Skip to main content
Full access
Clinical and Research Reports
Published Online: 7 October 2019

Impaired Action Control in Patients With Functional Movement Disorders

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Objective:

Despite being a major cause of neurological disability, the neural mechanisms of functional movement disorders (FMDs) remain poorly understood. Recent studies suggest that FMD is linked to dysfunctional motor and prefrontal regions that could lead to motor and cognitive impairments. The aim of this study was to investigate different components of action control in FMD by using choice-reaction, stop-signal, and Simon tasks.

Methods:

Thirty patients with an FMD were prospectively recruited from the University of Louisville Movement Disorders Clinic and compared with 53 healthy control subjects, recruited from the Vanderbilt University Medical Center Movement Disorders Clinic. FMD motor symptom severity was rated with the Simplified Functional Movement Disorder Rating Scale (S-FMDRS). By using a computer and handheld response grips, participants completed three action-control tasks (choice-reaction task, stop-signal task, and Simon task) that tested action initiation, action cancelation, and interference control over actions. Action-control measures were compared between groups with analyses of variance.

Results:

Patients with FMD were less proficient in suppressing incorrect response impulses on the Simon task and were slower to stop on the stop-signal task compared with healthy control subjects. No significant correlation with neuropsychological measurements, S-FMDRS scores, and action-control measurements was observed.

Conclusions:

These results suggest that two forms of inhibitory control, selective impulse inhibition and global action cancelation, are impaired in patients with FMD, independent of slowing on go reaction times. Improved understanding of action control in FMD may help in the development of new diagnostic and therapeutic strategies.
The pathophysiologic basis of involuntary motor symptoms in functional movement disorders (FMDs) remains poorly understood. There is emerging evidence from recent neuroimaging studies linking FMD to aberrant activity and morphology in prefrontal and basal ganglia circuitries (17). While these cortical-striatal abnormalities may be connected to the expression of aberrant movements, such as tremor and dystonia, dysfunction of prefrontal-basal ganglia circuitries is also responsible for deficits in the cognitive control of movement. Specifically, a network connecting the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal cortex, presupplementary motor area, striatum, and subthalamic nuclei is implicated directly in the inhibitory control of movement (for a review, see Aron et al [8]), which appears to be an intriguing system to investigate in FMD. It is noteworthy that Aron et al. (8) associated both stopping control and conflict control with frontal-striatal circuits. The aim of the present study was to investigate both types of control in patients with FMD.
A go-no-go task was used in two previous investigations reporting that individuals with FMD committed higher rates of errors on no-go trials (i.e., they reacted when they should have withheld a response), which suggests that FMD interferes with inhibitory control in situations with a strong response bias (5, 9).
In the present study, we investigated whether FMD exerts a dissociable effect on two forms of inhibitory control: the latency to stop an intentionally initiated action (action cancelation) and the proficiency in inhibiting unintentional action impulses (interference control).
To test this, participants performed three well-established cognitive tasks: a choice reaction task, a stop-signal task, and a Simon task. A basic two-choice reaction task quantified action initiation speed. The stop-signal task also provides an estimate of action initiation speed and a measure of action cancelation speed (stop-signal reaction time). Slower stopping latencies reflect increased difficulty in inhibiting ongoing intentional actions (10). Finally, the Simon task provides measures of conflict between goal-directed actions and automatic (unintentional) action impulses (11). The Simon task calls for a left or right response to a particular feature of a spatially lateralized stimulus (i.e., arrow direction). Reduced interference control is reflected by slower and less accurate responses when stimulus location and the action signaled by the arrow direction do not correspond (i.e., right hand response to a right-pointing arrow in the left visual half-field), compared with when they correspond.
We hypothesized that the dysfunction of prefrontal-basal ganglia circuits in patients with FMD would interfere with inhibitory motor control. Our first prediction was that the latency to inhibit an intentionally initiated movement would be slower among patients with FMD (slower action cancelation in the stop-signal task) compared with healthy control subjects. The demonstration that patients with FMD are slower to stop intentionally initiated movements could have interesting implications clinically. Second, we predicted that the inhibitory dysfunction in patients with FMD might be related to disruption in the ability to inhibit unintentionally triggered action impulses. This would imply that patients with FMD have difficulty inhibiting unintentionally activated movements (reduced interference control as measured by the Simon task).

