Skip to main content
Full access
Reviews & Overviews
Published Online: 15 June 2021

Beliefs About the Causes of Psychosis Among Persons With Psychosis and Mental Health Professionals: A Scoping Review

Abstract

Objective:

The beliefs that people with psychosis hold about causes of their illness (causal beliefs) can affect their choice to adhere to treatment and engage in mental health services. However, less is known about causal beliefs of mental health professionals (MHPs) and their impact on treatment adherence and service engagement. This review explored literature focusing on MHPs’ causal beliefs and mapped the degree of concordance between their causal beliefs and those of people with psychosis.

Methods:

A systematic literature search of PubMed, Embase, Scopus, PsycINFO, and Applied Social Sciences Index Abstracts and a gray-literature search of PsyArXiv and MedNar yielded 11,821 eligible references. The first author reviewed all titles and abstracts, and the coauthors reviewed 10% (N=1,200).

Results:

Forty-two articles were included. Most articles indicated that MHPs tend to endorse biogenetic beliefs (9 of 15 articles assessing MHPs’ beliefs, 60%), whereas people with psychosis tend to endorse psychosocial beliefs (16 of 31 articles, 52%) and other nonbiogenetic beliefs (in 8 of 31 articles, 26%). Most studies did not compare causal beliefs of people with psychosis and their treating MHP. Studies varied in design, setting, and measures.

Conclusions:

MHPs and people with psychosis often hold complex views composed of different types of causal beliefs. However, a gap in causal beliefs between these groups appears to exist, which may affect the therapeutic relationship and pose barriers to treatment adherence. Future studies should address this gap by developing interventions that facilitate open communication about causal beliefs to promote treatment alliance and shared decision making.

HIGHLIGHTS

To understand whether people with psychosis and mental health professionals differ in their beliefs about the causes of psychosis, a scoping review was conducted to identify and compare common causal beliefs of these groups.
Of 42 studies reviewed, most indicated that MHPs were more likely to endorse biogenetic causal beliefs, rather than psychosocial beliefs, whereas patients were more likely to endorse psychosocial causal beliefs, rather than biogenetic beliefs.
Research that focuses on gaps in causal beliefs between MHPs and people with psychosis and its effect on the treatment alliance and treatment adherence is needed.
“Causal beliefs” refer to beliefs regarding the causes of a condition; among persons who have the condition in question, such beliefs are often understood as part of active attempts to understand and cope with health threats. Causal beliefs influence emotional responses, coping strategies, and treatment choices (1). Among individuals with psychosis, a growing body of literature highlights the potential impact of causal beliefs on adherence behavior and service engagement (2, 3). Nonadherence to antipsychotic medication and disengagement from psychiatric services are highly prevalent, and although nonadherence and disengagement can be a manifestation of personal choice (4, 5), they are also associated with higher rates of relapse and readmission among persons with psychosis (68), making targeting causal beliefs in this population especially important.
At least six types of causal beliefs about psychosis can be identified in the literature: biogenetic, psychosocial, spiritual-religious, related to substance abuse (3, 9), related to personal characteristics (e.g., lack of willpower) (10), and as a part of the human experience (1113). Biogenetic beliefs about psychosis highlight genetics or heritability and emphasize pharmacological treatment to target presumed biological abnormalities (14). Psychosocial beliefs stress the role of psychological, social, and environmental influences (e.g., socialization, trauma, and upbringing) in the development of psychosis and emphasize psychosocial interventions, such as psychotherapy and recovery-oriented interventions, to target cognitions, emotions, and behavior (15). Spiritual-religious beliefs focus on spiritual explanations of the occurrence of psychosis, such as God’s will, witchcraft, or evil spirits (16). Substance abuse–related beliefs focus on drugs or alcohol as the reason for the occurrence of psychosis (17). Beliefs related to personal characteristics emphasize personality and other individual characteristics that contribute to the development of psychosis (10). Finally, beliefs that the psychotic experience is part of human experience emphasize psychosis as a part of reality, such as believing that one has special powers or that voices are real (11, 12, 18).
A recent systematic review that focused on the relationship between causal beliefs among people with psychosis and their outcomes found that people with psychosis mostly endorsed psychosocial beliefs and that their health outcomes were related to the type of causal beliefs they held (3). For example, biogenetic beliefs were associated with higher rates of medication adherence, whereas psychosocial beliefs were associated with greater psychotherapy engagement. Spiritual beliefs were associated with longer duration of untreated psychosis. Another significant finding was that causal beliefs were related to stigma: people with psychosis who endorsed a biogenetic causal belief were more likely to experience stigma and to exhibit stigmatizing attitudes toward others with psychosis. This relationship between biogenetic causal beliefs and stigma was also previously found among mental health professionals (MHPs) (19, 20) and the general public (21). Causal beliefs related to personal characteristics have also been found to be associated with stigma and negative attitudes toward mental illness among people with psychosis (2224) and the general public (10).
However, less attention has focused on causal beliefs about psychosis held by MHPs, and little is known about possible disagreements between MHPs and people with psychosis regarding causal beliefs. Research indicates that MHPs may hold different types of causal beliefs, compared with people with psychosis (e.g., 2527). However, understanding whether causal beliefs are a point of disagreement between MHPs and people with psychosis requires evaluating the existing evidence and mapping the types of causal beliefs that MHPs tend to hold. Therefore, the purpose of this review was to explore the scope and breadth of the literature focusing on MHPs’ causal beliefs and to map causal beliefs among MHPs and people with psychosis and the degree of concordance between them. Specifically, we focused on the following exploratory questions: What are common causal beliefs about psychosis among MHPs? How different are they from those of people with psychosis? What is the impact of MHPs’ profession on their causal beliefs? What is the impact of culture and psychiatric setting?

Methods

To identify studies for consideration or inclusion in this scoping review, we worked with a medical librarian to develop detailed search strategies for each database. (Details about the search strategy are provided in an online supplement to this article.) The search was developed for PubMed and was translated to Embase, Scopus, PsycINFO, and Applied Social Sciences Index Abstracts. A gray-literature search included PsyArXiv and MedNar. The search included no restrictions on publication date. The final database search was completed on March 26, 2019, and the gray-literature search was completed on March 27, 2019. We supplemented the results of the systematic search with relevant articles not identified with use of the search criteria but which we considered important for review purposes.
Studies included were those published in English that employed at least one measure specifically assessing causal beliefs about psychosis and that reported the content and endorsement levels of causal beliefs in the results. Furthermore, we included only studies that directly assessed people with psychosis (adults at least 18 years old who reported experiencing psychotic symptoms [e.g., hearing voices] or who had a diagnosis of schizophrenia spectrum disorder [schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, schizophreniform disorder, brief psychotic disorder, or psychotic disorder not otherwise specified]) or MHPs (psychiatrists, psychiatric nurses, psychologists, social workers, case managers, occupational therapists, and support workers). Studies excluded were review articles; studies with providers who were peer support workers, primary care physicians, or medical students; studies not reporting the content and levels of endorsement of causal beliefs; and studies that explored endorsement of only one type of causal belief (e.g., only biogenetic), because such studies did not allow for comparison of the endorsed types of causal beliefs.
The systematic search yielded 17,029 studies, with 42 from gray-literature sources (see PRISMA flow diagram [28] illustrating study selection process in the online supplement). After the removal of duplicates, 11,821 references were eligible for screening. Screening was performed with Covidence (covidence.org), the recommended standard platform for Cochrane reviews. The first author reviewed all titles and abstracts (N=11,821), and all other coauthors reviewed the first 10% of the titles and abstracts (N=1,200) for reliability. This process yielded high agreement rates, ranging from 96.3% to 99.3%. We discussed disagreements until agreement was reached. Screening titles and abstracts resulted in the exclusion of 11,681 records that failed to meet the inclusion criteria. The full text of the remaining 140 articles was screened by the first author with consultation, as needed, with the second and last author, which led to the exclusion of 101 articles that failed to meet the inclusion criteria. An additional three full-text articles (25, 29, 30) were excluded because they described the same sample and results of the same study (3133). Six additional articles were added after review of Carter et al.’s (3) references list and prior knowledge of the authors, and the final sample included 42 articles.
Data were charted by using a predesigned form. We charted the following descriptive data: sample characteristics (people with psychosis, MHPs, and recruitment setting), study characteristics (study design, sample size, country, and assessment schedule), measurement of causal belief, and study results (types and level of causal beliefs endorsed) (see table in online supplement).
The coding of causal beliefs into categories was conducted iteratively by four authors (R.R.O., D.R., I.H.-O., Y.Z.-I.) by using an a priori categorization system that was adjusted and developed throughout the process. The first author (R.R.O.) coded the results of each of the included articles; the coding was then reviewed by the second (D.R.), third (I.H.-O.), and last (Y.Z.-I.) authors. This step included categorization based on an a priori three-category system: biogenetic, psychosocial, and spiritual-religious. The second step included adjustment of the category definitions on the basis of existing literature to include causal beliefs that did not fall into the above three categories. This step resulted in the addition of three categories (beliefs related to substance abuse, related to personal characteristics, and as a part of the human experience), yielding a total of six categories (3, 17, 18, 34, 35).
We used the Critical Appraisal Skills Program (CASP) (36) to assess the methodological quality of the qualitative studies. The tool contains 10 questions and assesses quality in three domains: validity, presentation, and impact of study results. We used the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) Critical Appraisal Checklist for Analytical Cross Sectional Studies (37), the appraisal tool most preferred for such studies (38). The tool contains eight questions assessing sample and setting and measurements of exposure and outcome and addressing confounders and analysis. We also used the JBI Critical Appraisal Checklist for Studies Reporting Prevalence Data (39). The tool contains nine questions assessing sample and setting, validity, analysis, and response rates. The risk of bias was evaluated by the first author, who consulted with the second and last authors in cases of uncertainty. Disagreements were discussed among these three authors until agreement was reached.

