Skip to main content
Full access
Windows to the Brain
Published Online: 30 July 2020

Stress-Related Growth: Building a More Resilient Brain

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The concept that challenging life events have the potential to facilitate positive change has been explored for centuries in the areas of philosophy, theology, and the arts. However, scientific examination of stress-related growth (SRG) started in the 1990s (posttraumatic growth [PTG], thriving, finding benefits, adversarial growth) (2024). Research over the past half-century has also established that high levels of stress increase risk for development of both psychiatric and medical conditions (25). The growing recognition that individual outcomes following exposures to challenging events are quite diverse (i.e., ranging from highly detrimental to highly beneficial) led to the development of resilience as a broader framework (5, 7, 2628). In this context, resilience is conceptualized as a dynamic multifactorial process presenting multiple possibilities for improving outcomes (e.g., SRG)
The resilience cycle (Figure 1) encompasses ongoing interactions among a complex array of biological (e.g., genetic, epigenetic), neuropsychological (e.g., cognitive, emotional), and environmental (e.g., social, economic, and cultural) factors that affect how an individual responds to a particular stressful experience (39, 2630). A resilient (hardy) individual maintains an adaptive level of physiological and psychological functioning when challenged by events. Multiple types of evidence support the importance of moderately stressful events (tolerable stress) for increasing an individual’s level of resilience (stress inoculation, tempering, steeling, adversity-driven resilience) (15, 7). Events that induce little or no stress do not provide a resilience-building experience. Events that induce very high levels of stress may overwhelm an individual’s coping capacity, at least temporarily (toxic stress). It should be noted that SRG has been identified not only in relation to stressful or traumatic external events but also in relation to recovery from serious mental illness (31, 32). A personal account of recovery from psychosis noting psychological growth following several psychotic episodes suggested that opportunities for growth (e.g., intentional introspection, meaning making, benefit finding, and positive self-disclosure) should be intentionally incorporated into treatment and recovery plans (32).
FIGURE 1. The baseline level of resilience (initial state) is a product of complex interactions among multiple biological, psychological, and environmental factors that play a key role in an individual’s response to a stressful event (19). The adjustment process and the outcome also depend on the severity of stress experienced. If a stressful event is of moderate severity (tolerable stress), successful adjustment may increase resilience and promote growth (green). Events that induce little stress require little adjustment and thus are likely to result in recovery (yellow) without increased resilience (i.e., return to baseline functioning). Events that induce very high levels of stress (toxic stress) may overwhelm an individual’s coping capacity, at least temporarily, and result in impairment (red). Resilience level is modulated by many factors, including aspects of cognitive flexibility (e.g., deliberative rumination, finding meaning, reappraisal, self-reflection, and internal locus of control) and emotion regulation (e.g., suppression of emotion, emotional disequilibrium, and emotional hyper reactivity) (4, 812).
Although most research to date has focused on the construct of posttraumatic growth, this is only one aspect of a rather complex and multifaceted phenomenon of positive adaptation and development following events that are perceived as distressing regardless of valence (4, 28). An emerging body of literature indicates that growth is not limited to situations and events that have negative valence and that psychological growth may occur following any event that challenges an individual’s coping resources. A recent meta-analysis of longitudinal studies examining the effects of life events on psychological well-being reported a positive trend for self-esteem, positive relationships, and mastery following both positive and negative events, with no general evidence that negative life events had a stronger effect (33). Additionally, they found that in studies utilizing control groups, results did not significantly differ between the event and control groups, indicating that changes in outcome variables cannot necessarily be attributed to the reported life events.
Multiple conceptual models for adaptive coping with adversity and fostering resilience and growth have been proposed (e.g., PTG, coping circumplex model, systematic self-reflection, cognitive appraisal of resilience, cognitive growth, and stress) (4, 911, 34). All include an aspect of cognitive flexibility (e.g., cognitive processing, cognitive appraisal, rumination, problem-solving, meaning making, benefit finding, and self-reflection) as a primary mechanism for strengthening resilience after exposure to life stressors that require adaptation (i.e., psychological adjustment through the process of redefinition of the self and reappraisal of one’s beliefs). Several models include emotion regulation as the second key aspect of resilience (4, 9, 11). Recent reviews on the neurobiology of resilience converge on the central importance of the prefrontal cortex (PFC)-limbic circuitry (57, 3537). Higher resilience is associated with greater emotion regulation capacity (i.e., top-down regulation of emotional responses). Greater engagement of PFC areas important for cognitive and emotional control enhances inhibition of limbic areas such as the amygdala, reducing reactivity to stressful stimuli. Resilience-related differences in resting-state EEG have also been reported. A study of children that compared groups with and without maltreatment reported greater relative left central activity (lower left compared with right alpha power) in high-resilience (composite index of adaptive functioning) individuals from both groups, whereas low resilience was associated with greater right hemisphere activity (38). A study of middle-aged adults reported that higher right frontal activity was associated with increased levels of blood biomarkers (interleukin-6, C-reactive protein, and fibrinogen) indicating low-grade inflammation only in the group who reported having experienced moderate to severe abuse as children (Childhood Trauma Questionnaire) (39). As discussed by the authors, these results are consistent with higher right frontal activity indicating greater vulnerability to emotionally challenging stressors (diathesis-stress model) and that this may also increase risk for inflammation-related medical conditions.