Methods

Participants

Patients with FMD (N=30) were diagnosed using criteria by Williams et al. (12). Diagnosis was made by a board-certified neurologist (K.L.), and participants were recruited from the Movement Disorders Clinic at the University of Louisville Physicians group. Motor severity was evaluated in FMD patients using the Simplified Functional Movement Disorders Rating Scale (S-FMDRS) (13). Healthy control subjects (N=53) were recruited via community advertisement at the Vanderbilt University Movement Disorders Clinic (21 healthy control subjects performed the Simon task, and another 32 performed the stop and choice reaction tasks). Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study participants are summarized in Table 1.
TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics and action-control performance by study groupa
 Healthy control groupFunctional movement disorder (FMD) group (N=30)
 Simon task (N=21)Stop-signal task (N=32)All tasks
CharacteristicMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Age (years)46.911.468.010.3    47.314.79
Education (years)16.42.015.33.714.32.6
Symptom duration (years)    6.310.8
Symptom severityb    18.49.1
PHQ-15 somatic symptoms    12.45.7
GAD-7 anxiety    7.15.4
PHQ-9 depression    10.26.7
Choice-reaction time task      
 Choice reaction time (ms)  47268575153
 Choice accuracy (%)  973964
Stop-signal task      
 Go accuracy (%)  984983
 Go reaction time (ms)  611108801138
 Stop-signal reaction time (ms)  25053299117
 Signal-response reaction time (ms)  52892705143
 Signal-response rate (%)  483454
 SSD (ms)  331124477195
Simon task      
 C accuracy (%)9610  974
 Nc accuracy (%)9412  929
 C reaction time (ms)45875  611121
 Nc reaction time (ms)48777  653118
 Final delta slope–0.130.28  0.050.28
 First bin NC accuracy (%)8420  8218
a
Patients in the FMD group performed all three tasks: choice-reaction time task, stop-signal task, and Simon task, while healthy control subjects only performed the latter two. The male:female ratio for healthy control subjects who performed the Simon task, healthy control subjects who performed the stop-signal task, and patients in the FMD group was 6:15, 13:19, and 6:24, respectively. C accuracy=go/no-go accuracy, GAD-7=Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 scale, Nc=noncorresponding, PHQ-9=Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (depressive symptoms), PHQ-15=Patient Health Questionnaire-15, SSD=stop-signal delay.
b
Determined by using the Simplified Functional Movement Disorder Rating Scale.
Individuals with medical, neurological, or psychiatric comorbidities known to interfere with cognition were excluded on the basis of a standardized health eligibility questionnaire. A Montreal Cognitive Assessment score of 22 was used as the lower limit of cognitive performance for inclusion in the study.
Participants provided informed consent before participation in any study procedures, in full compliance with the standards of ethical conduct in human investigation as regulated by the institutional review boards of Vanderbilt University and the University of Louisville.

Experimental Tasks and Procedures

Participants completed a choice-reaction task, a stop-signal task, and a Simon task, measuring action initiation, action cancelation, and interference control, respectively. A detailed description of each task is provided in the online supplement. All study subjects completed the choice-reaction task first and subsequently performed the stop-signal and Simon tasks. Patients with FMD also completed three questionnaires to rate the severity of somatic symptoms: the Patient Health Questionnaire-15 (PHQ-15) (14), the Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7) scale (15), and the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) (depressive symptoms) (16).

Data Analyses

Choice-reaction task.

We compared go reaction times and accuracy rates from the choice-reaction task between the study groups.

Stop-signal task.

Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed in order to compare go reaction times and stop-signal reaction times from the stop-signal task as a function of group (FMD and healthy control subjects) to test whether action initiation and action cancelation were reduced in patients with FMD compared with control subjects. Stop-signal reaction times were estimated by using the horse-race model and integration method (17). Participants whose performance violated the horse-race model (<35% stop success, signal response reaction time larger than the mean go reaction time) were excluded from stop-task analysis (N=3).

Simon task.

