Site maintenance Wednesday, November 13th, 2024. Please note that access to some content and account information will be unavailable on this date.
Skip to main content
Full access
Other Articles
Published Online: 1 November 2001

Can Recovery From Depression Be Achieved?

Abstract

Even though a variety of treatments are available for depression, many patients experience an incomplete response, resulting in chronic functional impairment. The authors present a therapeutic heuristic that derives, in part, from a body of research that suggests that symptoms of mood disorders can be separated into three distinct components: somatic anxiety, which is most prominent in anxiety disorders, such as panic; anhedonia or low positive affect, which is most specific to depression; and general distress, which is present with both anxiety and depressive disorders. General distress and somatic anxiety appear to be significantly modulated by serotonin, and serotonergic drugs may exert their effects most significantly on these symptoms. On the other hand, positive affect, the dimension of reward-oriented motivation and enjoyment, appears to be most dependent on dopamine and, indirectly, norepinephrine. Thus a theoretical heuristic can be derived on the basis of the predominance of residual symptoms. Serotonergic agents would be chosen for monotherapy or augmentation for symptoms of distress. Alternatively, catecholaminergic drugs would be the first choice for anhedonia and decreased motivation.
Depressive disorders are common and often chronic (1,2). Depression is often considered a time-limited condition, but 50 to 85 percent of depressed persons will experience multiple episodes (3,4), and many are chronically ill (5,6). This reality creates a tremendous social burden in terms of both economics and human suffering (7). Throughout the world, mental disorders are among the most disabling conditions. The World Health Organization suggests that mental disorders account for five of the top ten causes of disability in industrialized countries; among these, unipolar major depression ranks first (8,9,10).
Although interventions such as antidepressant medications or psychotherapy significantly reduce the severity of depression for most patients, many will experience residual symptoms and impairment even after extended periods of treatment (11). Unfortunately, unlike diseases such as diabetes and hypertension, for which the end points of treatment are well defined, patients and clinicians may have difficulty recognizing when the desired goals of treatment for depression have been achieved. The purpose of this article is to review the nature of mood disorders as they relate to symptomatic and functional recovery and to suggest a framework for conceptualizing and assessing the outcome of treatment.
Nearly a decade ago, a consensus group was convened to define terms such as "treatment response"—typically defined as a 50 percent reduction in scores on the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Ham-D)—and "remission"—usually a Ham-D score of no more than 7 (12). Possible scores on the Ham-D range from 0 to 52, with higher scores indicating more severe depression. Remission is defined as a minimum level of symptoms on the Ham-D that is considered to be within the range of normal functioning. Recent research has indicated that many patients undergoing treatment do not reach this level of recovery and thus do not achieve a complete restoration of normal social and occupational functioning, such as returning to work (7,13). Therefore, accurate assessment of residual symptoms is critical to the management of depression.
In the past, the treatment of depression was largely under the purview of psychiatric practice. However, the introduction of newer, ostensibly safer, and better-tolerated antidepressants has increasingly shifted the treatment of acute depression into the primary care environment over the past decade. Because some people are treated successfully in primary care, a treatment "sieve" is created, in which a higher proportion of treatment-resistant patients present for psychiatric care. Among these patients, full recovery is even more difficult to achieve.
The problem, then, is that many patients do not recover rapidly or completely, and the clinician is faced with a treatment decision for which the empirical literature offers little guidance. Although research data support the benefits of particular treatments, few studies have compared the effectiveness of various treatments. In this paper we provide clinicians with a method for evaluating residual symptoms of depression that is oriented toward better matching of symptoms with treatment.