Both cognitive and emotional approaches to coping with adversity span a wide range of strategies, some of which are detrimental to functioning. For example, rumination (repetitive thinking about an event) can be either deliberate or intrusive (40, 41). Deliberate (reflective) ruminations are adaptive, voluntary purposeful thoughts focused on better understanding of the event (i.e., its meaning, consequences, and implications). Additionally, deliberate rumination may serve as a coping strategy for managing emotional responses. Intrusive (perseverative) ruminations (e.g., worry, brooding) are maladaptive, automatic unwanted thoughts or memories that increase an individual’s distress. Intrusive rumination may also involve attempts to suppress thoughts about a stressful event. As would be expected, research indicates that intrusive and deliberate ruminations play very different roles in outcomes (41). Recent longitudinal and cross-sectional studies in a wide range of trauma-exposed groups (e.g., middle school and college students, military veterans, emergency services personnel, cancer survivors, and patients with nervous system injuries) indicate that intrusive rumination is positively associated with psychiatric symptoms, whereas deliberative rumination is positively associated with SRG (10, 4249). As noted in several studies, these results support the potential of interventions facilitating deliberative rumination during recovery to promote better outcomes. Similarly, emotional coping may be positive and lead to emotional equilibrium (i.e., adequate emotional control), or it may be negative and result in emotional disequilibrium (e.g., emotional hyperreactivity, excessive anxiety, and suppressing emotions). These concepts are consistent with the broader body of literature that has demonstrated associations between emotion dysregulation and various forms of psychopathology (50). Studies identifying specific capacities or skills associated with higher resilience and/or better recovery from challenging events provide potential targets for interventions.
Interventions that promote cognitive flexibility and/or emotion regulation would be expected to strengthen resilience. Cognitive restructuring is one of the primary techniques utilized in cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), which involves identifying and challenging negative and rigid thought patterns (51, 52). The goal is to facilitate resilience rather than to solve a particular problem or achieve a certain outcome, as illustrated in a recent study of patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) comparing CBT with eye movement desensitization and reprocessing therapy (53). There were no differences in clinical efficacy, but only CBT improved general life functioning (Work and Social Adjustment Scale). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) is a transdiagnostic approach that has the primary focus of fostering psychological flexibility as a pathway toward adaptation and psychological well-being (54, 55). Recent systematic reviews and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of interventions that included therapeutic mindfulness (e.g., ACT, mindfulness-based stress reduction, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy) reported beneficial effects on a wide range of behavioral health outcomes (e.g., anxiety, depression, fatigue, stress, quality of life, and PTG) in adults with cancer (56, 57). Mindfulness-based interventions have also been shown to decrease use of maladaptive coping strategies (e.g., avoidance or disengagement coping, negative emotional coping) (58, 59). Dialectical-behavior therapy (DBT) may also be a useful therapeutic approach to facilitate emotion regulation, because it incorporates mindfulness, emphasizes the role of difficulties in emotion regulation, and focuses on the development of emotion-regulation skills (6062). DBT has been shown to be effective in decreasing transdiagnostic emotion dysregulation (6365). Overall, meta-analyses and systematic reviews of various psychosocial interventions generally show that interventions based on principles of CBT and mindfulness have the strongest effect sizes in fostering resilience and psychological growth following adverse events (66, 67). Imaging studies comparing pretreatment to posttreatment resting-state and/or task-activated functional MRI (fMRI) have reported changes in brain activation patterns following treatments that correlate with symptom improvements, suggesting beneficial neuroplastic processes (53, 6872).
