Skip to main content
Full access
Analysis and Commentary
Published Online: 4 February 2021

Frontotemporal Dementia: A Window to Alexithymia

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Alexithymia is pervasive among psychiatric patients, but its neurobiological mechanism is unclear. Patients with alexithymia cannot “read emotions,” a process involving interoception, or the perception of the body’s internal state, primarily in the insulae. The frontotemporal dementias are also associated with inability to correctly read emotions; hence, these dementias can provide a window into the mechanism of alexithymia. Patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) have a weak emotional signal with impaired emotional recognition, hypoemotionality, and decreased physiological arousal. bvFTD affects the insulae, and the weak emotional signal facilitates impaired interoceptive accuracy, resulting in an overreliance on cognitive appraisal rather than on internal sensations. In contrast, patients with semantic dementia, another frontotemporal dementia syndrome, can have intact interoception, but they have disturbed cognitive appraisal of the meaning of their bodily sensations. This “alexisomia” in semantic dementia can lead to misinterpreted somatic symptoms. Together, the findings in alexithymic patients and frontotemporal dementia syndromes support the model of impaired interoceptive accuracy as the mechanism of alexithymia, possibly from dysfunction in the insulae.
Alexithymia, or the inability to identify and describe one’s feelings, is a condition present in a broad range of psychiatric disorders. Although originally introduced to describe features of patients with psychosomatic disorders (1, 2), investigators have implicated alexithymia as part of the symptomatology of anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorders, disturbances in eating and addiction, schizophrenia and other psychotic conditions, psychopathy, autism, and a range of other disorders (3). Early theories for alexithymia stressed psychological traumas and high levels of psychological distress with overuse of defensive mechanisms, resulting in somatic symptoms and affect avoidance (4, 5). A further analysis of the neurobiological rather than psychodynamic mechanism of alexithymia could lead to a better understanding and management of this condition.
The term “alexithymia” means “lack of words, or inability to read emotions” (1). Indeed, patients with alexithymia are deficient in the cognitive processing of emotions. For example, alexithymia impairs emotion recognition from faces and voices (6, 7), and it can result in atypical emotional facial expressions (7, 8). Unraveling the nature of alexithymia requires a grounding in the neurobiology of emotion.

The Neurobiology of Emotion

Emotion is a process that involves the insulae and a number of frontotemporal structures. Emotional processing in the brain begins with the identification of salient or emotional stimuli mediated by the amygdala. This results in physiological arousal and interoception, the perception of internal body or somatic sensations. Interoceptive sensitivity is strongly correlated with the strength of the physiological arousal in the posterior insulae (9). The anterior insulae, particularly on the right, merge these internal sensations with cognitive evaluation, resulting in the awareness of feelings (10). This process involves reciprocal interactions between the anterior insulae and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which mediate emotional responding and the expression of affective arousal. The anterior insulae and ACC are core regions of the salience network, which activates in response to emotionally salient cues in the environment (11). In addition to interoception, the cognitive appraisal of one’s internal and external environment also contributes to emotional experience (12). Cognitive appraisal involves frontal regions such as the orbitofrontal cortex for emotional control, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex for affective meaning, and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex for emotional integration and regulation. Finally, other temporal lobe structures such as the inferior temporal and fusiform gyrus are important for affective signal detection and integration, and the anterior temporal poles specifically link interoceptive stimuli with their conceptual meaning (13).