Results

Methodological Quality of the Included Articles

Quality appraisal of the articles that employed qualitative methods indicated that two articles met all 10 criteria suggested by the CASP (13, 35), one article met nine of the criteria (18), four met eight (34, 4042), and the remaining met seven (43, 44), six (45), and five (46). The quality appraisal of the articles that included quantitative research methods indicated that only two articles (30, 47) met all the criteria suggested by the JBI Critical Appraisal Checklist for Analytical Cross Sectional Studies. The identification and management of confounding factors received the highest percentage of high-risk ratings in analytical cross-sectional studies. The validation of measurements of causal beliefs received the highest percentage of high risk of bias in descriptive cross-sectional studies. (The quality appraisal tables are available in the online supplement.)
Some of the included articles did not present precise endorsement rates of causal beliefs: seven articles (25, 4853) presented percentages without Ns, 10 articles presented means (27, 29, 43, 5460), and seven of the qualitative articles (13, 18, 26, 34, 35, 45, 46) and four of the quantitative articles (2, 22, 61, 62) did not present percentages or means. Therefore, we calculated approximate Ns based on percentages and total Ns where possible and present existing data in other cases.

Causal Beliefs of MHPs

Common types of causal beliefs about psychosis.

Of the 42 studies included, 15 (36%) explored causal beliefs of MHPs from a range of professions: psychiatrists (23, 26, 57, 59, 62); psychiatric nurses (25, 60, 63); and psychologists, social workers, occupational therapists, care coordinators, and mental health support workers (24, 27, 45, 52, 53, 55, 58, 61) (Table 1). Three of the studies are from the 1970s, which limited the comparability of results from different studies (58, 61, 62). Nine studies found a clear preference for biogenetic beliefs about psychosis among MHPs (2427, 53, 57, 59, 60, 63); six studies reported the endorsement of psychosocial beliefs alongside biogenetic beliefs (25, 52, 55, 59, 62, 63), suggesting that although MHPs are more inclined toward biogenetic beliefs about psychosis, they often hold complex causal models that include several beliefs simultaneously.
TABLE 1. Studies of causal beliefs about psychosis among mental health professionals (MHPs)
StudyCountryParticipantsNSettingOutcomesa
Carter et al., 2017 (55)United KingdomCommunity psychiatric nurses, social workers, psychiatrists, staff nurses, care coordinators, psychologists, team managers, occupational therapists, support workers, and others219Mental health centersThe study used 5-point scales, with higher scores indicating greater endorsement. MHPs’ scores were higher on the psychosocial scale (PS), compared with the biogenetic scale (BG) (M=4.11±1.63 vs. M=3.75±1.64). Psychologists’ PS scores were considerably higher than their BG scores (M=4.72±1.19 vs. M=2.29±1.88). Other professionals’ PS scores were similar to their BG scores. Psychiatrists were the only group to have higher BG scores, compared with their PS scores (M=4.38±.94 vs. 4.07±1.70).
Fitzgibbons and Shearn, 1972 (61)United StatesPsychiatrists, psychologists, and psychiatric social workers183Multiple settingsPsychiatrists endorsed the disease concept of schizophrenia and rejected interpersonal etiology. Psychologists and social workers endorsed interpersonal etiology while rejecting the disease concept of schizophrenia and did not differ significantly from each other with respect to these 2 factors. Endorsement rates were not reported.
Gallagher, 1977 (62)United StatesPsychiatrists109Members of the American Psychiatric AssociationNo clear preference was found for either biogenetic or psychosociogenetic views. Endorsement rates were not reported.
Grausgruber et al., 2007 (52)AustriaNonmedical mental health professionals, including psychiatric nurses, social workers, psychologists, physiotherapists, and occupational therapists1,479Mental health care institutions (mailed survey)MHPs endorsed a multicausal model of schizophrenia, including unhappy family situation (31%, N=∼454), genes (26%, N=∼390), and nervous strain (26%, N=∼389).
Harland et al., 2009 (57)United KingdomTrainee psychiatrists72Multiple settingsThe biological model was most frequently endorsed for schizophrenia. Two of the 3 statements most agreed with were related to schizophrenia: “The disorder results from brain dysfunction” (86% endorsed); “The appropriate study of the disorder involves discovery of biological markers and the effects of biological interventions” (80%). Schizophrenia was the least likely disorder to be attributed to other etiological models (i.e., nonbiological).
Kukulu and Ergün, 2007 (63)TurkeyPsychiatric nurses693Psychiatric wardsApproximately half (51%, N=279) of the nurses agreed that social problems caused schizophrenia, and most (93%, N=506) agreed that it is an illness present from birth.
Magliano et al., 2004 (25)ItalyNurses and psychiatrists300bMental health servicesFactors most frequently endorsed were heredity (psychiatrists, 75%, N=∼82; nurses, 74%, N=∼141), stress (psychiatrists, 66%, N=∼7; nurses, 53%, N=∼101), and family conflicts (psychiatrists, 46%, N=∼51; nurses, 48%, N=∼91).
Newmark et al., 1977 (58)United StatesPsychologists and psychiatrists381cMultiple settingsCompared with psychologists, psychiatrists rated both the psychodynamic perspective (p≤.001) and the biochemical-neurological perspective (p≤.001) as significantly more important.
Soskis, 1972 (59)United StatesPsychiatrists132Multiple settingsOn a scale of 1–3, psychiatrists’ highest endorsement was for the genetic perspective (M=2.2), closely followed by psychodynamic (M=2.1) and family-learning (M=2.1) perspectives.
Ting, 1997 (60)TaiwanPsychiatric nurses525HospitalsGenetic predisposition was ranked as the most important factor in the etiology of schizophrenia (M=1.83, rank order 1), followed by constitutional/biochemical imbalance (M=1.08, rank order 2).
Wahass and Kent, 1997 (53)United Kingdom and Saudi ArabiaPsychologists and psychiatrists295dMailed questionnaires sent to professionals in mental health sectorsBeliefs about etiology were related to culture, rather than to profession. For example, United Kingdom (UK) staff were more likely than staff in Saudi Arabia (SA) to endorse brain damage (UK psychologists, 57%, N=∼40; UK psychiatrists, 62%, N=∼53; SA psychologists, 36%, N=∼25; SA psychiatrists, 40%, N=∼28), negative childhood experiences (UK psychologists, 36%, N=∼25; UK psychiatrists, 22%, N=∼19; SA psychologists, 8%, N=∼6; SA psychiatrists, 6%, N=∼4).
a
When Ns were not reported, estimated Ns (e.g., N=∼454) were calculated on the basis of the total N presented in the article and the reported percentage.
b
190 nurses, 110 psychiatrists.
c
219 psychologists, 162 psychiatrists.
d
140 psychologists, 155 psychiatrists.

Impact of clinical profession on type of causal beliefs.