Dimensions of mood

Editor's Note: This paper is part of a series on the treatment of depression edited by Charles L. Bowden, M.D. Contributions are invited that address major depression, bipolar depression, dysthymia, and dysphoric mania. Papers should focus on integrating new information for the purpose of improving some aspect of diagnosis or treatment. For more information, please contact Dr. Bowden at the Department of Psychiatry, Mail Code 7792, University of Texas Health Science Center, 7703 Floyd Curl Drive, San Antonio, Texas 78229-3900; 210-567-6941; [email protected].
In this section we define the concept of mood and how dimensions of mood relate to anxiety and depressive disorders. On the basis of empirical findings and conceptualizations by Watson and colleagues (14,15) and other researchers (16), we suggest that although these categories have distinctive features, they also have broad areas of symptomatic overlap. We present a dimensional rather than a categorical approach to understanding mood and anxiety disorders. An understanding of these dimensions of mood and their underlying physiological bases may be helpful for predicting responses to pharmacological intervention.
Highly complex systems of emotional response have been proposed previously (17). However, when emotions are observed in the context of behavioral responses, a simpler, two-factor model emerges. Although described by many terms, these two emotional motivational systems can be described as appetitive, which involves approaches to rewarding stimuli, and defensive, which involves the behavioral management of threat (17). When either appetitive or defensive behavioral responses are engaged, more basic arousal responses can also be activated.
As proposed by Lang and associates (17), anxiety disorders can be seen as resulting from defensive motivational structures in the brain. For example, the emotional responses to exposure to phobic stimuli engage acute fear responses with attendant physiological arousal, such as tachycardia and tachypnea, that are similar to those seen in animals who are presented with a threatening stimulus from the environment.
Other disorders, such as generalized anxiety disorder, are less well connected to the emotional and behavioral responses to immediate threat. However, avoidance or behavioral inhibition can also be engaged as part of a conditioned fear response in which anxiety symptoms may be experienced in anticipation of exposure to threat (18). As Lang and colleagues (18) noted, "fear is generally held to be a reaction to an explicit threatening stimulus.… Anxiety is usually considered a more general state of distress, more long-lasting, prompted by less explicit or more generalized cues, involving physiological arousal but often without organized functional behavior."
More purely depressive symptoms involve an inhibition of emotional and physiological responses to rewarding stimuli. Thus a key component of depression is a relative reduction in approach behaviors toward otherwise rewarding environmental cues, such as food or a sexual partner. In fact, certain animal behavioral models of depression involve just such behaviors (17,18). Thus three emotional responses have emerged: acute physiological arousal characterized by panic or intense phobic fear, chronic anxiety reactions to anticipated threat, and emotional response to reward.
But is there a parallel in human psychopathology? Over the past 15 years or so, a body of research literature about the nature of mood regulation and mood disorders has emerged (19,20). Interest in this area has been partly driven by the observed overlap between anxiety and depression. There is substantial symptomatic and diagnostic comorbidity of anxiety and depressive disorders. In addition, they have a common response to treatments, including psychotherapy—for example, cognitive-behavioral therapy—and medication, such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) (14,21). Recent research has shown that common, fundamental substrates of human emotion are engaged in both anxiety and depressive disorders.
This view is supported by phenomenological, genetic, and neurobiological data. Clearly substantial overlap exists in the diagnostic criteria for depressive and anxiety disorders (16). However, there also is considerable aggregation of anxiety and depressive disorders, both at the individual level—that is, co-occurrence of anxiety and depressive disorders across a person's life span (16)—and within families (22,23,24,25,26).
In addition, the results of several studies indicate that generalized anxiety disorder and major depression have a common genetic diathesis and that differences between the two conditions may be determined by environmental rather than genetic factors (22,23,24). For example, stressful life events have been shown to increase the likelihood of both anxiety and depressive symptoms, but multiple stressors over a short period appear to increase susceptibility to depression per se (27). Finally, there is evidence of significant sharing of neurobiological factors, particularly hyperadrenergic and hypercortisolemic features (28). Together, these data support the notion of a genetic and physiological continuum between anxiety and depression.
Research on human emotion has consistently demonstrated a tripartite model of mood and anxiety disorders, as outlined in Table 1. Clark and Watson and their colleagues (14,15,19,20) developed the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ) to assess the higher-order dimensions of generalized distress; anxious arousal, or somatic anxiety; and anhedonia and positive affect (14,15). Items were initially selected on the basis of previous conceptual and empirical work on the dimensionality of emotion and the structure of psychopathology (19,29).
Final item selection and initial validation studies were conducted among both healthy samples and groups of patients who were experiencing anxiety, depressive disorders, and other disorders. Selected items were then subjected to factor analyses that yielded these three dimensions, supporting the tripartite model (14). Items within a dimension tend to be highly correlated, whereas the correlation between dimensions is relatively low.