Alexithymia and Interoception

The mechanism for alexithymia may be a disassociation between internal physiological arousal and externally-based cognitive appraisal (14). In alexithymia, investigators describe difficulty “elevating emotions from a sensorimotor level of experience” to a representational level, where they can be used as signaling interoception (15). On the cognitive appraisal side, others propose relative dysfunction of neocortical areas for symbolically and linguistically representing emotion (16). Indeed, reports of patients who underwent a commissurotomy, or surgical incision into the callosal commissure, indicate increased alexithymia, as preserved interoceptive feelings in the right hemisphere could no longer access left hemisphere language and symbolization (14).
Still other information links alexithymia to a failure of interoception (17, 18). Persons with higher levels of alexithymia appear to have poor knowledge of their internal states and decreased interoceptive accuracy (9); alexithymia can impair interoception of cardiac and respiratory output, muscular effort, and taste sensitivity (17). Poor cardiac sensitivity correlates with high levels of alexithymia (19), and higher self-reported interoceptive confusion occurs among alexithymic individuals (18, 20). In autism, alexithymia is associated with decreased interoceptive sensitivity even in the presence of an intact interoceptive signal (21, 22). Much research also associates damage to the anterior insulae with both interoceptive difficulties and alexithymia (23, 24). Finally, poor recognition of facial negative emotions is linked to both high levels of alexithymia and low levels of interoception (25).

Evidence from the Frontotemporal Dementias

An important degenerative disorder that alters emotion is behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). This disorder is the second most common neurodegenerative dementia among patients <65 years, after early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (26). Patients with bvFTD have behavioral changes such as impaired emotionality and empathy, apathy or abulia, disinhibition or impulsivity, dietary changes, and dysexecutive cognition from disease affecting the frontal and anterior lobes (26). The early focus of neuropathology in this disorder is in the anterior insulae, ACC, and the salience network.
In addition to bvFTD, the frontotemporal dementias include semantic dementia, also known as semantic variant primary progressive aphasia in its early stages. Semantic dementia results in a loss of semantics, or the meaning of words and objects, from atrophy in the anterior temporal lobes (27). The earliest changes in semantic dementia affect the meaning of words, manifesting as a semantic anomia, but as the disease progresses, there is a multimodal impairment in knowledge for objects, faces, sounds, smells, and other perceptions, regardless of modality, including confusion over the nature of somatic or bodily sensations (27).

Alexithymia in bvFTD

Most patients with bvFTD have emotional blunting with impaired recognition of emotions and decreased psychophysiological arousal to emotional stimuli (2833). Studies have shown that these patients are impaired in the recognition of emotions in pictures or videos (34, 35). Multiple studies document lower baseline tonic levels and decreased or insufficient phasic reactivity in both sympathetic and parasympathetic activity in bvFTD (11, 28, 30, 32, 36, 37). Patients with FTD have shown reduced sensitivity to different types of emotional stimuli, such as pain (29), fear conditioning to a loud noise (38), and aversive odors (39); when exposed to an unpleasant stimulus, patients with bvFTD had attenuation of the expected initial heart rate deceleration characteristic of an orienting response (33). Hypoemotionality also contributes to the lack of insight for their disease, which is best characterized as an “anosodiaphoria,” or lack of emotional caring, rather than a true anosognosia, or lack of cognitive awareness (40). The emotional blunting and decreased physiological arousal in bvFTD may correspond with atrophy or hypometabolism involving the anterior insulae and pregenual and middle cingulate cortex (37).
Beyond their impaired recognition of emotions and decreased physiological arousal, patients with bvFTD cannot accurately read their own interoceptive signals and do not reliably report their degree of emotional arousal (32). Patients with bvFTD have impaired explicit awareness of their own heartbeat (41), and they are unable to assess internal sensations for “gut level” cues (42). Investigators document a mismatch between intrinsic physiological arousal (implicitly feeling emotions) and reports of emotion or ratings of valence from the patients (explicit approval) (32). In general, those with bvFTD experience emotional stimuli as decreased in intensity, and they cannot estimate it except for differentiation of extremes of arousal (32). However, rather than underestimating emotional stimuli, patients with bvFTD may exaggerate their interoceptive sensations from an overreliance on cognitive appraisal using external contextual information, established rules, or environmental cues to explain their feelings (32, 43, 44).

