Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Articles
Published Online: 17 July 2015

Natural History of Poststroke Apathy During Acute Rehabilitation

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

To better understand the natural history of poststroke apathy, the authors tested 96 patients undergoing acute rehabilitation for stroke using the Apathy Inventory. A total of 28% of patients had apathy. Their Apathy Inventory scores improved a mean of 1 point by week 2 and 2 points by week 3, with the majority of patients remaining apathetic at discharge. Apathy severity correlated with aphasia, weakness, and impaired cognition but did not correlate with depression. These findings suggest that acute rehabilitation is an optimal setting for clinical trials for poststroke apathy, because apathy is associated with poor outcomes and shows only a small degree of spontaneous improvement.
Apathy is a syndrome of reduced goal-directed behavior, reduced goal-directed cognition, and flattened emotion. Apathy is present in approximately 35% of patients with stroke and is associated with worse outcomes and lower quality of life.1 Apathy in patients undergoing acute rehabilitation is also associated with a need for nursing home placement as well as related higher costs of care.2,3
There are currently no proven treatments for poststroke apathy. Ideally, treatment would begin soon after stroke, because apathy limits patients’ ability to participate in the intense rehabilitation programs typically offered only in the first months after stroke. To appropriately power trials in this period, we first need data on prevalence and natural history. Prevalence data are limited to patients with minimal cognitive and language deficits. A recent meta-analysis4 reported that only two studies included patients with aphasia; of these studies, one only had five patients with aphasia5 and the other did not use a formal measure of apathy.6 Regarding natural history, published studies typically began at 1 month after the stroke, and these studies suggest that apathy is typically persistent and is associated with less recovery in activities of daily living and cognition.3,79
To determine how the severity of apathy changes in the first weeks after stroke, we prospectively studied patients admitted to an American acute rehabilitation unit for ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke. We defined apathy based on clinician reports using the Apathy Inventory–Clinician (AI-C) version.10 This scale is based completely on observed behavior, allowing us to include patients with aphasia.
On the basis of published studies of apathy prevalence in different periods after stroke,1 we hypothesized that apathy severity would slightly improve during acute rehabilitation. We also sought to determine the relationship between poststroke apathy and depression. Finally, we performed secondary analyses to identify associations of apathy with other clinical factors such as aphasia, cognition, motor impairment, and rate of recovery from disability.

Methods

Subjects

From November 2013 to May 2014, we studied consecutively admitted patients undergoing acute rehabilitation for an ischemic or a hemorrhagic stroke that occurred within 30 days of study enrollment. All patients had some impairment from their stroke that prevented them from returning home after their hospitalization (typically a deficit in mobility with or without other deficits).
We excluded patients who could have had apathy from causes other than stroke, such as a neurodegenerative condition, dementia (defined as a score >3.4 on the Informant Questionnaire of Cognitive Decline in the Elderly11), or a medication that acted on dopamine (e.g., treatment for psychosis, nausea, or attention). We also excluded patients who were not independent before their current stroke (e.g., required assistance for mobility due to previous stroke or musculoskeletal disorder), because this would affect interpretation of outcome measures. We excluded patients admitted for <7 days, because this would give inadequate time to conduct our assessments. In addition, some patients were initially hypoaroused (i.e., required stimulation to maintain eye opening) as a result of stroke or medical issues; thus, we only studied them once or if they became consistently fully alert.
All measures reported here were included as part of the clinical record. The Burke Rehabilitation Hospital Institutional Review Board approved this analysis of the data, and procedures were followed in accordance with institutional guidelines.

Primary Outcome Measure

We used the AI-C to determine the prevalence and natural history of apathy.10 The AI-C asks the clinician to report on the following: 1) emotional blunting, 2) loss of initiative (i.e., goal-directed behavior), and 3) loss of interest in activities and other people. Each question is scored on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (no problem) to 4 (major problem), with a total score ranging from 0 to 12. We chose this measure because it allows an assessment of apathy solely based on observed behavior, which enabled us to include patients with aphasia. The more commonly used Apathy Scale12 and the Apathy Evaluation Scale13 assess patients’ internal states, which cannot be done in patients with aphasia. In addition, these more commonly used scales contain questions that are difficult to interpret for patients admitted to an inpatient rehabilitation unit.
The patients’ primary speech-language pathologist performed the AI-C starting 1 week after admission and weekly thereafter. The speech-language pathologists were asked to score based on behaviors observed during the entire previous week. For prevalence data, for patients without aphasia we report the AI-C performed closest in time to the measures of cognition and depression; for patients with aphasia we report the first AI-C.