Examination of the items in the MASQ helps provide a more concrete sense of the symptoms and mood states that are indicators of the dimensions of general distress, anhedonia or low positive affect, and somatic anxiety. Table 1 lists selected items from this instrument that load on these three higher-order dimensions of emotion. Three MASQ subscales relate to the higher-order dimension of generalized distress: anxiety, depression, and mixed. These scales assess affective states or symptoms that are typically associated with anxiety, depression, or a mixed anxious-depressed profile, respectively.
However, these subscales are highly correlated with one another—for example, r=.78 for the anxiety and depression subscales in a sample of 470 patients (14)—and items from all three subscales load on the broader dimension of generalized distress. Thus all three subscales tend to reflect features that are shared by anxious and depressed patients more than elements that distinguish depressive and anxiety disorders from each other. Indeed, it is likely that many manifestations of psychopathology are associated with elevated scores for indicators of generalized distress.
In contrast, scales that tap the dimensions of anhedonia and somatic anxiety have better discriminant validity. Relative to the generalized distress dimension, the anhedonia dimension tends to be much more specific to depression. That is, the correlation between anhedonia and measures of depression tend to be notably higher than correlations between anhedonia and measures of anxiety (14). The anhedonia dimension reflects the mood dimension of motivation, energy, and pleasure that reflects an active engagement with—as opposed to withdrawal from—rewarding stimuli from the external environment. We can readily see that the inhibition of appetitive emotions and behaviors is more specific to depressive disorders.
In contrast, anxious arousal, which primarily assesses somatic components of anxiety, as shown in Table 1, shows notably higher correlations with measures of anxiety, particularly panic, than with measures of depression (14). The anxious arousal subscale maps most closely onto the dimension of acute fear response to specific environment cues that we have described. Consistent with these observations are the correlations between specific MASQ scales observed by Watson and colleagues (14). Across five samples of patients and healthy subjects, the average correlation between the anxiety and depression subscales of the generalized distress dimension was .69, whereas the average correlation between subscales of the anxious arousal and anhedonia dimensions was only .34. Thus the subscales of the latter two dimensions have superior discriminant validity. This observation also supports the contention that the distress dimension is common to depressive and anxiety disorders.
Several empirical studies have tested this tripartite model of mood among patients with depressive and anxiety disorders. Although some unanticipated complexities have emerged, in general the results have been supportive of the model.
For example, Brown and colleagues (16) performed a factor analysis of the self-report responses of a large group of patients who had depressive and anxiety disorders. They found that the higher-order dimension of generalized distress was associated with all anxiety and depressive disorders. Somatic anxiety, on the other hand, was relatively specific to panic disorder. The positive emotional factor loaded inversely on both depression and social phobia, suggesting that anhedonia or low positive affect is linked to symptoms associated with depression but also with the social withdrawal associated with social phobia. The results of several additional studies also supported the tripartite formulation of Watson and Clark and colleagues (20).
In several respects the tripartite model has heuristic value for researchers and clinicians alike. First, this model helps account for the symptoms that are common to anxiety and depressive disorders. High levels of distress can be seen to reflect a common set of features of anxiety and depression, whereas anhedonia and somatic anxiety reflect unique aspects of depression and panic disorders, respectively. The notion that symptoms of distress represent shared or overlapping features of anxiety and depression is consistent with the view that these symptoms are dimensional rather than categorical elements of psychopathology (14,30).
Second, the tripartite model has important implications for clinicians and researchers who are interested in improving treatment outcomes. If, in fact, the generalized distress and anhedonia dimensions of depression are separable, changes in one dimension need not be a strong predictor of changes in the other. Thus a depressed person could experience a decline in negative affect without a corresponding increase in positive affect. These points underscore the importance of separately and comprehensively assessing these dimensions of depression and of applying interventions aimed at specific dimensions.
One reason that measures such as the MASQ may prove to be important in the assessment of treatment outcomes is that typical rating measures for depressive and anxiety disorders tend to be highly correlated with each other (15). Persons with either anxiety or depressive disorders tend to have elevated ratings on symptomatic measures of both anxiety and depression, such as the Ham-D and the Hamilton Rating Scale for Anxiety. This may be because many anxiety and depression rating scales are heavily weighted toward symptoms of general distress and cannot discriminate between dimensions of mood. Therefore, evidence of symptomatic improvement that is based on these instruments may primarily reflect a reduction in the symptoms of general distress that are common to both anxiety and depressive disorders.

