1515−1522.
Sherman J. J. (1998). Effects of psychotherapeutic treatments for PTSD: A meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11(3), 413−435.
Sledjeski E. M., Speisman B., Dierker L. C. (2008). Does number of lifetime traumas explain the relationship between PTSD and chronic medical conditions? Answers from the National Comorbidity Survey-Replication (NCS-R). Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31,341−349.
Smart R. G. (1964). The importance of negative results in psychological research. Canadian Psychologist., 5A, 225−232.
Smith M. W., Schnurr P. P., Rosenheck R. A. (2005). Employment outcomes and PTSD symptom severity. Mental Health Services Research, 7(2), 89−101.
Stein D. J., Ipser J., Seedat S. (2006). Pharmacotherapy for post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Chochrane Database of Systematic Reviews(1), Art. No.: CD002795.
Sterling T. D. (1959). Publication decisions and their possible effects on inferences drawn from tests of significance—Or vice versa. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 54,30−40.
Taylor S., Thordarson D. S., Fedoroff I. C., Maxfield L., Lovell K., Ogrodniczuk J. (2003). Comparative efficacy, speed, and adverse effects of three PTSD treatments: Exposure therapy, EMDR, and relaxation training. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(2), 330−338.
Van Etten M. L., Taylor S. (1998). Comparative efficacy of treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. [Article]. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 5(3), 126−144.
Zatzick D. F., Marmar C. R., Weiss D. S., Browner W. S., Metzler T. J., Golding J. M., et al. (1997). Posttraumatic stress disorder and functioning and quality of life outcomes in a nationally representative sample of male Vietnam veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(12), 1690−1695.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

History

Published online: 1 July 2013
Published in print: Summer 2013

Authors

Details

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Focus

PPV Articles - Focus

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share