Endorsement of causal beliefs was related to profession. One study compared causal beliefs of MHPs from different professions (55). All 12 psychologists who participated in the study endorsed psychosocial beliefs about psychosis (childhood trauma, adulthood trauma, and recent stress), whereas only three (25%) endorsed beliefs about the role of genetics, and two (17%) endorsed beliefs about chemical imbalance. Other nonmedical MHPs in this study (N=186) endorsed biogenetic beliefs (genetics, 48%, N=∼90; and chemical imbalance, 45%, N=∼84) and psychosocial beliefs (childhood trauma, 60%, N=∼111; and adulthood trauma, 50%, N=∼94) to a similar extent (55). Conversely, psychiatrists (N=21) were the only group with a higher endorsement of biogenetic beliefs (genetics, 100%, N=21; chemical imbalance, 91%, N=∼19) compared with psychosocial beliefs (childhood trauma, 81%, N=∼17; and adulthood trauma, 81%, N=∼17). Eight articles focused on psychiatrists and nurses (2527, 57, 5961, 63) and reported that they were more likely to endorse biogenetic beliefs compared with psychosocial beliefs. In four of these studies, endorsement of biogenetic beliefs ranged between 75% and 100%, and endorsement of psychosocial beliefs ranged between 48% and 81% (25, 26, 57, 63). The other four studies reported similar results without presenting percentages and Ns, indicating that psychiatrists and nurses saw psychosis more as a biological disorder than as a condition that is caused by psychosocial factors (27, 5961).
One study showed that this biological view was more salient for psychosis than for other mental conditions (57). In this study psychiatrists endorsed biogenetic beliefs about psychosis with great conviction, whereas for other conditions, such as major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and antisocial personality disorder, psychiatrists exhibited other sets of beliefs, not necessarily biogenetic. Specifically, two of the three statements receiving the most agreement in the study were related to psychosis: “The disorder results from brain dysfunction” (86%, N=62); “The appropriate study of the disorder involves discovery of biological markers and the effects of biological interventions” (80%, N=57).
Of note, Newmark et al. (58) found opposite results for the impact of clinical profession on endorsement of causal beliefs. The lack of reported data prevents reporting the specific percentages and Ns; however, the authors reported that psychologists consensually endorsed biogenetic beliefs, whereas psychiatrists did not show a clear preference for a specific causal belief. However, the results of Newmark et al. (58) should be interpreted with caution, because the study included a variety of work settings: psychiatrists and psychologists working in private practice emphasized a psychosocial perspective, whereas psychiatrists and psychologists working in state or university hospitals emphasized the biogenetic perspective.

Cultural differences among MHPs.

Only one study compared causal beliefs of MHPs from different cultures. Wahass and Kent (53) explored attitudes of psychologists and psychiatrists toward auditory hallucinations, comparing MHPs from the United Kingdom (UK) and Saudi Arabia (SA). The results showed that causal beliefs about psychosis varied by MHP nationality. UK staff were more likely than SA staff to endorse psychosocial beliefs: 36% of UK psychologists (N=∼16) and 22% of UK psychiatrists (N=∼10) endorsed negative childhood experiences as a cause, compared with 8% (N=∼4) of SA psychologists and 6% (N=∼3) of SA psychiatrists. Almost half (40%, N=∼18) of the UK psychologists and more than a quarter (28%, N=∼13) of the UK psychiatrists endorsed environmental factors, compared with 10% (N=∼5) of the SA psychologists and 8% (N=∼4) of the SA psychiatrists. Most of the UK psychologists (64%, N=∼29) and a third (34%, N=∼16) of the UK psychiatrists endorsed stressful life events, compared with 40% (N=∼20) of the SA psychologists and 10% (N=∼5) of the SA psychiatrists. UK psychologists were more likely to endorse a genetic component (38%, N=∼17), compared with SA psychologists (14%, N=∼6). Therefore, UK MHPs presented a wider range of possible causal beliefs, compared with SA MHPs. Of interest, psychologists and psychiatrists within the same culture tended to agree with each other about etiology.

Causal Beliefs of People With Psychosis

Common types of causal beliefs about psychosis.