Alexisomia in Semantic Dementia

Semantic dementia is a unique frontotemporal degeneration syndrome that affects the temporal poles, which are convergence zones for multimodal representations of knowledge, including interoceptive signals (45). In semantic dementia, dysfunction of anterior temporal regions can impair semantic meaning of interoceptive information. This results in impaired interoceptive accuracy from a disassociation of somatic sensations from semantic knowledge (46, 47). “Alexisomia,” or difficulty in reading somatic symptoms, is the alexithymia-related phenomenon in which somatic sensations are misread by patients with semantic dementia.
Because of their alexisomia, patients with semantic dementia may be prone to somatic symptom disorder from inability to identify the meaning of bodily sensations (48). Ten percent of autopsy-confirmed semantic dementia patients have presented with somatization, especially those with prominent right temporal lobe atrophy (49). They may become excessively concerned and “somaticize” over their normal bodily sensations because of confusion over their meaning, origin, and identity, and may even conclude that their unknown somatic sensations or symptoms mean that they are deteriorating or dying (Cotard’s syndrome) (50). Although pain and temperature awareness may be increased in patients with semantic dementia (29), this increased sensory awareness does not seem to reflect genuine hypersensitivity but rather an overattribution of salience in semantic dementia (29) (i.e., because they do not know what the sensations mean, they are primed to overreact to them [49]).

Conclusions

Alexithymia is an important but poorly understood symptom that is present among a wide range of psychiatric disorders. Early descriptions of the mechanism of alexithymia suggested a primary disorder from a dissociation of “limbic” or interoceptive physiological signals from cortical representations or interpretations (15, 16). Investigators suggested that psychological trauma with overactive defense mechanisms blocked this “limbic-cortical” association; however, more recent studies point to disturbed interoceptive processing itself as central to alexithymia. Similar difficulties among two frontotemporal dementia syndromes, bvFTD and semantic dementia, shed light on the neurobiological mechanism of this symptom. Alexithymia in bvFTD results from a weak emotional and physiological signal with impaired interoceptive accuracy and a consequent overreliance on external context and rules for explaining the experience. Alexisomia in semantic dementia results from inability to correctly read intact interoception due to an impairment in the ability to derive the meaning of bodily sensations. These findings in brain syndromes known to affect the emotional centers of the brain, particularly insulae where interoception is centered, support a deficit in interoceptive accuracy as central to alexithymia from a blunted emotional physiological signal, from impaired cognitive appraisal, or from disturbance in interoceptive processing in the insulae. Clearly, more research can offer further insights into the neurobiological basis of alexithymia based on this model.