Mood dimensions and response to antidepressants

The next question is whether an understanding of these mood dimensions is particularly helpful in making decisions about treatment. Here we review information on the relationship between the neurochemistry that is relevant to the actions of antidepressant drugs and dimensions of mood. In both animal and human models of psychopathology, symptoms of general distress and somatic anxiety appear to depend to a significant extent on the serotonin (5-HT) system (31,32). Drugs such as the 5-HT1B/2A/2C receptor agonist methachlorophenylpiperazine (33) and the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tertralin (34) have been shown to be anxiogenic in studies of both humans and animals, suggesting an important role for these receptors in the genesis of anxiety.
Furthermore, SSRIs and serotonin receptor modulators—for example, mirtazapine and nefazodone—reduce symptoms of anxiety and panic disorders (35) in humans and inhibit behaviors that are linked to anxiety in animals (32). Together, these findings support the notion of serotonin modulation of the distress and somatic anxiety domains. Consistent with speculations by Petty and colleagues (30), such modulation may reflect the fact that serotonin promotes the broader maintenance of emotional and behavioral homeostasis in the face of stress.
Evidence suggests that antidepressants that act mainly through serotonin modulate primarily the general distress dimension (36). For example, Knutson and associates (37) administered either paroxetine or placebo to healthy volunteers in a double-blind manner over the course of four weeks. SSRIs were associated with a decrease in distress but no significant changes in positive affect. Clinical studies of the specific symptoms affected by SSRIs also are consistent. Bodkin and colleagues (38) found that SSRIs significantly reduced symptoms of panic and anxiety in 18 of 20 patients with depression. However, these patients did not report increased energy. Indeed, ten of 21 patients reported decreased energy during treatment with SSRIs.
This observation contrasts with the effects of tricyclic antidepressants, which, as a rule, act via combined serotonergic and noradrenergic mechanisms and tend to produce relatively rapid effects on a broad set of symptoms, including symptoms associated with distress and appetitive motivation (39). When considered from the perspective of the tripartite model, these results suggest that SSRIs reduce levels of general distress and anxious arousal but have limited effects on the anhedonia dimension, perhaps even worsening these symptoms somewhat. Such a conclusion underscores the importance of assessing the effects of medications on specific components of depression.
As a cautionary note, we should add that although there appear to be strong links between serotonergic functioning and the inhibition of generalized distress and negative affect, the specificity of these links is unclear. As several commentators have noted (40,41), an alternative view is that serotonin has general inhibitory functions that modulate both positive and negative affect. Consistent with this view, Zald and Depue (41) recently found that among healthy volunteers, the maximum prolactin response to d,l-fenfluramine (a serotonin agonist), a measure of the responsiveness of the serotonin system, was inversely correlated with ratings of both daily negative (r=-.42) and positive (r=-.47) emotions. These findings are consistent with a view that the role of serotonin could be understood in a broader context of emotional regulation. Such regulation has been referred to as emotional constraint—that is, the inhibition of both positive and negative affect as well as other cognitive and affective processes (40). Such findings introduce some ambiguity about the precise effects of SSRIs on symptoms of depression.