Of the included articles, 35 focused on people with psychosis and indicated that their most endorsed causal beliefs were psychosocial (13, 22, 23, 26, 27, 29, 43, 45, 48, 50, 51, 54, 56, 6466) and other nonbiogenetic beliefs (30, 34, 35, 4042, 46, 49, 67) (Table 2). For example, Magliano et al. (50) found that most of the individuals with psychosis who reported on their causal beliefs (76%, N=150 of 198) mentioned at least one social cause and that more than half (58%, N=114 of 198) reported exclusively social causes. Family conflicts (41%, N=∼81) were the most commonly reported social cause, followed by trauma (39%, N=∼77), work and school difficulties (34%, N=∼67), and psychological issues (33%, N=∼65). Similarly, Carter et al. (54) found that people with psychosis endorsed psychosocial beliefs (childhood trauma, 54%, N=∼167; adulthood trauma, 60%, N=∼187; recent stress, 57%, N=∼177; and personal sensitivity, 64%, N=∼197) more often than they did biogenetic beliefs (hereditary factors, 45%, N=∼140; and chemical imbalance, 46%, N=∼144). Conversely, Van Dorn et al. (24) found a dominance of biogenetic causal beliefs over psychosocial beliefs among people with psychosis. The most frequently endorsed cause was chemical imbalance (89%, N=∼93). These different findings might be attributable to their asking participants about a hypothetical person with psychosis, rather than asking them about their own experiences.
TABLE 2. Studies of causal beliefs about psychosis among people with psychosis
StudyCountryNaSettingOutcomesb
Angermeyer and Klusmann, 1988 (48)Germany198Psychiatric hospitalsMost patients chose a combination of two or more categories: 64% (N=∼126) endorsed “family” as a cause, 71% (N=∼140) endorsed “personality,” 88% (N=∼174) endorsed “recent psychosocial factors,” and 32% (N=∼62) endorsed “biology.”
Caqueo-Urízar et al., 2015 (29)Chile, Peru, and Bolivia253Public clinicsParticipants rated causal factors on a 3-point scale, with higher scores indicating a stronger endorsement of the item as a cause of psychosis. Scores were higher for psychosocial factors (M=2.69±2.83) than for biological (M=1.67±1.73) and magical-religious (M=1.16±1.92) factors.
Carter et al., 2018 (13)United Kingdom15Community mental health (CMH) teams, early intervention teams (EITs)The category of belief most frequently endorsed was psychosocial. Other categories (biological, drug use, and unusual beliefs) were also frequently mentioned. In this qualitative study, endorsement rates were not reported.
Carter et al., 2018 (54)England311CMH teams, EITs, and inpatient unitsParticipants rated causal factors on a 5-point scale, with higher scores indicating a stronger endorsement of the item as a cause of psychosis. Scores were higher on the psychosocial scale (M=3.37±1.06), compared with the biogenetic scale (M=3.13±1.15). Scores were lowest on the spiritual scale (M=2.30±1.34).
Charles et al., 2007 (49)India100HospitalMost held complex models of illness: 60% (N=∼60) held at least one nonbiomedical causal belief, and 32% (N=∼32) endorsed disease as a cause. Spiritual beliefs were widely held. Patients’ endorsement of a disease model and a belief in karma and evil spirits as causes of illness were associated with higher stigma scores.
Chilale et al., 2017 (46)Malawi24Different sources in the community, including traditional healers (participants in an earlier study were approached).Among causal beliefs, sociocultural factors, such as witchcraft, spirit possession, and curses, were dominant. In this qualitative study, endorsement rates were not reported.
Conrad et al., 2007 (43)Jordan and Germany47cPsychiatric hospitals (1 Jordanian, 2 German)Participants rated causal beliefs on a 5-point scale, with higher scores indicating a stronger endorsement of the item as a cause of psychosis. For both groups, the most endorsed belief was psychosocial stress (Germans, M=25.8±4.9; Jordanians, M=25.5±6.5). Jordanians endorsed supernatural factors more strongly, compared with Germans (M=10.1±5.1 vs. M=7.7±4.3).
Dudley et al., 2009 (42)United Kingdom21EITsThe most endorsed cause was drug use (N=6, 29% of the variance). None of the factors reflected a “medical model” of psychosis.
Freeman et al., 2013 (66)United Kingdom92Mental health services, both inpatient and outpatientThe most endorsed causes were psychosocial: stress (72%, N=65 of 90), state of mind (68%, N=61 of 89), other people (64%, N=58 of 90,), and personality (60%, N=54 of 90).
Gómez-de-Regil, 2014 (56)Mexico62HospitalParticipants rated causal factors on a 4-point scale, with higher scores indicating a stronger endorsement of the item as a cause of psychosis. Participants rated society factors highest (M=12.7±4.1), followed by personality (M=11.9±3.7), family (M=11.1±4.5), biology (M=10.4±3.5), and esoteric (M=8.8±3.1).
Holzinger et al., 2003 (23)Germany100Outpatient clinics, CMH services, and office-based psychiatristsParticipants cited psychosocial causes about twice as often as biogenetic causes. The main psychosocial cause cited was psychosocial stress (66%, N=66).
Hussain et al., 2017 (22)Pakistan100HospitalThe most frequently endorsed beliefs were psychosocial. Beliefs least likely to be endorsed were related to alcohol and drugs. Endorsement rates were not reported.
Johnson et al., 2012 (30)India131HospitalsEndorsed causes were spiritual (black magic, 73%, N=96; evil spirits, 18%, N=23; and punishment by God, 11%, N=14), hereditary factors (<1%, N=1), disease (14%, N=17), and psychosocial factors (11%, N=14). About a fifth of participants (22%) endorsed models with multiple causal factors. The proportion endorsing disease models steadily increased over time. The number endorsing nonmedical models was high at recruitment, fell over the first year of treatment, and then rose dramatically.
Jones et al., 2003 (40)United Kingdom20Different sources in the community“Positive spiritual perspective” was the most endorsed factor (N=7). Those whose responses loaded onto this factor perceived voices as positive experiences, derived from spiritual sources. They condemned a biomedical view of hearing voices. The second most-endorsed factor was the personal relevance perspective (N=4), in which hearing voices was related to personal life events within a psychological framework.
Kinderman et al., 2006 (35)United Kingdom20d7 psychiatrists referred inpatients and outpatientsParticipants held multiple beliefs simultaneously. The most common account implied an interaction between personal characteristics and psychosocial stresses. In this qualitative study, endorsement rates were not reported.
Lund and Swartz, 1998 (41)South Africa10Community psychiatry clinicMost respondents (N=7) understood their condition in terms of a “spiritual” or “mystical” explanation.
Magliano et al., 2009 (50)Italy241Mental health clinicsAt least one social cause was cited by 76% (N=150 of 198), and 58% (N=114 of 198) cited exclusively social causes. The most frequently cited social cause was family conflicts (41%, N=∼81), followed by traumas (39%, N=∼77). Biological causes were cited by 10% (N=∼20). In regard to stigma, those who attributed their mental disorder to social causes scored lower on recognizability, compared with those who made other attributions (lower scores indicated that they were less likely to feel they would be recognized as a person with mental illness).
Makanjuola et al., 2016 (44)Nigeria, Ghana, and Kenya85Clinics of complementary and alternative practitionersEndorsements were equal for spiritual (49%, N=64) and biopsychosocial (51%, N=66) causes. In regard to stigma, most who scored high in stigma tended to hold supernatural causal beliefs.
Maraj et al., 2017 (17)Canada171eEITsBlack Africans were less likely than White Europeans to attribute psychosis to hereditary factors (37%, N=15 vs. 57%, N=51) or to substance abuse (32%, N=13, vs. 52%, N=47). No differences in explanatory models were noted between the Black Caribbean and White European groups.
McCabe and Priebe, 2004 (47)United Kingdom119Mental health clinicsWhites cited biological causes (35%, N=10) more frequently, compared with African Caribbeans (7% N=2), Bangladeshis (N=0), and West Africans (11%, N=3). Supernatural causes were cited by African Caribbeans (10%, N=3), Bangladeshis (27%, N=7), and West Africans (29%, N=8) and were not cited at all by Whites. Social causes were cited more frequently by African Caribbeans (60%, N=18) and Bangladeshis (42%, N=11), compared with Whites (31%, N=9).
Sanders et al., 2011 (67)New Zealand111Mental health servicesThe three most frequently endorsed causes were drugs and alcohol (26%, N=14), family relationships and abuse (22%, N=12), and biological causes (20%, N=11). No significant differences in causal beliefs were detected between Māori and New Zealand Europeans.
Sayre, 2000 (18)United States35HospitalParticipants endorsed both psychosocial and biogenetic beliefs. In this qualitative study, endorsement rates were not reported.
Syrén and Hultsjö, 2014 (34)Sweden33Outpatient unitsPsychosocial and spiritual causes were the most frequently endorsed. In this qualitative study, endorsement rates were not reported. Some participants did not regard psychosis as a problem and believed that they had been exclusively selected for extraordinary missions.
Wall et al., 2017 (64)United Kingdom72fHospitalStress or worry was the most endorsed explanation among both forensic (65%, N=26) and general psychiatry (81%, N=32) inpatients. Forensic inpatients were more likely than general psychiatry inpatients to attribute psychosis to drug use (55%, N=22 vs. 38%, N=12).
Watson et al., 2006 (51)United Kingdom100National Health Service (NHS) TrustsInternal causal attributions were widely endorsed, with 68% (N=∼68) agreeing that “My state of mind played a major part in causing my current problems/illness.” Attributing psychosis to one’s state of mind was significantly related to higher levels of anxiety (rs=.27, p<.01) and attributing psychosis to stress was related to higher levels of self-esteem (rs=.27, p<.01),.
Williams and Steer, 2011 (2)United Kingdom663 NHS mental health centersThe most frequently endorsed beliefs were that psychosis was caused by stress or worry, a trauma, chemical imbalance, thinking about things too much, mental attitude, or family problems. Only descriptive information was presented regarding causal beliefs, because the scale’s items were not easily classified into meaningful dimensions, according to the authors.
Yalvaç et al., 2017 (65)Turkey148Outpatient psychiatry units“Internal problems” was the most endorsed cause by participants from Ankara (58%, N=57), followed by “family problems” (44%, N=44). Among those from Van province, the most endorsed cause was “no idea” (38%, N=18), followed by “family problems” (31%, N=15).
a
In all studies, all participants were people with psychosis (Johnson et al. [30] reported that participants had first-episode psychosis).
b
When Ns were not reported, estimated Ns (e.g., N=∼126) were calculated on the basis of the total N presented in the article and the reported percentage.
c
24 Jordanians, 23 Germans.
d
10 inpatients, 10 outpatients.
e
90 White European, 41 Black African, 40 Black Caribbean.
f
40 forensic, 32 general.
The studies found a preference for psychosocial beliefs among people with psychosis, however, like MHPs, they often held complex causal models composed of different types of causal beliefs (e.g., 27, 46, 49, 56, 64).

Spiritual-religious causal beliefs.

Seven studies reported that people with psychosis who came from cultures that value spirituality were more likely to endorse spiritual-religious causal beliefs about psychosis, compared with other types of beliefs (30, 40, 41, 4446, 49). For example, a qualitative study conducted in Malawi by Chilale et al. (46) found that participants attributed mental illness to sociocultural factors; witchcraft, spirit possession, and curses were the main determinants endorsed. Similarly, Jones et al. (40), who conducted their study in the UK but recruited participants from various settings, including two spiritualist churches and one evangelical Christian church, found that a factor that emerged in their factor analysis was “the positive spiritual perspective” (N=7 of 20 participants)—namely, the perception of voices as positive experiences derived from spiritual forces.
In line with these findings, differences in causal beliefs were found among people with psychosis of different nationalities. People from Western societies were found to endorse biogenetic and psychosocial causal beliefs more often than did people from non-Western countries, who were more likely to endorse spiritual-religious beliefs, compared with other types of beliefs. Six studies compared causal beliefs of people of different nationalities or different cultural backgrounds and reported on these differences (17, 29, 43, 47, 65, 67). For example, Conrad et al. (43) compared causal beliefs of people from Jordan and Germany and found that whereas both groups endorsed predominantly psychosocial beliefs—and specifically the belief in psychosocial stress as a cause (83% of Jordanians [N=∼20] and 61% of Germans [N=∼14])—Germans mentioned biogenetic beliefs more often (26% of the Germans [N=∼6] and none of the Jordanians), and Jordanians mentioned spiritual-religious beliefs more often (29% of the Jordanians [N=∼7] and none of the Germans).
Similar findings were reported from a study conducted in Canada (17). Black participants cited biogenetic beliefs (heredity, 37%, N=15) and beliefs related to substance abuse (32%, N=13) less often than did White participants (heredity, 57%, N=51; and substance abuse, 52%, N=47). Finally, a study conducted in the UK among four ethnic groups (47) found that causal beliefs differed significantly by ethnicity. Whites cited biological causes (35%, N=10) more frequently, compared with African Caribbeans (7%, N=2), Bangladeshis (N=0), and West Africans (11%, N=3); and these three groups cited supernatural causes more frequently than did Whites, who did not endorse this type of belief (10%, N=3; 27%, N=7; and 29%, N=8, respectively). African Caribbeans (60%, N=18) and Bangladeshis (42%, N=11) cited social causes more frequently than did Whites (31%, N=9).

Beliefs related to personal characteristics.