Secondary Measures and Associated Features

We interviewed patients with intact language using the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (0–60, with higher scores representing worse depression) to quantify depressive symptoms.14
Patients with intact language performed version 1 of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (0–30, with lower scores indicating worse cognition) to measure overall cognition.15 We converted these scores to a z score based on a large reported sample, taking into account age and education.16
We used the FIM score (18–126, with lower scores representing more disability; UB Foundation Activities, Inc., Buffalo, NY) to determine the level of disability from admission (first 3 days) to discharge (final 3 days). We used the change in the total FIM as well as the FIM efficiency (change in total FIM score divided by length of stay) to measure improvement from admission to discharge. We used a line cancellation test (≤34 of 36 considered abnormal) to determine the presence of neglect.17 Finally, we used the Fugl-Meyer assessment to determine motor function.18

Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB software (MathWorks, Natick, MA) using built-in and Statistics Toolbox codes. Gaussian-distributed data are reported as means and standard deviations; non-Gaussian–distributed data are reported as medians and interquartile ranges. The AI-C was treated as an ordinal variable because it is not a linear measure of apathy severity. We used the Mann-Whitney U test for correlation of the AI-C with binary variables, producing a z score and a p value. We used the Spearman’s rank-order correlation coefficient for correlation with continuous variables, producing a coefficient ranging from −1 to 1 and a p value. Box plots were created using MATLAB’s “boxplot.m” with default parameters. The thick lines within the gray boxes represent the median, whereas the edges of the gray boxes represent the 25th to 75th percentiles; whiskers extend to the most extreme data points not considered outliers; outliers (crosses) are those values outside the 25th to 75th percentiles by +1.5 times the interquartile range, corresponding to approximately 99.3% coverage if the data are normally distributed.

Results

We screened 257 patients who were consecutively admitted to Burke Rehabilitation Hospital with a diagnosis of ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke. We excluded 161 of these patients based on criteria defined in the Methods, resulting in 96 patients eligible for this study.
Patients included in the study had a mean age of 72.8 years (SD=13.9), 38.5% were men, the median length of hospital stay was 24.5 days (interquartile range=13.5), and 15 patients (16%) had a hemorrhagic stroke. Nineteen patients (20%) had aphasia and thus were not testable with the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Overall, patients were moderately disabled (mean admission FIM score=44.9 [SD=17.1]), and they were moderately weak (mean upper extremity Fugl-Meyer score=37.3 [SD=23.7], N=59; and mean lower extremity Fugl-Meyer score=21.3 [SD=9.4], N=65). Patients were also cognitively impaired (mean Montreal Cognitive Assessment z score=−1.11 [SD=2.0], N=76), with 26% of subjects 2 SD below the mean.

Apathy Severity

On the basis of the AI-C score from the treating speech-language pathologist, we found that 27 patients (28%) had apathy (AI-C score ≥4, with at least one of the three dimensions ≥2). Apathy severity was not normally distributed, because 42 patients (44%) had a score of zero (i.e., no signs of apathy) (Figure 1).
FIGURE 1. Apathy Severity Across All 96 Patientsa
a The median score is 1, with a 25th percentile of 0 and a 75th percentile of 4. AI-C, Apathy Inventory–Clinician.
We explored the relationships among the three subscores of the AI-C (emotional blunting, loss of interest, and loss of initiative) to determine whether patients presented with separate syndromes. We found that the scores generally correlated highly, because only seven of the 96 subjects had a subscore >1 point away from another subscore.