However, the two alternative views of the actions of serotonin or of drugs such as SSRIs—that they may have a relatively selective inhibitory effect on negative affect as opposed to inhibition of both positive and negative emotion—suggest that an SSRI would not be the medication of choice if a primary goal was the reversal of anhedonia and an increase in positive affect. In fact, the inhibition of both positive and negative emotions by serotonin could explain the "flatness" of mood that some patients experience while taking SSRIs.
A variety of basic studies have implicated noradrenergic mechanisms in the genesis of anxiety (42,43,44,45,46). Noradrenergic agents have been shown to reduce symptoms of panic as well (47,48), possibly through their effects on locus coeruleus activity (45). However, a study by Pohl and associates (49) showed that although the noradrenergic antidepressant desipramine has antipanic properties, it was associated with selective increases in the anxiety-related side effect of jitteriness among patients with panic disorder. Also, the antianxiety effects of imipramine in generalized anxiety disorder were found to be inversely proportional to the concentrations of the desipramine metabolite (50). Thus the noradrenergic agents may produce a specific benefit among patients with panic disorder while somewhat worsening the symptoms of anxiety overall in acute treatment.
Alternatively, several sources suggest that the dimension of positive affect is much more dependent on catecholaminergic activity, particularly dopaminergic activity (51). Depue and colleagues (52) showed that positive emotion was associated with dopaminergic activity, specifically a decrease in prolactin and an increase in spontaneous blink rates in response to the dopamine receptor agonist bromocriptine. There is evidence that both changes reflect functional dopaminergic activity, perhaps as a result of linkages between the trait of positive emotionality and postsynaptic dopamine receptor sensitivity. This finding, in turn, is broadly consistent with a large body of literature that links dopamine and brain reward systems.
However, as Salamone and colleagues have suggested (11), dopaminergic activity may not mediate reward per se but, rather, the capacity to engage in effortful behaviors to attain reward. For example, dopamine D2 receptor antagonist drugs such as pimozide and haloperidol do not reduce reward when the "cost" of acquisition is low—that is, when there are no obstacles to be surmounted. However, reduction of dopaminergic activity—for example, in the nucleus accumbens—reduces the willingness of rats to overcome restraints in obtaining a food reinforcement. Thus dopamine appears to be associated more closely with effortful behaviors that reflect heightened appetitive motivation than with the pleasure or consummatory response to rewards per se (11).
On the basis of the evidence we have presented, it might be expected that medications or other biological manipulations that affect dopaminergic systems would result in a more pronounced effect on anhedonia. Collectively, these data suggest that serotonin may be more important for the modulation of fear responses, such as general distress, whereas dopamine may be more closely associated with anhedonia or, more specifically, low motivation.