Whether self-reported growth following challenging events is primarily positive is a matter of considerable debate (34, 7381). A key problem with research in this area is reliance on retrospective reports of self-perceived growth that may not represent genuine transformation (76, 79, 8286). The few longitudinal studies indicate that self-perceived growth does not correlate with more objective measures of psychological growth (i.e., measures of actual psychological resources) (82, 85, 87, 88). In addition, a longitudinal study assessing veterans decades after their participation in the Yom Kippur War found that self-reported growth was linked with several detrimental outcomes, including higher rates of loneliness and lower dyadic adjustment in marital relationships (78, 80). The authors suggested that self-perceived growth may be better described as a set of defensive beliefs, a potentially maladaptive strategy for coping with distress. The Janus Face model encompasses this dichotomy by proposing that self-perceived growth following traumatic events has both a functional or constructive side and an illusory (e.g., self-deceptive or dysfunctional) side that may coexist (89, 90). The constructive side is correlated with healthy adjustment, whereas the illusory side is correlated with denial. Similarly, other studies have concluded that self-reported growth can indicate an adaptive outcome of successful cognitive coping or a positive illusion that is a result of avoidance and denial (76, 91). However, the question remains whether self-perception of growth may be beneficial to individuals even if it does not reflect measurable change. On one hand, illusory perceptions (i.e., beliefs that are not grounded in reality) are generally considered maladaptive. On the other hand, some studies have demonstrated that even perceptions of positive change may be linked with more adaptive coping (i.e., positive reinterpretation), better mental health (e.g., lower symptoms of anxiety and depression, higher stress tolerance), and increased quality of life (74, 77, 9294). Thus, illusory or self-deceptive growth is not always associated with maladjustment. If illusory perceptions of growth coexist with and do not hinder active coping (e.g., deliberate ruminations), then they may serve as a short-term adaptive palliative coping strategy helping individuals deal effectively with the aftermath of distressing events (83, 90, 95).
Most research on the neurobiological correlates of SRG has also utilized self-report questionnaires (e.g., the Post Traumatic Growth Index [PTGI]) that assess retrospective perception of change. Several studies have reported correlations between PTGI scores and imaging metrics within the dorsolateral PFC (Figure 2) (1315). A study of healthy adults utilizing group spatial independent component analysis of resting-state fMRI reported a positive correlation between PTGI scores and higher resting-activity level in two left hemisphere areas that are part of the central executive network (dorsolateral PFC, superior parietal lobule) (13). A longitudinal MRI study of healthy young adults identified changes in regional gray matter volume (whole brain voxel-based morphometry) between images acquired prior to and 3 months after they experienced the East Japan Great Earthquake (14). PTGI scores were positively and significantly associated only with change in the right dorsolateral PFC (14). In the region of interest analysis, PTGI scores were positively correlated, whereas posttraumatic stress symptoms (Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale [CAPS]) were negatively correlated with changes in gray matter volume in the same area. As noted by the authors, individuals with low PTGI scores had reduced volume in this area, suggesting that the direction of volumetric changes may indicate adaptive versus maladaptive responses to stress (14). Congruent with this hypothesis, an earlier longitudinal MRI study that compared patients with PTSD and healthy individuals reported that at the earliest imaging time point (average of 1.42 years since the trauma), the PTSD group had greater cortical thickness within portions of dorsolateral PFC bilaterally (Figure 2) (15). Greater cortical thickness was positively correlated with better performance on tests of executive functioning in the PTSD group. It also correlated with greater symptom reduction (CAPS score) during the previous year and by the final time point (average of 3.85 years since the trauma). Cortical thickness had normalized by the final time point, and cortical thinning correlated with symptom resolution, indicating a role for dorsolateral PFC in supporting recovery from PTSD (15).
FIGURE 2 and COVER. Upper panel and Cover: Most research on the neurobiological correlates of stress-related growth (SRG) has utilized self-report questionnaires that assess retrospective perception of change. Two studies of healthy adults that explored relationships between Post Traumatic Growth Index (PTGI) score and imaging metrics identified the importance of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC). A resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) study reported a positive correlation between PTGI scores and higher resting-activity level in two left hemisphere areas that are part of the central executive network (gold) (13). A longitudinal MRI study of individuals who experienced the East Japan Great Earthquake reported that PTGI scores were positively and significantly associated only with change in the right dorsolateral PFC volume (orange) (14). Of note, individuals with low PTGI scores had reduced volume in this area, suggesting that the direction of volumetric changes may indicate adaptive versus maladaptive responses to stress (14). Lower panel: Congruent with this hypothesis, another longitudinal MRI study that compared patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and healthy individuals reported that the PTSD group initially had greater cortical thickness within portions of the dorsolateral PFC bilaterally (yellow) (15). Initial cortical thickness correlated with greater symptom reduction at the final timepoint. By the final time point, cortical thickness had normalized, and cortical thinning correlated with symptom resolution, indicating a role for dorsolateral PFC in supporting recovery from PTSD (15).