Change Across Time

We documented apathy severity weekly, although patients had varied lengths of hospital stay; this resulted in fewer data points at the longer time durations. Patients without apathy at week one generally had no change in their apathy score over their stay (Figure 2A), though some worsened slightly with five developing apathy at some point during their stay. Patients with apathy at week 1 (AI-C ≥4) typically demonstrated improvement in apathy severity, although this improvement was only to a small degree (Figure 2B). Specifically, from week 1 to week 2, patients with apathy had a mean change of −1.04 (SD=1.73) in their AI-C score, with five of the 24 patients showing resolution of apathy (AI-C <4). From weeks 1 to 3, patients with apathy had a mean change of −2.40 (SD=2.03) in their AI-C score, with seven of the 15 patients showing resolution of apathy. From weeks 1 to 4, patients with apathy had a mean change of −2.14 (SD=2.03) in their AI-C score, with one of the seven patients showing resolution of apathy.
FIGURE 2. Changes in AI-C Scoresa
a Changes in AI-C scores as measured weekly during rehabilitation stay for (A, C, and E) patients with no apathy (AI-C <4) at week 1 and (B, D, and F) patients with apathy (AI-C ≥4) at week 1. Box plots are shown for the following: (A and B) all subjects, (C and D) patients without aphasia, and (E and F) patients with aphasia. See the Methods for a description of the box plot. AI-C, Apathy Inventory–Clinician.

Correlations With Other Clinical Features

Aphasia correlated significantly with apathy severity (Figure 3 and Table 1). Using an AI-C cutoff value of 4 to define apathy, we determined that 10 (53%) of the 19 patients with aphasia were apathetic, whereas only 17 (22%) of the 77 patients without aphasia were apathetic. Regarding a change in apathy severity, patients with and without aphasia both showed a small degree of improvement during the first 2 weeks, but only the patients without aphasia improved during the subsequent weeks (Figure 2, D and F). Patients with aphasia were also less likely to have resolution of apathy. In patients who stayed at least 2 weeks, seven of 14 patients without aphasia had resolution of apathy compared with only three of 10 patients with aphasia.
FIGURE 3. Box Plot of Apathy Severity in Patients Without Aphasia Compared With Patients With Aphasiaa
a See the Methods for description of the box plot. AI-C, Apathy Inventory–Clinician.
TABLE 1. Correlations Between Apathy Inventory–Clinician Score and Clinical Variablesa
VariableNMann-Whitney U Test z ScoreSpearman Correlation Coefficient
Binary measures   
 Aphasia963.29** 
 Gender960.07 
 Neglect842.09* 
 Discharge to nursing home923.07** 
Continuous measures   
 Age at admission96 –0.04
 Days since stroke96 0.18
 Years of education76 –0.06
 MADRS score77 0.16
 FIM at admission96 –0.54**
 FIM change89 –0.29**
 FIM efficiency89 –0.38**
 MoCA z score76 –0.37**
 Fugl-Meyer total score at discharge48 –0.51**
a
Because of time constraints and unplanned discharges, treating therapists were only able to perform the Fugl-Meyer assessment, neglect testing, and discharge FIM on a subset of our patients (numbers studied are listed in the table). One patient refused the MoCA testing. FIM, Functional Independence Measure; MADRS, Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment.
*p<0.05. **p<0.01.
We measured depression severity using the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale in the 77 patients without aphasia. Based on a published cutoff value of 18,19 15 (19.5%) of the 77 patients were depressed. We found no correlation between the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale and AI-C scores (Figure 4 and Table 1).
FIGURE 4. Correlation Between Apathy Severity and Depression Severitya
a Dashed lines represent published cutoff points for defining depression/apathy. AI-C, Apathy Inventory–Clinician; MADRS, Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale.
Regarding other clinical variables, apathy severity had a significant correlation with the following: overall cognition (Montreal Cognitive Assessment z score); disability (admission FIM score); change in FIM score from admission to discharge, whether measured directly or divided by length of stay (FIM efficiency); presence of neglect (line cancellation); and motor impairment (Fugl-Meyer score) at discharge (Table 1). Patients with apathy were also more likely to go to a nursing home after discharge. Gender, age, days since stroke onset, and years of education did not have statistically significant associations with apathy severity.