Reaction time latencies for corresponding and noncorresponding trials faster than 150 ms (anticipatory reactions) were excluded. For each level of correspondence (i.e., corresponding and noncorresponding), mean reaction times and log-transformed accuracy rates were calculated to approximate normal distribution. Participants with >50% omission or commission errors on corresponding trials were excluded from the Simon analysis (N=4).
Similar to our previous work using the Simon task (18, 19), we used the dual process activation suppression (DPAS) model to dissociate fast response impulses and inhibition of incorrect responses. Distributional analyses were implemented by rank-ordering single-trial reaction times from fastest to slowest and by dividing reaction times into six equal-sized bins.
According to the DPAS model, impulse capture is measured by plotting the accuracy rates against the reaction time for each level of correspondence (conditional accuracy function). The proportion of fast errors on noncorresponding trials (i.e., within fastest reaction time bin) reflects the strength of the incorrect response capture (20).
The DPAS also allows measurement of the proficiency of inhibitory control over time by means of delta plots, which show the size of the Simon effect on reaction time as a function of the reaction time. According to the DPAS model, when an incorrect response has been triggered by noncorresponding stimulus information, it takes time for inhibitory control to build up. Therefore, the proficiency of inhibitory control is most clearly reflected at the slope of the interference reduction between the final two (slowest) reaction time bins of the delta plot (delta slope) (21). A flat or positive-going delta slope indicates reduced interference control, which would be predicted for patients with FMD compared with control subjects.
We first compared reaction times and accuracy rates from the Simon task by means of an ANOVA, within-subjects factor correspondence (corresponding, noncorresponding), and between-subjects factor group (FMD patients, healthy control subjects). Second, we performed ANOVAs to compare final delta slopes and fast impulsive errors as a function of group.

Correlations Between Cognitive Performance and Clinical Measures

We also correlated ratings of patients’ clinical motor symptom severity (measured with the S-FMDRS), depression, anxiety, and somatic severity with measures of action control (go reaction time, stop-signal reaction times, delta slopes, and impulsive errors).

Results

Demographic characteristics and clinical variables of the study participants, as well as group effects on mean reaction times and accuracy rates of the action-control tasks, are summarized in Table 1.

Participants’ Demographic Characteristics

Predominant symptoms of patients with FMD were present with the following frequencies: tremor (53.3%), dystonia (20%), gait disorder (10%), myoclonus (10%), and tics (6.7%).
Healthy control subjects who performed the choice-reaction and stop-signal tasks were similar in education level (p>0.1) but older in age (F=36.3, df=1, 58, p<0.05) and more likely to be male (χ2=10.0, df=1, p<0.05) compared with patients in the FMD group. Healthy control subjects who performed the Simon task were similar in age and sex distribution (all p values >0.2) but had more years of education compared with patients in the FMD group (F=11.2, df=1, 45, p<0.05).

Choice-Reaction Time Task (Action Initiation)

Patients with FMD were significantly slower to initiate actions than healthy control subjects, as evidenced by longer go reaction times on the choice-reaction time task and the stop-signal task (choice-reaction time task: F=11.79, df=1, 57, p<0.01, mFMD=575 ms, mHC=472 ms).

Stop-Signal Task (Action Cancelation)

Patients with FMD were significantly slower to initiate actions than healthy control subjects, as evidenced by longer go reaction times on the stop-signal task (Fstop=35.16, df=1, 58, p<0.001, mFMD=801 ms, mHC=611 ms). Patients with FMD were also slower to stop voluntary actions (slower action cancelation), expressed by longer stop-signal reaction times (stop-signal reaction times; F=4.6, df=1, 58, p<0.05 mFMD=299 ms, mHC=250 ms) (Figure 1). Go reaction times and stop-signal reaction times were independent in both groups (i.e., go reaction times and stop-signal reaction times did not correlate in either group, rFMD=0.17, p=0.4; rHC=0.17, p=0.35), indicating independence of action initiation and action cancelation.
FIGURE 1. Trial examples and performance on action-control tasks for patients with functional movement disorder (FMD) and healthy control (HC) subjectsa
a Panel A shows trial examples of the stop-signal task in which participants were instructed to respond to the direction of the dark gray arrow with a left or right button press and to stop their response when the arrow turned purple (shown as light gray here) (30% of trials). Panel B shows the Simon task in which participants manually responded to the direction of the gray arrows presented on the screen with a left or right button press, irrespective of the location of the arrow. Panel C shows the conditional accuracy functions for noncorresponding (Nc) trial types. Errors are predominantly associated with the fastest reaction times on Nc trials, both in the patient and HC groups. Panel D shows the Simon effect across reaction time bins (delta plot); FMD patients were significantly less proficient at suppressing interference compared with HC subjects (i.e., more positive-going delta slope at the slow end of the distribution). Panel E shows the mean stop-signal reaction time. FMD patients demonstrated significantly slower stop reaction times compared with HC subjects. Panel F shows the mean go reaction time for the stop-signal task; FMD patients were significantly slower compared with HC subjects. Error bars represent standard errors. C=corresponding trial types.