As detailed in recent reviews, an individual’s ability to dismiss unwanted memories when they intrude into conscious awareness is an important aspect of healthy emotion regulation that may be modifiable (16, 17). This capacity is experimentally assessed by pairing items to be remembered with reminder cues and comparing results when participants are instructed to recall (Think) or instructed to not recall (NoThink) the paired items when cued. Participants also report whether a memory intrusion occurred during the NoThink condition. In healthy individuals, suppression of an intrusive memory decreases the frequency of subsequent intrusions and weakens (as indexed by decreased priming effects) the memory trace (suppression-induced forgetting, motivated forgetting, adaptive forgetting, beneficial forgetting, and selective forgetting). Recent studies of suppression-induced forgetting provide evidence that both explicit and implicit memory traces are affected (96, 97). Ability to suppress intrusions varies considerably across healthy individuals, and lower ability has been associated with higher maladaptive traits (e.g., anxiety, brooding) (17). A study of college students found that higher ability was associated with higher trauma exposure, suggesting the possibility that this capacity may be increased by successfully coping with moderate levels of adversity (16, 17). Impaired ability to suppress intrusive memories on this task has been reported for multiple psychiatric disorders, including PTSD (16, 17).
Cross-sectional fMRI studies of healthy individuals performing the Think/NoThink task have reported that memory suppression is associated with increased activation (NoThink >Think) in frontal and parietal cortices and decreased activation (NoThink <Think) in memory-related areas (e.g., hippocampus, memory-associated domain-specific cortical areas) (Figure 3) (16, 18, 19). An fMRI study that utilized unpleasant images demonstrated that an area in the right dorsolateral PFC exerted parallel top-down inhibition of the hippocampus and domain-specific areas (e.g., amygdala, sensory cortices) (18). Effective connectivity analyses indicated that intrusion of memory into awareness in the NoThink condition, which triggers increased effort to dismiss the memory, was associated with increased top-down inhibition (i.e., increased negative coupling with the right dorsolateral PFC). Greater success at suppressing intrusive memories (i.e., higher memory control) was associated with reduced intrusion frequency and reduced memory-associated negative emotion, indicating weakening of both episodic and emotional memory traces (18). An fMRI study that compared trauma-exposed individuals (in the Paris terrorist attacks) with and without PTSD to unexposed individuals on this task reported that all three groups improved on suppression with practice (fewer intrusions during NoThink trials) to a similar extent (19). However, the PTSD group differed from the trauma-exposed without PTSD and unexposed groups on functional metrics. The PTSD group had neither the robust decrease in memory strength for NoThink items (i.e., decreased priming effect indicating weakening of memory traces) nor the increase in top-down inhibition (i.e., increased negative coupling between the right dorsolateral PFC and areas important for memory such as the hippocampus) during suppression of intrusive memories seen in the other groups. As discussed by the authors, these findings indicate an impairment in memory regulation in the PTSD group that may be the reason memory intrusions are so persistent (19). Multiple authors have noted that results from this task suggest that it may be time to re-examine the common belief that all forms of memory suppression are maladaptive (1619, 97).
FIGURE 3. An individual’s ability to dismiss unwanted memories when they intrude into conscious awareness is an important aspect of healthy emotion regulation (16, 17). This capacity is experimentally assessed by pairing items to be remembered with reminder cues and comparing results when participants are instructed to recall (Think) or instructed to not recall (NoThink) the paired items when cued. In healthy individuals, successful suppression of an intrusive memory weakens the memory trace (as indexed by decreased priming effects). Impaired ability to suppress intrusive memories on this task has been reported for multiple psychiatric disorders (16, 17). fMRI studies of healthy individuals performing the Think/NoThink task have reported that memory suppression is associated with increased activation (NoThink >Think) in frontal and parietal cortices and decreased activation (NoThink <Think) in memory-related areas (e.g., hippocampus, memory-associated domain-specific cortical areas). Approximate locations and extents of cortical activations and deactivations from two studies of healthy individuals are color-coded onto representative MRIs (18, 19). One study compared trauma-exposed individuals with and without PTSD to unexposed individuals (19). The authors reported that the PTSD group did not have the decreases in memory strength for NoThink items or the increases in top-down inhibition during suppression of intrusive memories seen in the other groups. As discussed by the authors, these findings indicate an impairment in memory regulation in the PTSD group that may be the reason memory intrusions are so persistent.
In conclusion, recent studies are contributing a new understanding of adaptive and maladaptive stress-related changes in the brain, resilience, and to the neuroplastic changes underlying SRG. This understanding offers promising evidence for recovery following even serious adversities and psychological traumas.

References

1.
Shakespeare-Finch J, Lurie-Beck J: A meta-analytic clarification of the relationship between posttraumatic growth and symptoms of posttraumatic distress disorder. J Anxiety Disord 2014; 28:223–229
2.