Discussion

We found that apathy was present in 28% of patients undergoing inpatient acute rehabilitation for an ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke. We further found that apathy improved only modestly during the acute rehabilitation stay (a 1-point decrease by week 2 and a 2-point decrease by week 3) and the majority of patients with apathy remained apathetic at discharge. To our knowledge, this study is the first that assesses the change in apathy severity during the first few weeks after stroke and one of the few prevalence studies that include patients with aphasia.
Our prevalence of poststroke apathy is lower than that reported in the literature. In five studies of a similar time range after stroke (10–50 days), the mean prevalence of apathy was 36% (95% confidence interval, 30%−42%).1 These studies excluded patients with aphasia; in our data set, patients without aphasia had a prevalence of 22%. Our finding of a lower prevalence of apathy may be attributable to the use of a different scale (the AI-C in this study versus the Apathy Scale used in most of the reported studies) and to our choice of a cutoff value of 4 points. Future studies would be better served by defining apathy based on consensus criteria,20,21 rather than using arbitrary cutoff points on scales.
We found a markedly higher prevalence of apathy in patients with aphasia compared with patients without aphasia (53% versus 22%, respectively). We were only able to find one other study that included a significant number of patients with aphasia.6 Carota et al.6 studied 273 patients in the first days after stroke and found an overall apathy prevalence of 48%, with no difference in the 90 patients with aphasia. It is unclear why the results differed, although nurses in the study by Carota et al.6 reported the presence of apathy, whereas speech-language pathologists in our study evaluated patients and used a validated scale. It is also unclear why patients with aphasia had a higher prevalence of apathy. This could relate to the lesion location or total lesion burden. Future studies would benefit from the inclusion of detailed imaging data.
Patients with apathy showed a slight improvement in apathy severity during the course of their stay in the rehabilitation hospital (2–4 weeks), although most patients with apathy at week 1 remained apathetic at discharge. The few studies that provide longitudinal data on poststroke apathy began assessment at least 1 month after stroke. Most studies found that apathy is rather stable during the 6–15 months poststroke,3,5,7 although one study showed that apathy lasts on average for only 6 months.8
We found no correlation between apathy severity and depression severity in the subset of patients who were tested with the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale, which requires intact language. Previous studies have reported both significant and nonsignificant associations of these conditions.1 The inconsistency in the literature may be attributable to the variety of scales used to assess apathy and depression, many of which have overlapping questions. In our study, the apathy measure was based solely on observed apathetic and not dysphoric-type behaviors, although our depression measure (Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale) does include two self-report measures linked to apathy (lassitude and inability to feel).
We also found that apathy severity correlated with impaired cognition (Montreal Cognitive Assessment score), disability (FIM score), neglect (line cancellation), and weakness (Fugl-Meyer score). Associations between apathy and worse cognition have been a relatively consistent finding in the apathy literature.1 Most studies also found a similar association between apathy and increased disability, although they varied in methods of assessment.1 Our study also confirmed our previous finding that patients with worse apathy severity are more likely to be discharged from the inpatient rehabilitation hospital to a nursing home, rather than to their home.2 Overall, these associated symptoms of poststroke apathy suggest that treatment of apathy may improve outcomes in a variety of domains22; however, with our correlational data, it is not possible to dissociate the effect of apathy from severity of stroke.
This study has some limitations. First, it is challenging to measure apathy in patients with aphasia because language is typically our best insight into a patient’s goal-directed cognition and emotion. We attempted to ameliorate this by asking the evaluating speech-language pathologists to comment on nonverbal communication by the patients. In addition, we did not measure the presence of apathy before stroke, so it is possible that some of our patients had apathy as a lifelong personality trait. A final limitation is that our study lacked a test of interrater reliability of apathy severity. We attempted to conduct a test of reliability between speech-language pathologists who saw their own patients compared with the speech-language pathologists who saw the patients in a group therapy session; however, our pilot work showed that patients’ level of motivation and affect varied between the two settings and thus could not be an accurate test of interrater reliability.
In summary, we found that apathy is common in the first weeks after stroke in patients undergoing acute rehabilitation, and shows only a small degree of improvement during this period. We further found that apathy is associated with worse cognitive dysfunction, neglect, aphasia, worse motor function, more overall disability, and a slower rate of recovery. Clinically, these findings suggest that we should follow up on patients with apathy, especially those with more severe apathy, because they typically do not improve and would likely benefit from treatment. Our results also indicate that the acute rehabilitation setting is ideal for conducting clinical trials for poststroke apathy treatment because apathy is common, is associated with poor outcomes, and shows only a small degree of spontaneous improvement.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Janet Herbold and the speech-language pathologists at Burke Rehabilitation Hospital for assistance with acquiring the data.