Simon Task (Interference Control)

Performance on noncorresponding (Nc) trials was slower and less accurate than on corresponding trials (reaction time: F=75.69, df=1, 45, p<0.001; accuracy: F=20.26, df=1, 45, p<0.001), but there was no interaction with group (F values <2.6, df=1, 45, p values >0.12). Across Simon conditions, patients with FMD were slower to respond than control subjects but equally accurate (reaction time: F=28.67, df=1, 45, p<0.001; accuracy: F<1.0, df=1, 45, p >0.9).
Moreover, the delta-slope analysis demonstrated that the proficiency of inhibitory control was significantly impaired in the FMD group (F=4.56, df=1, 45, p<0.05) (Figure 1). That is, the inhibition slope (i.e., the delta slope between the last two bins of the reaction time distribution) in patients with FMD (m=0.05) was positive going compared with the inhibition slope among healthy control subjects (m=–0.13), which reflects poor inhibition of involuntary action impulses (poor interference control) among patients with FMD. Response impulse-capture rates (accuracy rates in the fastest reaction time bin of the Nc trials), on the other hand, were similar between the two study groups (F=0.02, df=1, 45, p=0.87) (Figure 1).

Correlations Between Behavior and Clinical Measures

Increased somatic severity on the PHQ-15 was related to slower stopping reaction times (p=0.01, r=0.51); however, this finding did not hold for Bonferroni correction. No other significant correlations were found between clinical and action-control variables (r values <0.39, p values >0.12).

Discussion

Individuals with FMD present with aberrant movements, such as tremor and dystonia. There is considerable debate regarding the origins of these movements, including whether they are intentional or unintentional. We investigated how FMD affects the ability to inhibit intentionally initiated movements (action cancelation as measured by the stop-signal task) and the ability to inhibit unintentionally (automatically) activated motor impulses (interference control as measured by the Simon task), which represent dissociable inhibitory control systems. Participants with FMD were globally slower to react than healthy control subjects. Independent of this motor slowing, FMD was associated with slower stopping latencies on the stop-signal task when trying to inhibit an intentionally initiated action, as well as with less proficient inhibition of unintentionally triggered motor impulses, as evidenced by the delta slope in the Simon task. Thus, FMD appears to have a global effect on forms of inhibitory motor control, irrespective of the putative sources of the involuntary movement disorder associated with the condition. The presence of inhibitory control dysfunction is consistent with imaging studies showing altered frontal-basal ganglia circuitry in FMD (16), although future studies need to establish a direct link.

Limitations

There are several limitations to this study. The healthy control subjects who performed the stop-signal task were not age matched with the patients in the FMD group. However, the significant impairment in stopping speed observed in the FMD group compared with that found among older healthy control subjects would likely be even more pronounced in contrast to a younger control group. Another limitation is that patients in the FMD group were tested while they were taking their regular medications (e.g., antidepressants, analgesics, muscle relaxants), and thus the impact of these drugs on performance could not be quantified. Moreover, healthy control subjects were not tested regarding anxiety and depression, and therefore the results of the FMD group can exclusively be contrasted with a normative group only. Anxiety and depression scores were respectively mild and moderate and did not correlate with any of the cognitive measures. However, task errors could have been processed differentially in the FMD group as a result of anxiety or mood problems, which could influence action control on a trial-by-trial basis. Future studies should explore the influences of mood and anxiety on action control in a larger sample.