Eleven studies reported that people with psychosis often attributed their psychotic experiences to their personal characteristics (e.g., personal sensitivity or lack of willpower) (17, 18, 2224, 51, 56, 64, 66). Of note is that attributing psychosis to personal characteristics was also evident in other studies reviewed here, but it was measured as part of psychosocial beliefs (together with social-environmental causal beliefs) and not treated as a separate category. Although it makes sense to view beliefs related to personal characteristics and to social causes as intertwined, it seems that the former may have unique consequences for the individual’s self-esteem and coping strategies. Three studies defined beliefs related to personal characteristics as a form of self-blame (2224). Two studies reported on the negative impact of this belief. One found that endorsing this type of belief was associated with patients’ perception that the psychotic experience had a negative influence on their emotional state (p≤0.05) and on life in general (p≤0.001) (56). Watson et al. (51) found a relationship between internal causal attribution (i.e., attributing psychosis to the self) and higher levels of anxiety (p<0.01), whereas attribution of psychosis to external factors, such as stress, was related to higher self-esteem (p<0.01).
On the other hand, two studies suggested that beliefs related to personal characteristics may lead to higher motivation for change and to better engagement in psychotherapy (18, 66). Freeman et al. (66) explored factors predicting engagement in cognitive-behavioral therapy for psychosis. They found that people who were more engaged in therapy attributed the cause of their problems to their personality (p=0.015) and state of mind (p=0.003) compared with those who were less engaged. Similarly, in a qualitative study, Sayre (18) found that people who attributed their difficulties to their personality and behavior appeared to feel some responsibility and were motivated to make behavioral changes in order to cope with their problems. People in this group found psychiatric treatment helpful and felt that it enabled them to gain some control over their symptoms.

Beliefs that the psychotic experience is part of the human experience.

Several qualitative studies included in the review enabled a deeper understanding of causal beliefs that are often endorsed by people with psychosis (13, 18, 34, 35). These studies reported that some participants believed that their experiences were real and part of their general experience as human beings. These participants did not define the experiences as psychosis or as part of a mental illness. For example, Syrén and Hultsjö (34) analyzed data from two qualitative studies and reported that one of the four categories that emerged under “causes of illness” was “it is not a disease.” People who endorsed this belief thought that they had been selected for extraordinary missions and did not believe that their experience was a psychotic illness. In addition to the study by Syrén and Hultsjö (34), who studied outpatients, three additional studies examined patients in inpatient settings. Sayre (18) interviewed 35 inpatients diagnosed as having schizophrenia and defined one of the six causal belief categories in her study as “ordination”—the belief that one actually has special powers or duties and the perception that one’s hospitalization is a misunderstanding.
Kinderman et al. (35) interviewed both outpatients and inpatients and found that inpatients experiencing active psychosis did not identify their experience as an “illness” or as distinct from their usual experiences. Instead, they experienced it as an inseparable part of their identity. In contrast, the most common causal belief cited by outpatients, who were in a period of remission, implied an interaction between personal characteristics (cast in terms of vulnerability) and psychosocial stresses. Finally, Carter et al. (13) explored the development of causal beliefs among people with psychosis. Nine of the 15 participants in their study described the development of their causal beliefs as shifting from a belief that their experiences were real perceptions and thus they did not need causal beliefs to explain them, to a gradual search for reasons for their experiences, often after their first contact with mental health care.

Differences in Beliefs Between People With Psychosis and MHPs

Considerable gaps were found in four studies comparing the causal beliefs of people with psychosis and MHPs (24, 26, 27, 45) (Table 3). In one study, although specific Ns or percentages were not provided, the authors reported that biogenetic factors were considered by MHPs (i.e., pharmacists, psychologists, psychiatric nurses, and psychiatric social workers) to be the most predominant cause of psychosis, whereas societal factors were the causes most frequently endorsed by people with psychosis, with some variations depending on the type of causal belief classification used in the analysis (27). This trend was found in two more studies (24, 26). Uniquely, in a qualitative study conducted in Mali, Napo et al. (45) found an influence of cultural context on causal beliefs of both MHPs and people with psychosis: psychosocial factors, such as breaking taboos and engaging in family conflicts, were seen by both groups as playing an important role in causing schizophrenia. Although the study did not specify the experts’ professions (except for one traditional healer mentioned), this finding suggests that gaps between people who experience psychosis and MHPs may be moderated by cultural background.
TABLE 3. Studies comparing causal beliefs about psychosis among people with psychosis and among mental health professionals (MHPs)
StudyCountryParticipantsNSettingOutcomes
Napo et al., 2012 (45)MaliPeople with psychosis and medical practitioners, including a traditional healer20aHospitalPsychosocial factors, such as breaking of taboos and family conflicts, were seen by people with psychosis and by experts as playing an important role in causing schizophrenia. In this qualitative study, endorsement rates were not reported.
Luderer and Böcker, 1993 (26)GermanyPeople with psychosis and psychiatrists51bPsychiatric hospitalPsychiatrists saw psychoses as biological disorders (endorsement rates were not reported). Among people with psychosis, only 24% stressed the idea of an underlying biological disorder, 41% attributed schizophrenia to their living conditions, and 35% attributed it to their personal characteristics.
Tarakita et al., 2018 (27)JapanPeople with psychosis and MHPs (medical staff other than psychiatrists)559cHospitals and mental clinicThe authors created 4 subscales (psychosocial, biological, environmental, and cultural) based on 2 measures. Medical staff predominantly endorsed biological conceptions, and people with psychosis endorsed psychosocial beliefs. Only factor loading was reported for these subscales.
Van Dorn et al., 2005 (24)United StatesPeople with psychosis and MHPs, including psychiatrists, psychologists, clinical social workers and case managers189dMHPs were from community mental health clinics and hospitals (mailed survey); for people with psychosis, the setting was not mentioned.“Chemical imbalance” was the most frequently endorsed case by both people with psychosis (89%, N=93) and MHPs (98%, N=82). People with psychosis were significantly less likely than MHPs to endorse a chemical imbalance as a cause. Psychosocial causes were more frequently endorsed by people with psychosis (85%, N=88,) than MHPs (67%, N=55), and biogenetic causes were more frequently endorsed by MHPs (96%, N=79) than by people with psychosis (74%, N=77).
a
15 people with psychosis, 5 practitioners.
b
People with psychosis, N of psychiatrists not specified.
c
212 people with psychosis, 347 MHPs.
d
104 people with psychosis, 85 MHPs.

Causal Beliefs and Stigma

Only three studies explored the relationship between causal beliefs and stigma among people with psychosis. One study found higher endorsement rates of spiritual-religious causal beliefs (witchcraft and spiritual attack) among people with psychosis who had scores indicating high self-stigma (70%, N=14), compared with those who had scores indicating low self-stigma (33%, N=4) (44). Another study reported that people who considered their psychotic experiences as exclusively attributable to social causes acknowledged lower levels of recognizability (i.e., the feeling of being identifiable as mentally ill by other people) (p<0.02) (50). Finally, Charles et al. (49) found that higher stigma among people with psychosis was associated with biogenetic causal beliefs about psychosis (p=0.09), as well as with a belief in karma (p=0.02) and evil spirits (p=0.08).
Four studies explored causal beliefs and stigma among MHPs (24, 52, 53, 63), but three of them analyzed the two variables separately without attempting to study associations between them (24, 53, 63). Grausgruber et al. (52) did not find a significant relationship between causal beliefs and stigma in their sample of MHPs.