Dooley LN, Slavich GM, Moreno PI, et al: Strength through adversity: Moderate lifetime stress exposure is associated with psychological resilience in breast cancer survivors. Stress Health 2017; 33:549–557
3.
Cathomas F, Murrough JW, Nestler EJ, et al: Neurobiology of resilience: interface between mind and body. Biol Psychiatry 2019; 86:410–420
4.
Crane MF, Searle BJ, Kangas M, et al: How resilience is strengthened by exposure to stressors: the systematic self-reflection model of resilience strengthening. Anxiety Stress Coping 2019; 32:1–17
5.
Feder A, Fred-Torres S, Southwick SM, et al: The biology of human resilience: opportunities for enhancing resilience across the lifespan. Biol Psychiatry 2019; 86:443–453
6.
Ioannidis K, Askelund AD, Kievit RA, et al: The complex neurobiology of resilient functioning after childhood maltreatment. BMC Med 2020; 18:32
7.
Malhi GS, Das P, Bell E, et al: Modelling resilience in adolescence and adversity: a novel framework to inform research and practice. Transl Psychiatry 2019; 9:316
8.
Seiler A, Jenewein J: Resilience in cancer patients. Front Psychiatry 2019; 10:208
9.
Yao Z-F, Hsieh S: Neurocognitive mechanism of human resilience: a conceptual framework and empirical review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2019; 16:5123
10.
Brooks M, Graham-Kevan N, Lowe M, et al: Rumination, event centrality, and perceived control as predictors of post-traumatic growth and distress: the Cognitive Growth and Stress model. Br J Clin Psychol 2017; 56:286–302
11.
Stanisławski K: The Coping Circumplex Model: an integrative model of the structure of coping with stress. Front Psychol 2019; 10:694
12.
Boykin DM, Anyanwu J, Calvin K, et al: The moderating effect of psychological flexibility on event centrality in determining trauma outcomes. Psychol Trauma 2020; 12:193–199
13.
Fujisawa TX, Jung M, Kojima M, et al: Neural basis of psychological growth following adverse experiences: a resting-state functional MRI study. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0136427–e0136427
14.
Nakagawa S, Sugiura M, Sekiguchi A, et al: Effects of post-traumatic growth on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex after a disaster. Sci Rep 2016; 6:34364–34364
15.
Lyoo IK, Kim JE, Yoon SJ, et al: The neurobiological role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in recovery from trauma: longitudinal brain imaging study among survivors of the South Korean subway disaster. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68:701–713
16.
Engen HG, Anderson MC: Memory control: a fundamental mechanism of emotion regulation. Trends Cogn Sci 2018; 22:982–995
17.
Nørby S: Forgetting and emotion regulation in mental health, anxiety and depression. Memory 2018; 26:342–363
18.
Gagnepain P, Hulbert J, Anderson MC: Parallel regulation of memory and emotion supports the suppression of intrusive memories. J Neurosci 2017; 37:6423–6441
19.
Mary A, Dayan J, Leone G, et al: Resilience after trauma: The role of memory suppression. Science 2020; 367:eaay8477
20.
Affleck G, Tennen H: Construing benefits from adversity: adaptational significance and dispositional underpinnings. J Pers 1996; 64:899–922
21.
Park CL, Cohen LH, Murch RL: Assessment and prediction of stress-related growth. J Pers 1996; 64:71–105
22.
Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG: The Posttraumatic Growth Inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma. J Trauma Stress 1996; 9:455–471
23.
O’Leary VE, Alday CS, Ickovics JR: Models of life change and posttraumatic growth, in Posttraumatic Growth: Positive Changes in the Aftermath of Crisis. Edited by Tedeschi RG, Park CL, Calhoun LG. Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 1998, pp 127–151
24.
Linley PA, Joseph S: Positive change following trauma and adversity: a review. J Trauma Stress 2004; 17:11–21
25.
Cohen S, Murphy MLM, Prather AA: Ten surprising facts about stressful life events and disease risk. Annu Rev Psychol 2019; 70:577–597
26.
Southwick SM, Bonanno GA, Masten AS, et al: Resilience definitions, theory, and challenges: interdisciplinary perspectives. Eur J Psychotraumatol 2014; 5:25338
27.
van Breda AD: A critical review of resilience theory and its relevance for social work. Soc Work 2018; 54:1–18
28.
Rakesh G, Morey RA, Zannas AS, et al: Resilience as a translational endpoint in the treatment of PTSD. Mol Psychiatry 2019; 24:1268–1283
29.
Snijders C, Pries L-K, Sgammeglia N, et al: Resilience against traumatic stress: current developments and future directions. Front Psychiatry 2018; 9:676
30.