References

1.
Sifneos PE, Apfel-Savitz R, Frankel FH : The phenomenon of alexithymia: observations in neurotic and psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom 1977 ; 28 : 47 – 57
2.
López-Muñoz F, Pérez-Fernández F : A history of the alexithymia concept and its explanatory models: an epistemological perspective. Front Psychiatry 2020 ; 10 : 1026
3.
Serafini G, De Berardis D, Valchera A, et al : Alexithymia as a possible specifier of adverse outcomes: clinical correlates in euthymic unipolar individuals. J Affect Disord 2020 ; 263 : 428 – 436
4.
McDougall J : Alexithymia: a psychoanalytic viewpoint. Psychother Psychosom 1982 ; 38 : 81 – 90
5.
Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JD : The alexithymia construct: a potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics 1991 ; 32 : 153 – 164
6.
Grynberg D, Chang B, Corneille O, et al : Alexithymia and the processing of emotional facial expressions (EFEs): systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectives. PLoS One 2012 ; 7 : e42429
7.
Heaton P, Reichenbacher L, Sauter D, et al : Measuring the effects of alexithymia on perception of emotional vocalizations in autistic spectrum disorder and typical development. Psychol Med 2012 ; 42 : 2453 – 2459
8.
Trevisan DA, Bowering M, Birmingham E : Alexithymia, but not autism spectrum disorder, may be related to the production of emotional facial expressions. Mol Autism 2016 ; 7 : 46
9.
Murphy J, Brewer R, Catmur C, et al : Interoception and psychopathology: a developmental neuroscience perspective. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2017 ; 23 : 45 – 56
10.
Craig AD : How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009 ; 10 : 59 – 70
11.
Sturm VE, Brown JA, Hua AY, et al : Network architecture underlying basal autonomic outflow: evidence from frontotemporal dementia. J Neurosci 2018 ; 38 : 8943 – 8955
12.
Seth AK : Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cogn Sci 2013 ; 17 : 565 – 573
13.
Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y : The enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 2007 ; 130 : 1718 – 1731
14.
TenHouten WD, Hoppe KD, Bogen JE, et al : Alexithymia: an experimental study of cerebral commissurotomy patients and normal control subjects. Am J Psychiatry 1986 ; 143 : 312 – 316
15.
Meza-Concha N, Arancibia M, Salas F, et al : Towards a neurobiological understanding of alexithymia. Medwave 2017 ; 17 : e6960
16.
MacLean PD : Psychosomatic disease and the visceral brain; recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosom Med 1949 ; 11 : 338 – 353
17.
Murphy J, Catmur C, Bird G : Alexithymia is associated with a multidomain, multidimensional failure of interoception: evidence from novel tests. J Exp Psychol Gen 2018 ; 147 : 398 – 408
18.
Brewer R, Cook R, Bird G : Alexithymia: a general deficit of interoception. R Soc Open Sci 2016 ; 3 : 150664
19.
Herbert BM, Herbert C, Pollatos O : On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J Pers 2011 ; 79 : 1149 – 1175
20.
Longarzo M, D’Olimpio F, Chiavazzo A, et al : The relationships between interoception and alexithymic trait: the Self-Awareness Questionnaire in healthy subjects. Front Psychol 2015 ; 6 : 1149
21.
Shah P, Hall R, Catmur C, et al : Alexithymia, not autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex 2016 ; 81 : 215 – 220
22.
Gaigg SB, Cornell AS, Bird G : The psychophysiological mechanisms of alexithymia in autism spectrum disorder. Autism 2018 ; 22 : 227 – 231
23.
Hogeveen J, Bird G, Chau A, et al : Acquired alexithymia following damage to the anterior insula. Neuropsychologia 2016 ; 82 : 142 – 148
24.
Goerlich-Dobre KS, Bruce L, Martens S, et al : Distinct associations of insula and cingulate volume with the cognitive and affective dimensions of alexithymia. Neuropsychologia 2014 ; 53 : 284 – 292
25.
Cook R, Brewer R, Shah P, et al : Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci 2013 ; 24 : 723 – 732
26.
Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, et al : Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 2011 ; 134 : 2456 – 2477
27.
Hodges JR, Mitchell J, Dawson K, et al : Semantic dementia: demography, familial factors and survival in a consecutive series of 100 cases. Brain 2010 ; 133 : 300 – 306
28.
Joshi A, Mendez MF, Kaiser N, et al : Skin conductance levels may reflect emotional blunting in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2014 ; 26 : 227 – 232
29.
Fletcher PD, Downey LE, Golden HL, et al : Pain and temperature processing in dementia: a clinical and neuroanatomical analysis. Brain 2015 ; 138 : 3360 – 3372