Discussion

The purpose of this review was to describe the scope of literature and to map the existing evidence on causal beliefs about psychosis among MHPs and people with psychosis. Results suggest that both MHPs and people with psychosis often hold complex causal models composed of different types of causal beliefs and that a gap exists in the different sets of causal beliefs held by MHPs and by people with psychosis. A central finding was that MHPs endorsed biogenetic causal beliefs of psychosis more often than they did other types of beliefs (2427, 53, 57, 59, 60, 63) and that profession type had an impact on the causal beliefs of MHPs. Those from medical professions were found to hold mostly biogenetic beliefs, whereas those from nonmedical professions, such as psychologists and social workers, were found to hold psychosocial beliefs more often than they held other types of causal beliefs. Unlike MHPs, people with psychosis were found to frequently endorse psychosocial beliefs about psychosis, a finding reported previously in other reviews (3, 68).
Although psychosocial causal beliefs often refer to social and psychological aspects as intertwined, the results of this review shed light on the inherent difference between them. Social-environmental causal beliefs may be associated with positive outcomes, such as higher self-esteem (51), whereas beliefs related to personal characteristics were found to be frequently associated with negative consequences, such as self-blame (2224), poorer emotional state (56), and higher levels of anxiety (51). On the other hand, beliefs related to personal characteristics appear to have a potential to facilitate positive change and engagement in therapy (18, 66).
The findings of qualitative studies in this review suggest that people who experience acute psychosis may be more likely to believe that the psychotic experience is part of the human experience, rather than regarding it as psychosis or as mental illness (13, 18, 35). Instead, these individuals experienced psychosis as inseparable from their identity (69, 70). For some, the process of separating one’s “self” from the illness is an important part of the recovery process (70). This finding echoes the concept of “narrative insight,” which refers to the different ways in which people make sense of their psychotic experiences, which often does not accord with traditional psychiatric definitions (69, 71, 72).
Another factor found to influence causal beliefs is culture. Studies focusing on culture and type of causal beliefs reported that people with psychosis who come from Western societies were more inclined toward biogenetic and psychosocial causal beliefs, compared with those from non-Western cultures, who were more likely to endorse spiritual-religious beliefs. Only one study explored causal beliefs of MHPs from different cultures and reported that the strongest impact on causal beliefs was culture (53). Although not focused on culture per se, a recent related qualitative systematic review that focused on the role of spirituality in the formation of causal beliefs about mental illness found considerable gaps between the spiritual needs of people with mental illness and the services provided by psychiatric systems, which failed to address those needs (73).
Differences in causal beliefs between people with psychosis and MHPs may also negatively affect the therapeutic relationship, which is closely linked to treatment adherence and outcome (74, 75). Collaboration and agreement on the purpose and goals of treatment are core features of the therapeutic relationship (76, 77). Research has shown that disagreements on a wide range of issues are highly prevalent between MHPs and people with psychosis (7881) and that such disagreements are associated with poorer outcomes (82) and less satisfaction with the therapeutic relationship (83). Therefore, on the basis of four studies that compared causal beliefs of MHPs and people with psychosis (24, 26, 27, 45), we assume that the gaps in causal beliefs between these groups can interfere with the therapeutic relationship, an issue that requires more research. Increasing MHPs’ awareness of gaps in causal beliefs and fostering patient–MHP collaborative and open communication, such as Open Dialogue (84), shared decision making, and other engagement methods (8588), about causal beliefs, can improve the therapeutic relationship.
Although results of the review indicate that MHPs are more likely to endorse biogenetic causes, compared with people with psychosis, who are more likely to endorse psychosocial causes, both groups often hold complex causal models that integrate different types of beliefs. Discussing causal beliefs as part of team meetings can enable MHPs to critically evaluate their own beliefs, raise awareness among others, and help lead to the endorsement of more complex causal models that hold multiple angles and perspectives. In turn, such models can help MHPs be more open-minded about the causal beliefs of the people they treat, which can strengthen the therapeutic alliance. When a person seeks to understand the reasons for his or her experience, whether the person defines it as psychosis or not, the MHP should encourage an open discussion about causal beliefs, exploring with the person as part of the therapeutic process different types of beliefs. Moreover, causal beliefs can serve as an important target for intervention (66, 89, 90) and help people with psychosis create links between their life history and their current experiences, which can lead to the development of a more coherent sense of self (91, 92).
Only one study found that people with psychosis were more likely to endorse biogenetic causal beliefs over psychosocial beliefs (24), and this study used case vignettes. This finding may suggest that when asked about other people with psychosis and not about their own experiences, people with psychosis are more likely to hold biogenetic beliefs. A systematic review focusing on people with mental illness and MHPs found that biogenetic beliefs were related to higher levels of stigma among both groups (19). Another recent systematic review of stigma among MHPs highlighted the relationship between biogenetic beliefs and stigma; biogenetic causal beliefs led to more negative attitudes of MHPs toward people with schizophrenia (20). Moreover, biogenetic explanations rather than psychosocial explanations have been found to evoke significantly less empathy and more dehumanization among MHPs toward people with mental illness (93, 94). Because the results of the review presented here point to potential gaps between MHPs, who were found to be more likely to endorse biogenetic than other types of causal beliefs, and people with psychosis, who were found to be more likely to endorse psychosocial causal beliefs than they were other types of beliefs, it is likely that such discrepancies will be related to higher levels of stigma among MHPs, which may be deleterious to the therapeutic relationship and to patients’ adherence. However, this assumption requires further research, because only one study explored the relationship between MHPs’ causal beliefs and stigma (52), and only three reported on the relationship between causal beliefs and stigma among people with psychosis (44, 49, 50).
Although this review has provided valuable information about the causal beliefs of MHPs and people with psychosis and about existing gaps between these two groups, its results are subject to certain limitations. The studies included in the review used a wide range of methodologies and measures to assess causal beliefs and were conducted in different geographical areas during different periods, making it hard to compare the findings regarding causal beliefs of MHPs and people with psychosis. Future studies should attempt to assess causal beliefs by using existing standardized measurements, such as the Illness Perception Questionnaire for Schizophrenia (9) and Short Explanatory Model Interview (95). Second, in some of the included studies, specific endorsement rates were not reported. To reflect an accurate and extensive presentation of the state of arts and not to ignore studies that did not provide clear numbers, we provided estimated Ns in seven cases. These estimated Ns were calculated on the basis of the total N presented in the article and the reported percentage. In cases in which percentages were not reported, we present the existing data. Third, because of the iterative nature of the data-coding process of content that was conducted by four authors, we did not assess interrater reliability. However, disagreements were discussed throughout the process until agreement was reached.

Conclusions

The included studies were heterogeneous in their quality, breadth of reported data, year of publication, design, setting, and measures, which highlights a need for further research using validated measures of causal beliefs. Results provide evidence supporting differences in causal beliefs between MHPs and people with psychosis. MHPs were more likely to endorse biogenetic beliefs about psychosis over other types of beliefs; people with psychosis were more likely to endorse psychosocial beliefs and other nonbiogenetic beliefs about their experience, rather than biogenetic beliefs. These possible differences can harm the therapeutic alliance, which is closely linked to treatment adherence and outcomes. Overcoming gaps in causal beliefs requires that MHPs become aware of their own causal beliefs and of potential gaps so that they can discuss them openly and respectfully with people with psychosis. Such discussions may help people with psychosis integrate their current experiences with their life history, which can facilitate the process of constructing a personally meaningful narrative of self and illness, an important part of the recovery process (96). Given the scarce evidence regarding differences in causal beliefs between MHPs and people with psychosis and the relationship between causal beliefs and stigma, future research should focus on exploring these issues and their impact on the therapeutic alliance.

Footnote

Ms. Rosenthal Oren, who received scholarship support from the Lazlo N. Tauber Foundation, submitted this work as part of the University of Haifa’s doctoral program requirements.