Gijzel SMW, Whitson HE, van de Leemput IA, et al: Resilience in clinical care: getting a grip on the recovery potential of older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2019; 67:2650–2657
31.
Slade M, Rennick-Egglestone S, Blackie L, et al: Post-traumatic growth in mental health recovery: qualitative study of narratives. BMJ Open 2019; 9:e029342
32.
Lee YY, Verma S, Subramaniam M: Beyond recovery: exploring growth in the aftermath of psychosis. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11:108
33.
Mangelsdorf J, Eid M, Luhmann M: Does growth require suffering? a systematic review and meta-analysis on genuine posttraumatic and postecstatic growth. Psychol Bull 2019; 145:302–338
34.
Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG: Posttraumatic growth: conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychol Inq 2004; 15:1–18
35.
Bolsinger J, Seifritz E, Kleim B, et al: Neuroimaging correlates of resilience to traumatic events—a comprehensive review. Front Psychiatry 2018; 9:693
36.
Holz NE, Tost H, Meyer-Lindenberg A: Resilience and the brain: a key role for regulatory circuits linked to social stress and support. Mol Psychiatry 2020; 25:379–396
37.
Dolcos F, Katsumi Y, Moore M, et al: Neural correlates of emotion-attention interactions: From perception, learning, and memory to social cognition, individual differences, and training interventions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 108:559–601
38.
Curtis WJ, Cicchetti D: Emotion and resilience: a multilevel investigation of hemispheric electroencephalogram asymmetry and emotion regulation in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Dev Psychopathol 2007; 19:811–840
39.
Hostinar CE, Davidson RJ, Graham EK, et al: Frontal brain asymmetry, childhood maltreatment, and low-grade inflammation at midlife. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017; 75:152–163
40.
Cann A, Calhoun LG, Tedeschi RG, et al: Assessing posttraumatic cognitive processes: the Event Related Rumination Inventory. Anxiety Stress Coping 2011; 24:137–156
41.
Luca M: Maladaptive rumination as a transdiagnostic mediator of vulnerability and outcome in psychopathology. J Clin Med 2019; 8:314
42.
Hill EM, Watkins K: Women with ovarian cancer: examining the role of social support and rumination in posttraumatic growth, psychological distress, and psychological well-being. J Clin Psychol Med Settings 2017; 24:47–58
43.
Morgan JK, Desmarais SL, Mitchell RE, et al: Posttraumatic stress, posttraumatic growth, and satisfaction with life in military veterans. Mil Psychol 2017; 29:434–447
44.
Shigemoto Y, Low B, Borowa D, et al: Function of personal growth initiative on posttraumatic growth, posttraumatic stress, and depression over and above adaptive and maladaptive rumination. J Clin Psychol 2017; 73:1126–1145
45.
Kelly G, Morris R, Shetty H: Predictors of post-traumatic growth in stroke survivors. Disabil Rehabil 2018; 40:2916–2924
46.
Goldberg LD, McDonald SD, Perrin PB: Predicting trajectories of posttraumatic growth following acquired physical disability. Rehabil Psychol 2019; 64:37–49
47.
Ragger K, Hiebler-Ragger M, Herzog G, et al: Sense of coherence is linked to post-traumatic growth after critical incidents in Austrian ambulance personnel. BMC Psychiatry 2019; 19:89
48.
Xu W, Jiang H, Zhou Y, et al: Intrusive rumination, deliberate rumination, and posttraumatic growth among adolescents after a tornado: the role of social support. J Nerv Ment Dis 2019; 207:152–156
49.
Yang S-K, Ha Y: Predicting posttraumatic growth among firefighters: the role of deliberate rumination and problem-focused coping. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2019; 16:3879
50.
Aldao A, Gee DG, De Los Reyes A, et al: Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: current and future directions. Dev Psychopathol 2016; 28(4pt1):927–946
51.
Greenberger D, Padesky CA: Mind Over Mood: Change How You Feel by Changing the Way You Think. New York, Guilford Publications, 2015
52.
Hawley LL, Padesky CA, Hollon SD, et al: Cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression using mind over mood: CBT skill use and differential symptom alleviation. Behav Ther 2017; 48:29–44
53.
Santarnecchi E, Bossini L, Vatti G, et al: Psychological and brain connectivity changes following trauma-focused CBT and EMDR treatment in single-episode PTSD patients. Front Psychol 2019; 10:129
54.
Hayes SC: Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies–republished article. Behav Ther 2016; 47:869–885
55.
Dindo L, Van Liew JR, Arch JJ: Acceptance and commitment therapy: a transdiagnostic behavioral intervention for mental health and medical conditions. Neurotherapeutics 2017; 14:546–553
56.