Conclusions

This study suggests that patients with FMD are significantly delayed in their latencies to stop intentionally initiated movements and in their proficiency to inhibit unintentional motor impulses. Clinically, an improved understanding of how FMD affects action-control processes might advance evaluation techniques and diagnostic criteria and pave the way for the development of more effective therapies.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.neuropsych.19030076.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Boeckle M, Liegl G, Jank R, et al: Neural correlates of conversion disorder: overview and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on motor conversion disorder. BMC Psychiatry 2016; 16:195
2.
Conejero I, Thouvenot E, Abbar M, et al: Neuroanatomy of conversion disorder: towards a network approach. Rev Neurosci 2018; 29:355–368
3.
Edwards MJ: Neurobiologic theories of functional neurologic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol 2017; 139:131–137
4.
Voon V, Ekanayake V, Wiggs E, et al: Response inhibition in motor conversion disorder. Mov Disord 2013; 28:612–618
5.
Vuilleumier P: Brain circuits implicated in psychogenic paralysis in conversion disorders and hypnosis. Neurophysiol Clin 2014; 44:323–337
6.
Perez DL, Dworetzky BA, Dickerson BC, et al: An integrative neurocircuit perspective on psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and functional movement disorders: neural functional unawareness. Clin EEG Neurosci 2015; 46:4–15
7.
Maurer CW, LaFaver K, Limachia GS, et al: Gray matter differences in patients with functional movement disorders. Neurology 2018; 91:e1870–e1879
8.
Aron AR, Herz DM, Brown P, et al: Frontosubthalamic circuits for control of action and cognition. J Neurosci 2016; 36:11489–11495
9.
Hammond-Tooke GD, Grajeda FT, Macrorie H, et al: Response inhibition in patients with functional neurological symptom disorder. J Clin Neurosci 2018; 56:38–43
10.
Bissett PG, Logan GD: Balancing cognitive demands: control adjustments in the stop-signal paradigm. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2011; 37:392–404
11.
Kornblum S, Hasbroucq T, Osman A: Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility: a model and taxonomy. Psychol Rev 1990; 97:253–270
12.
Williams DT, Ford B, Fahn S: Phenomenology and psychopathology related to psychogenic movement disorders. Adv Neurol 1995; 65:231–257
13.
Nielsen G, Ricciardi L, Meppelink AM, et al: A simplified version of the Psychogenic Movement Disorders Rating Scale: The Simplified Functional Movement Disorders Rating Scale (S-FMDRS). Mov Disord Clin Pract (Hoboken) 2017; 4:710–716
14.
Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB: The PHQ-15: validity of a new measure for evaluating the severity of somatic symptoms. Psychosom Med 2002; 64:258–266
15.
Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, et al: A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med 2006; 166:1092–1097
16.
Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB: The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med 2001; 16:606–613
17.
Logan GD: On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users’ guide to the stop-signal paradigm; in Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language. Edited by Dagenbach D, Carr TH. San Diego, Academic Press, 1994, pp 189–239
18.
Wylie SA, Ridderinkhof KR, Bashore TR, et al: The effect of Parkinson’s disease on the dynamics of on-line and proactive cognitive control during action selection. J Cogn Neurosci 2010; 22:2058–2073
19.
Ridderinkhof KR: Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychol Res 2002; 66:312–323
20.
van den Wildenberg WP, Wylie SA, Forstmann BU, et al: To head or to heed? beyond the surface of selective action inhibition: a review. Front Hum Neurosci 2010; 4:222
21.
Levitt H: Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 1971; 49:2-, 467

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 73 - 78
PubMed: 31587628

History

Received: 25 March 2019
Revision received: 22 May 2019
Accepted: 25 June 2019
Published online: 7 October 2019
Published in print: Winter 2020

Keywords

  1. Functional Neurologic Disorders
  2. Neuropsychology
  3. Stop-signal Task
  4. Movement Disorders

Authors

Details

Nelleke C. van Wouwe, Ph.D. [email protected]
The Department of Neurosurgery (van Wouwe, Wylie) and the Department of Neurology (Mohanty, Lingaiah, LaFaver), University of Louisville, Louisville, Ky.; and the Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville (van Wouwe).
Diksha Mohanty, M.D.
The Department of Neurosurgery (van Wouwe, Wylie) and the Department of Neurology (Mohanty, Lingaiah, LaFaver), University of Louisville, Louisville, Ky.; and the Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville (van Wouwe).
Anushree Lingaiah, M.B.B.S.
The Department of Neurosurgery (van Wouwe, Wylie) and the Department of Neurology (Mohanty, Lingaiah, LaFaver), University of Louisville, Louisville, Ky.; and the Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville (van Wouwe).
Scott A. Wylie, Ph.D.
The Department of Neurosurgery (van Wouwe, Wylie) and the Department of Neurology (Mohanty, Lingaiah, LaFaver), University of Louisville, Louisville, Ky.; and the Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville (van Wouwe).
Kathrin LaFaver, M.D.
The Department of Neurosurgery (van Wouwe, Wylie) and the Department of Neurology (Mohanty, Lingaiah, LaFaver), University of Louisville, Louisville, Ky.; and the Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville (van Wouwe).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. van Wouwe ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share