References

1.
Leventhal H, Nerenz D, Steel DJ: Illness Representations and Coping With Health Threats. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1984
2.
Williams K, Steer H: Illness perceptions: are beliefs about mental health problems associated with self-perceptions of engagement in people with psychosis? Behav Cogn Psychother 2011; 39:151–163
3.
Carter L, Read J, Pyle M, et al: The impact of causal explanations on outcome in people experiencing psychosis: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychother 2017; 24:332–347
4.
Roe D, Davidson L: Noncompliance, nonadherence, and dropout: outmoded terms for modern recovery-oriented mental health. Psychiatr Serv 2017; 68:1076–1078
5.
Zisman-Ilani Y, Shern D, Deegan P, et al: Continue, adjust, or stop antipsychotic medication: developing and user testing an encounter decision aid for people with first-episode and long-term psychosis. BMC Psychiatry 2018; 18:142
6.
Doyle R, Turner N, Fanning F, et al: First-episode psychosis and disengagement from treatment: a systematic review. Psychiatr Serv 2014; 65:603–611
7.
García S, Martínez-Cengotitabengoa M, López-Zurbano S, et al: Adherence to antipsychotic medication in bipolar disorder and schizophrenic patients: a systematic review. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2016; 36:355–371
8.
Kishi T, Ikuta T, Matsui Y, et al: Effect of discontinuation v maintenance of antipsychotic medication on relapse rates in patients with remitted/stable first-episode psychosis: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2019; 49:772–779
9.
Lobban F, Barrowclough C, Jones S: Assessing cognitive representations of mental health problems: I. the Illness Perception Questionnaire for Schizophrenia. Br J Clin Psychol 2005; 44:147–162
10.
Dietrich S, Beck M, Bujantugs B, et al: The relationship between public causal beliefs and social distance toward mentally ill people. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2004; 38:348–354
11.
Garrett M, Silva R: Auditory hallucinations, source monitoring, and the belief that “voices” are real. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:445–457
12.
Phalen P, Warman D, Martin JM, et al: Public understanding of different kinds of voice-hearing experiences: causal beliefs, perceptions of mental illness, and stigma. Psychiatr Rehabil J 2019; 42:331–340
13.
Carter L, Read J, Pyle M, et al: “I believe I know better even than the psychiatrists what caused it”: exploring the development of causal beliefs in people experiencing psychosis. Community Ment Health J 2018; 54:805–813
14.
Rüsch N, Todd AR, Bodenhausen GV, et al: Biogenetic models of psychopathology, implicit guilt, and mental illness stigma. Psychiatry Res 2010; 179:328–332
15.
Longdon E, Read J: “People with problems, not patients with illnesses”: using psychosocial frameworks to reduce the stigma of psychosis. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 2017; 54:24–28
16.
Patel V: Explanatory models of mental illness in sub-Saharan Africa. Soc Sci Med 1995; 40:1291–1298
17.
Maraj A, Anderson KK, Flora N, et al: Symptom profiles and explanatory models of first-episode psychosis in African-, Caribbean- and European-origin groups in Ontario. Early Interv Psychiatry 2017; 11:165–170
18.
Sayre J: The patient’s diagnosis: explanatory models of mental illness. Qual Health Res 2000; 10:71–83
19.
Larkings JS, Brown PM: Do biogenetic causal beliefs reduce mental illness stigma in people with mental illness and in mental health professionals? A systematic review. Int J Ment Health Nurs 2018; 27:928–941
20.
Valery KM, Prouteau A: Schizophrenia stigma in mental health professionals and associated factors: a systematic review. Psychiatry Res 2020; 290:113068
21.
Kvaale EP, Gottdiener WH, Haslam N: Biogenetic explanations and stigma: a meta-analytic review of associations among laypeople. Soc Sci Med 2013; 96:95–103
22.
Hussain S, Imran N, Hotiana UA, et al: Illness perceptions in patients of schizophrenia: a preliminary investigation from Lahore, Pakistan. Pak J Med Sci 2017; 33:829–834
23.
Holzinger A, Kilian R, Lindenbach I, et al: Patients’ and their relatives’ causal explanations of schizophrenia. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003; 38:155–162
24.
Van Dorn RA, Swanson JW, Elbogen EB, et al: A comparison of stigmatizing attitudes toward persons with schizophrenia in four stakeholder groups: perceived likelihood of violence and desire for social distance. Psychiatry 2005; 68:152–163
25.
Magliano L, De Rosa C, Fiorillo A, et al: Beliefs of psychiatric nurses about schizophrenia: a comparison with patients’ relatives and psychiatrists. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2004; 50:319–330
26.
Luderer H-J, Böcker FM: Clinicians’ information habits, patients’ knowledge of diagnoses and etiological concepts in four different clinical samples. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1993; 88:266–272
27.
Tarakita N, Yoshida K, Sugawara N, et al: Differences in etiological beliefs about schizophrenia among patients, family, and medical staff. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2018; 15:137–142
28.
Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, et al: Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med 2009; 151:264–269, W64
29.
Caqueo-Urízar A, Breslau J, Gilman SE: Beliefs about the causes of schizophrenia among Aymara and non-Aymara patients and their primary caregivers in the Central-Southern Andes. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2015; 61:82–91
30.
Johnson S, Sathyaseelan M, Charles H, et al: Insight, psychopathology, explanatory models and outcome of schizophrenia in India: a prospective 5-year cohort study. BMC Psychiatry 2012; 12:159
31.
Caqueo-Urízar A, Boyer L, Baumstarck K, et al: The relationships between patients’ and caregivers’ beliefs about the causes of schizophrenia and clinical outcomes in Latin American countries. Psychiatry Res 2015; 229:440–446
32.
Magliano L, Fiorillo A, De Rosa C, et al: Beliefs about schizophrenia in Italy: a comparative nationwide survey of the general public, mental health professionals, and patients’ relatives. Can J Psychiatry 2004; 49:322–330
33.
Johnson S, Sathyaseelan M, Charles H, et al: Predictors of insight in first-episode schizophrenia: a 5-year cohort study from India. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2014; 60:566–574
34.
Syrén S, Hultsjö S: A striving towards “normality”: illness-related beliefs among individuals living with a psychotic disorder. Issues Ment Health Nurs 2014; 35:842–850
35.
Kinderman P, Setzu E, Lobban F, et al: Illness beliefs in schizophrenia. Soc Sci Med 2006; 63:1900–1911
36.
CASP Checklist. Oxford, United Kingdom, Critical Appraisal Skills Programme, 2018. https://casp-uk.net/wp-content/uploads/2018/01/CASP-Qualitative-Checklist-2018.pdf
37.
Moola S, Munn Z, Tufanaru C, et al: Checklist for Analytical Cross Sectional Studies. Adelaide, Australia, Joanna Briggs Institute, 2017. https://jbi.global/sites/default/files/2019-05/JBI_Critical_Appraisal-Checklist_for_Analytical_Cross_Sectional_Studies2017_0.pdf
38.
Ma L-L, Wang Y-Y, Yang Z-H, et al: Methodological quality (risk of bias) assessment tools for primary and secondary medical studies: what are they and which is better? Mil Med Res 2020; 7:7
39.
Munn Z, Moola S, Lisy K, et al: Methodological guidance for systematic reviews of observational epidemiological studies reporting prevalence and cumulative incidence data. Int J Evid-Based Healthc 2015; 13:147–153
40.
Jones S, Guy A, Ormrod JA: A Q-methodological study of hearing voices: a preliminary exploration of voice hearers’ understanding of their experiences. Psychol Psychother 2003; 76:189–209
41.
Lund C, Swartz L: Xhosa-speaking schizophrenic patients’ experience of their condition: psychosis and amafufunyana. S Afr J Psychol 1998; 28:62–70
42.
Dudley R, Siitarinen J, James I, et al: What do people with psychosis think caused their psychosis? A Q methodology study. Behav Cogn Psychother 2009; 37:11–24
43.
Conrad R, Schilling G, Najjar D, et al: Cross-cultural comparison of explanatory models of illness in schizophrenic patients in Jordan and Germany. Psychol Rep 2007; 101:531–546
44.
Makanjuola V, Esan Y, Oladeji B, et al: Explanatory model of psychosis: impact on perception of self-stigma by patients in three sub-Saharan African cities. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2016; 51:1645–1654
45.
Napo F, Heinz A, Auckenthaler A: Explanatory models and concepts of West African Malian patients with psychotic symptoms. Eur Psychiatry 2012; 27(suppl 2):S44–S49
46.
Chilale HK, Silungwe ND, Gondwe S, et al: Clients and carers perception of mental illness and factors that influence help-seeking: where they go first and why. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2017; 63:418–425
47.
McCabe R, Priebe S: Explanatory models of illness in schizophrenia: comparison of four ethnic groups. Br J Psychiatry 2004; 185:25–30
48.
Angermeyer MC, Klusmann D: The causes of functional psychoses as seen by patients and their relatives: I. the patients’ point of view. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci 1988; 238:47–54
49.
Charles H, Manoranjitham SD, Jacob KS: Stigma and explanatory models among people with schizophrenia and their relatives in Vellore, South India. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2007; 53:325–332
50.
Magliano L, Fiorillo A, Del Vecchio H, et al: What people with schizophrenia think about the causes of their disorder. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc 2009; 18:48–53
51.
Watson PWB, Garety PA, Weinman J, et al: Emotional dysfunction in schizophrenia spectrum psychosis: the role of illness perceptions. Psychol Med 2006; 36:761–770
52.
Grausgruber A, Meise U, Katschnig H, et al: Patterns of social distance towards people suffering from schizophrenia in Austria: a comparison between the general public, relatives and mental health staff. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2007; 115:310–319
53.
Wahass S, Kent G: A cross-cultural study of the attitudes of mental health professionals towards auditory hallucinations. Int J Soc Psychiatry 1997; 43:184–192
54.
Carter L, Read J, Pyle M, et al: Causal beliefs in people experiencing psychosis: the relationship to treatment accessed and the perceived helpfulness of treatment. Psychol Psychother 2018; 91:332–344
55.
Carter L, Read J, Pyle M, et al: Mental health clinicians’ beliefs about the causes of psychosis: differences between professions and relationship to treatment preferences. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2017; 63:426–432
56.
Gómez-de-Regil L: Causal attribution and illness perception: a cross-sectional study in Mexican patients with psychosis. ScientificWorldJournal 2014; 2014:969867
57.
Harland R, Antonova E, Owen GS, et al: A study of psychiatrists’ concepts of mental illness. Psychol Med 2009; 39:967–976
58.
Newmark CS, Gentry L, Ziff DR: Psychological and psychiatric opinions on schizophrenia. Prof Psychol Res Pr 1977; 8:56–63
59.
Soskis DA: A survey of psychiatric opinion on schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 1972; 13:573–580
60.
Ting HT: A survey of psychiatric nurses in Taiwan: beliefs about etiology and nature of nurse interactions with relatives of people with mental illness. Psychiatr Rehabil J 1997; 21:174–176
61.
Fitzgibbons DJ, Shearn CR: Concepts of schizophrenia among mental health professionals: a factor-analytic study. J Consult Clin Psychol 1972; 38:288–295
62.
Gallagher BJ III: The attitudes of psychiatrists toward etiological theories of schizophrenia. J Clin Psychol 1977; 33:99–104
63.
Kukulu K, Ergün G: Stigmatization by nurses against schizophrenia in Turkey: a questionnaire survey. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs 2007; 14:302–309
64.
Wall ELE, Mirfin D, White S, et al: Patient perceptions of schizophrenia in forensic and general adult psychiatry. J Forensic Psychiatry Psychol 2017; 28:357–367
65.
Yalvaç HD, Mutlu EA, Kotan Z, et al: Explanatory models of illness, help seeking behaviours and related factors in patients with schizophrenia: a comparative study from two different provinces of Turkey. Community Ment Health J 2017; 53:951–957
66.
Freeman D, Dunn G, Garety P, et al: Patients’ beliefs about the causes, persistence and control of psychotic experiences predict take-up of effective cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis. Psychol Med 2013; 43:269–277
67.
Sanders D, Kydd R, Morunga E, et al: Differences in patients’ perceptions of schizophrenia between Māori and New Zealand Europeans. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2011; 45:483–488
68.
Baines T, Wittkowski A: A systematic review of the literature exploring illness perceptions in mental health utilising the self-regulation model. J Clin Psychol Med Settings 2013; 20:263–274
69.
Roe D, Ben-Yishai A: Exploring the relationship between the person and the disorder among individuals hospitalized for psychosis. Psychiatry 1999; 62:370–380
70.
Roe D, Chopra M, Rudnick A: Persons with psychosis as active agents interacting with their disorder. Psychiatr Rehabil J 2004; 28:122–128
71.
Roe D, Hasson-Ohayon I, Kravetz S, et al: Call it a monster for lack of anything else: narrative insight in psychosis. J Nerv Ment Dis 2008; 196:859–865
72.
Roe D, Kravetz S: Different ways of being aware of a psychiatric disability: a multifunctional narrative approach to insight into mental disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 2003; 191:417–424
73.
Milner K, Crawford P, Edgley A, et al: The experiences of spirituality among adults with mental health difficulties: a qualitative systematic review. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2019; 29:e34
74.
Totura CMW, Fields SA, Karver MS: The role of the therapeutic relationship in psychopharmacological treatment outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Psychiatr Serv 2018; 69:41–47
75.
Shattock L, Berry K, Degnan A, et al: Therapeutic alliance in psychological therapy for people with schizophrenia and related psychoses: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychother 2018; 25:e60–e85
76.
Horvath AO, Luborsky L: The role of the therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 1993; 61:561–573
77.
Bordin ES: The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy 1979; 16:252–260
78.
Lasalvia A, Boggian I, Bonetto C, et al: Multiple perspectives on mental health outcome: needs for care and service satisfaction assessed by staff, patients and family members. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2012; 47:1035–1045
79.
Ofir-Eyal S, Hasson-Ohayon I, Bar-Kalifa E, et al: Agreement between clients with schizophrenia and mental health workers on clients’ social quality of life: the role of social cognition and symptoms. Psychol Psychother 2017; 90:125–137
80.
Hasson-Ohayon I, Roe D, Kravetz S, et al: The relationship between consumer insight and provider-consumer agreement regarding consumer’s quality of life. Community Ment Health J 2011; 47:607–612
81.
Roe D, Weishut DJN, Jaglom M, et al: Patients’ and staff members’ attitudes about the rights of hospitalized psychiatric patients. Psychiatr Serv 2002; 53:87–91
82.
Shadmi E, Gelkopf M, Garber-Epstein P, et al: Relationship between psychiatric-service consumers’ and providers’ goal concordance and consumers’ personal goal attainment. Psychiatr Serv 2017; 68:1312–1314
83.
Cavelti M, Tschümperlin RM, Hubl D, et al: Therapeutic relationship and concordance of client- and clinician-rated motivational goals in treatment of people with psychosis: an exploratory study. Psychosis 2018; 10:90–98
84.
Seikkula BA, Jukka AJ: Open Dialogue in psychosis: I. an introduction and case illustration. J Constr Psych 2001; 14:247–265
85.
Salyers MP, Zisman-Ilani Y: Shared decision-making and self-directed care; in The Palgrave Handbook of American Mental Health Policy. Edited by Goldman HH, Frank RG, Morrissey JP. London, Palgrave Macmillan, 2020
86.
Zisman-Ilani Y, Barnett E, Harik J, et al: Expanding the concept of shared decision making for mental health: systematic search and scoping review of interventions. Ment Health Rev 2017; 22:191–213
87.
Storm M, Husebø AML, Thomas EC, et al: Coordinating mental health services for people with serious mental illness: a scoping review of transitions from psychiatric hospital to community. Adm Policy Ment Health Ment Health Serv Res 2019; 46:352–367
88.
Zisman-Ilani Y, Roe D, Elwyn G, et al: Shared decision making for psychiatric rehabilitation services before discharge from psychiatric hospitals. Health Commun 2019; 34:631–637
89.
Petrie KJ, Cameron LD, Ellis CJ, et al: Changing illness perceptions after myocardial infarction: an early intervention randomized controlled trial. Psychosom Med 2002; 64:580–586
90.
McAndrew LM, Musumeci-Szabó TJ, Mora PA, et al: Using the common sense model to design interventions for the prevention and management of chronic illness threats: from description to process. Br J Health Psychol 2008; 13:195–204
91.
Hasson-Ohayon I, Kravetz S, Lysaker PH: The special challenges of psychotherapy with persons with psychosis: intersubjective metacognitive model of agreement and shared meaning. Clin Psychol Psychother 2017; 24:428–440
92.
Roe D, Davidson L: Self and narrative in schizophrenia: time to author a new story. Med Humanit 2005; 31:89–94
93.
Lebowitz MS, Ahn WK: Effects of biological explanations for mental disorders on clinicians’ empathy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2014; 111:17786–17790
94.
Pavon G, Vaes J: Bio-genetic vs psycho-environmental conceptions of schizophrenia and their role in perceiving patients in human terms. Psychosis 2017; 9:245–253
95.
Lloyd KR, Jacob KS, Patel V, et al: The development of the Short Explanatory Model Interview (SEMI) and its use among primary-care attenders with common mental disorders. Psychol Med 1998; 28:1231–1237
96.
Leonhardt BL, Huling K, Hamm JA, et al: Recovery and serious mental illness: a review of current clinical and research paradigms and future directions. Expert Rev Neurother 2017; 17:1117–1130