Shiyko MP, Hallinan S, Naito T: Effects of mindfulness training on posttraumatic growth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mindfulness 2017; 8:848–858
57.
Xunlin NG, Lau Y, Klainin-Yobas P: The effectiveness of mindfulness-based interventions among cancer patients and survivors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Support Care Cancer 2020; 28:1563–1578
58.
Cousin G, Crane C: Changes in disengagement coping mediate changes in affect following mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in a non-clinical sample. Br J Psychol 2016; 107:434–447
59.
Messer D, Horan JJ, Turner W, et al: The effects of internet-delivered mindfulness training on stress, coping, and mindfulness in university students. AERA Open 2016; 2:2332858415625188
60.
Linehan MM, Armstrong HE, Suarez A, et al: Cognitive-behavioral treatment of chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:1060–1064
61.
Chapman AL: Dialectical behavior therapy: current indications and unique elements. Psychiatry (Edgmont) 2006; 3:62–68
62.
Linehan M: DBT Skills Training Manual. New York, Guilford Publications, 2014
63.
Neacsiu AD, Eberle JW, Kramer R, et al: Dialectical behavior therapy skills for transdiagnostic emotion dysregulation: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther 2014; 59:40–51
64.
Cavicchioli M, Movalli M, Vassena G, et al: The therapeutic role of emotion regulation and coping strategies during a stand-alone DBT Skills training program for alcohol use disorder and concurrent substance use disorders. Addict Behav 2019; 98:106035
65.
DeCou CR, Comtois KA, Landes SJ: Dialectical behavior therapy is effective for the treatment of suicidal behavior: a meta-analysis. Behav Ther 2019; 50:60–72
66.
Roepke AM: Psychosocial interventions and posttraumatic growth: a meta-analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 2015; 83:129–142
67.
Ludolph P, Kunzler AM, Stoffers-Winterling J, et al: Interventions to promote resilience in cancer patients. Dtsch Arztebl Int 2019; 51-52:865–872
68.
Goodman M, Carpenter D, Tang CY, et al: Dialectical behavior therapy alters emotion regulation and amygdala activity in patients with borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2014; 57:108–116
69.
Dixon MR, Wilson AN, Habib R: Neurological evidence of acceptance and commitment therapy effectiveness in college-age gamblers. J Contextual Behav Sci 2016; 5:80–88
70.
Smallwood RF, Potter JS, Robin DA: Neurophysiological mechanisms in acceptance and commitment therapy in opioid-addicted patients with chronic pain. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2016; 250:12–14
71.
Young KS, Burklund LJ, Torre JB, et al: Treatment for social anxiety disorder alters functional connectivity in emotion regulation neural circuitry. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2017; 261:44–51
72.
Young KS, van der Velden AM, Craske MG, et al: The impact of mindfulness-based interventions on brain activity: a systematic review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018; 84:424–433
73.
Blix I, Hansen MB, Birkeland MS, et al: Posttraumatic growth, posttraumatic stress and psychological adjustment in the aftermath of the 2011 Oslo bombing attack. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2013; 11:160–160
74.
Grace JJ, Kinsella EL, Muldoon OT, et al: Post-traumatic growth following acquired brain injury: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1162
75.
Lahav Y, Solomon Z, Levin Y: Posttraumatic growth and perceived health: the role of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Am J Orthopsychiatry 2016; 86:693–703
76.
Schubert CF, Schmidt U, Rosner R: Posttraumatic growth in populations with posttraumatic stress disorder—a systematic review on growth-related psychological constructs and biological variables. Clin Psychol Psychother 2016; 23:469–486
77.
Casellas-Grau A, Ochoa C, Ruini C: Psychological and clinical correlates of posttraumatic growth in cancer: a systematic and critical review. Psychooncology 2017; 26:2007–2018
78.
Lahav Y, Kanat-Maymon Y, Solomon Z: Posttraumatic growth and dyadic adjustment among war veterans and their wives. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1102
79.
Cheng CT, Ho SM, Hou YC, et al: Constructive, illusory, and distressed posttraumatic growth among survivors of breast cancer: a 7-year growth trajectory study. J Health Psychol (Epub ahead of print, August 6, 2018) doi: 10.1177/1359105318793199
80.
Stein JY, Levin Y, Bachem R, et al: Growing apart: a longitudinal assessment of the relation between post-traumatic growth and loneliness among combat veterans. Front Psychol 2018; 9:893
81.
Tedeschi RG, Shakespeare-Finch J, Taku K, et al: Posttraumatic Growth: Theory, Research, and Applications. London, Routledge, 2018
82.
Frazier P, Coyne J, Tennen H: Post-traumatic growth: a call for less, but better, research. Eur J Pers 2014; 28:337–338
83.