References

1.
van Dalen JW, Moll van Charante EP, Nederkoorn PJ, et al: Poststroke apathy. Stroke 2013; 44:851–860
2.
Harris AL, Elder J, Schiff ND, et al: Post-stroke apathy and hypersomnia lead to worse outcomes from acute rehabilitation. Transl Stroke Res 2014; 5:292–300
3.
Mayo NE, Fellows LK, Scott SC, et al: A longitudinal view of apathy and its impact after stroke. Stroke 2009; 40:3299–3307
4.
Caeiro L, Ferro JM, Costa J: Apathy secondary to stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cerebrovasc Dis 2013; 35:23–39
5.
Castellanos-Pinedo F, Hernández-Pérez JM, Zurdo M, et al: Influence of premorbid psychopathology and lesion location on affective and behavioral disorders after ischemic stroke. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2011; 23:340–347
6.
Carota A, Berney A, Aybek S, et al: A prospective study of predictors of poststroke depression. Neurology 2005; 64:428–433
7.
Withall A, Brodaty H, Altendorf A, et al: A longitudinal study examining the independence of apathy and depression after stroke: the Sydney Stroke Study. Int Psychogeriatr 2011; 23:264–273
8.
Mikami K, Jorge RE, Moser DJ, et al: Incident apathy during the first year after stroke and its effect on physical and cognitive recovery. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2013; 21:848–854
9.
Santa N, Sugimori H, Kusuda K, et al: Apathy and functional recovery following first-ever stroke. Int J Rehabil Res 2008; 31:321–326
10.
Robert PH, Clairet S, Benoit M, et al: The apathy inventory: assessment of apathy and awareness in Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002; 17:1099–1105
11.
Jorm AF: The Informant Questionnaire on cognitive decline in the elderly (IQCODE): a review. Int Psychogeriatr 2004; 16:275–293
12.
Starkstein SE, Mayberg HS, Preziosi TJ, et al: Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1992; 4:134–139
13.
Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S: Reliability and validity of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. Psychiatry Res 1991; 38:143–162
14.
Montgomery SA, Asberg M: A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry 1979; 134:382–389
15.
Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al: The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53:695–699
16.
Rossetti HC, Lacritz LH, Cullum CM, et al: Normative data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population-based sample. Neurology 2011; 77:1272–1275
17.
Wilson BA, Cockburn J, Halligan P: Behavioural Inattention Test: Manual. Suffolk, UK, Thames Valley Test Company, 1987
18.
Fugl-Meyer AR, Jääskö L, Leyman I, et al: The post-stroke hemiplegic patient. 1. a method for evaluation of physical performance. Scand J Rehabil Med 1975; 7:13–31
19.
Williams JB, Kobak KA: Development and reliability of a structured interview guide for the Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale (SIGMA). Br J Psychiatry 2008; 192:52–58
20.
Robert P, Onyike CU, Leentjens AF, et al: Proposed diagnostic criteria for apathy in Alzheimer’s disease and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Eur Psychiatry 2009; 24:98–104
21.
Mulin E, Leone E, Dujardin K, et al: Diagnostic criteria for apathy in clinical practice. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2011; 26:158–165
22.
Goldfine AM, Schiff ND: What is the role of brain mechanisms underlying arousal in recovery of motor function after structural brain injuries? Curr Opin Neurol 2011; 24:564–569

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 333 - 338
PubMed: 26185903

History

Received: 1 January 2015
Revision received: 3 March 2015
Accepted: 24 March 2015
Published online: 17 July 2015
Published in print: Fall 2015

Authors

Details

Juliana M. Kennedy, B.S.
From the Burke Rehabilitation Center, White Plains, N.Y. (JMK, DAG, AMG); and Stony Brook Medicine Department of Neurology, Stony Brook, N.Y. (AMG).
Dora A. Granato, M.S., C.C.C.-S.L.P.
From the Burke Rehabilitation Center, White Plains, N.Y. (JMK, DAG, AMG); and Stony Brook Medicine Department of Neurology, Stony Brook, N.Y. (AMG).
Andrew M. Goldfine, M.D.
From the Burke Rehabilitation Center, White Plains, N.Y. (JMK, DAG, AMG); and Stony Brook Medicine Department of Neurology, Stony Brook, N.Y. (AMG).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Goldfine; e-mail: [email protected]
Presented at the American Neurological Association Annual Meeting, Philadelphia, PA, April 26–May 3, 2014.

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development10.13039/100000071: K23HD079556
Research reported in this publication was supported by the Burke Medical Research Institute and by award K23HD079556 from the NIH Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.Funded in part by the Harvey Goodstein Charitable Foundation, as well as Gary, Sari, and Annie Roboff in memory of their mother, Phyllis Roboff.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share