30.
Guo CC, Sturm VE, Zhou J, et al : Dominant hemisphere lateralization of cortical parasympathetic control as revealed by frontotemporal dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2016 ; 113 : E2430 – E2439
31.
Marshall CR, Hardy CJD, Russell LL, et al : The functional neuroanatomy of emotion processing in frontotemporal dementias. Brain 2019 ; 142 : 2873 – 2887
32.
Balconi M, Cotelli M, Brambilla M, et al : Understanding emotions in frontotemporal dementia: The explicit and implicit emotional cue mismatch. J Alzheimers Dis 2015 ; 46 : 211 – 225
33.
Joshi A, Jimenez E, Mendez MF : Pavlov’s orienting response in frontotemporal dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2017 ; 29 : 351 – 356
34.
Werner KH, Roberts NA, Rosen HJ, et al : Emotional reactivity and emotion recognition in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 2007 ; 69 : 148 – 155
35.
Marshall CR, Hardy CJD, Allen M, et al : Cardiac responses to viewing facial emotion differentiate frontotemporal dementias. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2018 ; 5 : 687 – 696
36.
Sturm VE, Sible IJ, Datta S, et al : Resting parasympathetic dysfunction predicts prosocial helping deficits in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Cortex 2018 ; 109 : 141 – 155
37.
Kumfor F, Hazelton JL, Rushby JA, et al : Facial expressiveness and physiological arousal in frontotemporal dementia: phenotypic clinical profiles and neural correlates. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2019 ; 19 : 197 – 210
38.
Hoefer M, Allison SC, Schauer GF, et al : Fear conditioning in frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2008 ; 131 : 1646 – 1657
39.
Perry DC, Datta S, Sturm VE, et al : Reward deficits in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia include insensitivity to negative stimuli. Brain 2017 ; 140 : 3346 – 3356
40.
Mendez MF, Shapira JS : Loss of emotional insight in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia or “frontal anosodiaphoria”. Conscious Cogn 2011 ; 20 : 1690 – 1696
41.
García-Cordero I, Sedeño L, de la Fuente L, et al : Feeling, learning from and being aware of inner states: interoceptive dimensions in neurodegeneration and stroke. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2016 ; 371 : 20160006
42.
Eckart JA, Sturm VE, Miller BL, et al : Diminished disgust reactivity in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 2012 ; 50 : 786 – 790
43.
Kumfor F, Ibañez A, Hutchings R, et al : Beyond the face: how context modulates emotion processing in frontotemporal dementia subtypes. Brain 2018 ; 141 : 1172 – 1185
44.
Carr AR, Paholpak P, Daianu M, et al : An investigation of care-based vs. rule-based morality in frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, and healthy controls. Neuropsychologia 2015 ; 78 : 73 – 79
45.
Fletcher PD, Warren JD : Semantic dementia: a specific network-opathy. J Mol Neurosci 2011 ; 45 : 629 – 636
46.
Kamminga J, Kumfor F, Burrell JR, et al : Differentiating between right-lateralised semantic dementia and behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: an examination of clinical characteristics and emotion processing. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2015 ; 86 : 1082 – 1088
47.
Marshall CR, Hardy CJD, Russell LL, et al : Impaired interoceptive accuracy in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Front Neurol 2017 ; 8 : 610
48.
Gan JJ, Lin A, Samimi MS, et al : Somatic symptom disorder in semantic dementia: The role of alexisomia. Psychosomatics 2016 ; 57 : 598 – 604
49.
Landqvist Waldö M, Santillo AF, Gustafson L, et al : Somatic complaints in frontotemporal dementia. Am J Neurodegener Dis 2014 ; 3 : 84 – 92
50.
Mendez MF, Ramírez-Bermúdez J : Cotard syndrome in semantic dementia. Psychosomatics 2011 ; 52 : 571 – 574

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 157 - 160
PubMed: 33535804

History

Received: 7 October 2020
Accepted: 21 October 2020
Published online: 4 February 2021
Published in print: Spring 2021

Keywords

  1. Frontotemporal Dementia
  2. Semantic Dementia
  3. Alexithymia

Authors

Affiliations

Mario F. Mendez, M.D., Ph.D. [email protected]
Departments of Neurology and Behavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles; and Neurology Service, Neurobehavior Unit, VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Mendez ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Supported by the National Institute on Aging (grant 1RF1AG050967).Dr. Mendez reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share