Jayawickreme E, Blackie LE: Post‐traumatic growth as positive personality change: evidence, controversies and future directions. Eur J Pers 2014; 28:312–331
84.
Owenz M, Fowers BJ: Perceived post-traumatic growth may not reflect actual positive change: a short-term prospective study of relationship dissolution. J Soc Pers Relat 2018; 36:3098–3116
85.
Boals A, Bedford LA, Callahan JL: Perceptions of change after a trauma and perceived posttraumatic growth: a prospective examination. Behav Sci (Basel) 2019; 9:10
86.
Kunz S, Joseph S, Geyh S, et al: Perceived posttraumatic growth and depreciation after spinal cord injury: actual or illusory? Health Psychol 2019; 38:53–62
87.
Frazier P, Tennen H, Gavian M, et al: Does self-reported posttraumatic growth reflect genuine positive change? Psychol Sci 2009; 20:912–919
88.
Park CL, Sinnott SM: Testing the validity of self-reported posttraumatic growth in young adult cancer survivors. Behav Sci (Basel) 2018; 8:116
89.
Maercker A, Zoellner T: The Janus face of self-perceived growth: toward a two-component model of posttraumatic growth. Psychol Inq 2004; 15:41–48
90.
Zoellner T, Maercker A: Posttraumatic growth in clinical psychology: a critical review and introduction of a two component model. Clin Psychol Rev 2006; 26:626–653
91.
Achterhof R, Dorahy MJ, Rowlands A, et al: Predictors of posttraumatic growth 10-11 months after a fatal earthquake. Psychol Trauma 2018; 10:208–215
92.
Sawyer A, Ayers S, Field AP: Posttraumatic growth and adjustment among individuals with cancer or HIV/AIDS: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2010; 30:436–447
93.
Silva SM, Moreira HC, Canavarro MC: Examining the links between perceived impact of breast cancer and psychosocial adjustment: the buffering role of posttraumatic growth. Psychooncology 2012; 21:409–418
94.
Teodorescu D-S, Siqveland J, Heir T, et al: Posttraumatic growth, depressive symptoms, posttraumatic stress symptoms, post-migration stressors and quality of life in multi-traumatized psychiatric outpatients with a refugee background in Norway. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2012; 10:84
95.
Fleeson W: Four ways of (not) being real and whether they are essential for post-traumatic growth. Eur J Pers 2014; 28:332–361
96.
Taubenfeld A, Anderson MC, Levy DA: The impact of retrieval suppression on conceptual implicit memory. Memory 2019; 27:686–697
97.
Wang Y, Luppi A, Fawcett J, et al: Reconsidering unconscious persistence: Suppressing unwanted memories reduces their indirect expression in later thoughts. Cognition 2019; 187:78–94

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: A4 - 212

History

Received: 14 May 2020
Accepted: 9 June 2020
Published in print: Summer 2020
Published online: 30 July 2020

Keywords

  1. Neuroanatomy
  2. Stress

Authors

Affiliations

Anna S. Ord, Psy.D.
The Veterans Affairs Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, and the Research and Academic Affairs Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Ord, Hurley, Taber); the Mental Health Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Stranahan); the Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, N.C. (Hurley); the Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Hurley); the Division of Biomedical Sciences, Via College of Osteopathic Medicine, Blacksburg, Va. (Taber); and the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Taber).
Kathryn R. Stranahan, Psy.D.
The Veterans Affairs Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, and the Research and Academic Affairs Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Ord, Hurley, Taber); the Mental Health Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Stranahan); the Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, N.C. (Hurley); the Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Hurley); the Division of Biomedical Sciences, Via College of Osteopathic Medicine, Blacksburg, Va. (Taber); and the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Taber).
Robin A. Hurley, M.D. [email protected]
The Veterans Affairs Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, and the Research and Academic Affairs Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Ord, Hurley, Taber); the Mental Health Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Stranahan); the Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, N.C. (Hurley); the Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Hurley); the Division of Biomedical Sciences, Via College of Osteopathic Medicine, Blacksburg, Va. (Taber); and the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Taber).
Katherine H. Taber, Ph.D.
The Veterans Affairs Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, and the Research and Academic Affairs Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Ord, Hurley, Taber); the Mental Health Service Line, W.G. Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, N.C. (Stranahan); the Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, N.C. (Hurley); the Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Hurley); the Division of Biomedical Sciences, Via College of Osteopathic Medicine, Blacksburg, Va. (Taber); and the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (Taber).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Hurley ([email protected]).

Funding Information

This research was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs Office of Academic Affiliations Advanced Fellowship Program in Mental Illness, Research, and Treatment and Clinical Psychology Residency Program.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share