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services
Psychiatric Services
Pages: 1178 - 1192
PubMed: 34126775

History

Received: 18 June 2020
Revision received: 26 September 2020
Revision received: 24 November 2020
Revision received: 23 December 2020
Accepted: 8 January 2021
Published online: 15 June 2021
Published in print: October 01, 2021

Keywords

  1. Psychosis
  2. Cognitive therapy

Authors

Details

Rotem Rosenthal Oren, M.A.
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel (Rosenthal Oren, Roe); Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel (Hasson-Ohayon); Ginsburg Health Sciences Library, Temple University, Philadelphia (Roth); Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, College of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia (Thomas, Zisman-Ilani).
David Roe, Ph.D.
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel (Rosenthal Oren, Roe); Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel (Hasson-Ohayon); Ginsburg Health Sciences Library, Temple University, Philadelphia (Roth); Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, College of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia (Thomas, Zisman-Ilani).
Ilanit Hasson-Ohayon, Ph.D.
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel (Rosenthal Oren, Roe); Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel (Hasson-Ohayon); Ginsburg Health Sciences Library, Temple University, Philadelphia (Roth); Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, College of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia (Thomas, Zisman-Ilani).
Stephanie Roth, M.L.I.S.
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel (Rosenthal Oren, Roe); Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel (Hasson-Ohayon); Ginsburg Health Sciences Library, Temple University, Philadelphia (Roth); Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, College of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia (Thomas, Zisman-Ilani).
Elizabeth C. Thomas, Ph.D.
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel (Rosenthal Oren, Roe); Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel (Hasson-Ohayon); Ginsburg Health Sciences Library, Temple University, Philadelphia (Roth); Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, College of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia (Thomas, Zisman-Ilani).
Yaara Zisman-Ilani, M.A., Ph.D. [email protected]
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel (Rosenthal Oren, Roe); Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel (Hasson-Ohayon); Ginsburg Health Sciences Library, Temple University, Philadelphia (Roth); Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, College of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia (Thomas, Zisman-Ilani).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Zisman-Ilani ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share