Mechanisms of action of antidepressants

The mechanisms of action of currently available antidepressants include the enhancement of transmission of monoamines, such as serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine, and the antagonism of serotonin receptors (53). The former class includes reuptake blockers, such as tricyclics and SSRIs; monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs); and the alpha2 antagonist mirtazapine, which induces the release of both norepinephrine and serotonin. The latter class includes 5-HT2 antagonists, such as nefazodone and trazodone, as well as mirtazapine, which also blocks 5-HT3 receptors. Therefore, the proximal mechanism of action of all currently available antidepressants involves the regulation of synaptic transmission via the monoamines.
Many drugs are relatively selective for one of the monoaminergic systems—for example, serotonin for the SSRIs and norepinephrine for reboxetine and desipramine. However, before we assume that these agents are truly selective, it is important to keep in mind that there is considerable co-localization and "cross-talk" of monoamines in the central nervous system (54). That is, monoamines are mutually regulating. Norepinephrine itself as well as norepinephrine selective reuptake inhibitors (NSRIs) enhance the release of both dopamine and serotonin in the forebrain (55).
The effect of NSRIs on serotonin and dopamine may be explained by the observation that norepinephrine, acting through alpha1 receptors, can induce release of these transmitters (54). However, in addition, the norepinephrine transporter protein—the reuptake site—also has high affinity for the reuptake of dopamine (56). Thus NSRIs are, in effect, dopamine reuptake inhibitors as well. This concept is supported by Karson's finding (57) that spontaneous blink rates—as noted, a putative measure of forebrain dopamine activity—were elevated among depressed patients who were taking tricyclic antidepressants. Thus enhancement of norepinephrine transmission—by reuptake blockade or by a reduction in the metabolism of norepinephrine by inhibition of monoamine oxidase—would be expected to induce an enhancement of arousal via the norepinephrine mechanisms (58) but also to enhance reward acquisition—that is, motivation—through actions on dopaminergic systems.
The interplay of serotonin with norepinephrine and dopamine is more complicated because of the complexity of the serotonin receptor system relative to the catecholamines. For example, serotonin has been shown to both enhance and inhibit dopaminergic activity in the frontal cortex and nucleus accumbens, depending on the type of serotonin receptor subtype that is activated (21). The inhibition of dopamine by serotonin—primarily via 5-HT2C receptors (59,60,61)—may explain why highly selective SSRIs have been shown to reduce dopamine release in the frontal cortex or nucleus accumbens (62,63) and reward acquisition in animals (64,65,66).
This effect may parallel the mood-flattening effect of these agents among some patients and is consistent with the concept of a link between serotonin and constraint of dopaminergic systems as posited by Depue and colleagues (40). Clinically, it is important to keep in mind that not all patients who are treated with SSRIs will experience a flattening effect. However, it has been our observation that when a therapeutic failure occurs with one of these agents, it is often the result of a lack of improvement in the anhedonia experienced by many depressed patients.

A therapeutic heuristic

Clearly depression is a serious illness that is associated with a level of functional impairment as great as or greater than that associated with other medical conditions (7,67). Further, as we have noted, a simple amelioration in symptoms may not lead to full functional recovery (7). We have argued that it is possible to separate depressive symptoms into two relatively independent constructs: general distress and anhedonia. In fact, we suggest that separating depressive symptoms into those most closely akin to anxiety—fear, negative rumination, and anxious mood—and those indicative of low pleasure and motivation can provide a framework for evaluating the adequacy of response and determining the next step in treatment.
As we have noted, depression rating scales tend to be heavily weighted toward the distress dimension of mood (15) at the expense of the positive affective dimension. Rating instruments such as the Social Adjustment Scale (SAS) (67) and the Social Adaptation Self-Evaluation Scale (SASE) (68) may be useful to some clinicians in evaluating residual impairment in important areas of role functioning, such as work, housework, and parenting, which could in turn be considered to be related to the positive emotion-high motivation dimension.
As an alternative to these relatively complicated rating instruments, Stahl (69) recently provided a simple clinical method for assessing functional outcome: asking "What are three signal events in your life that you do when you are well but not when you are depressed or anxious?" The assessment of these events over time may provide a simple but effective assessment of functional outcome linked to motivation.
A careful assessment of both symptomatic and functional outcomes often leads to the realization that the patient will experience an incomplete outcome with any given antidepressant treatment. The question then becomes "What next?" Here, the "functional assay"—that is, the question "Has your functioning returned to normal?"—takes us to the stage of deciding on the next strategy in therapy.
Response rates—that is, the likelihood that a patient will experience significant positive change from baseline—do not differ much between antidepressants. However, remission—or complete recovery—does appear to vary between drugs. In particular, tricyclic antidepressants appear to produce remission rates that are higher than those achieved with SSRIs among patients who are more severely depressed, especially those with melancholia (70,71,72). Furthermore, it has been suggested that venlafaxine may be associated with a small but significantly greater remission rate than that associated with the SSRIs (73).
In a recent pooled analysis of all clinical trials that compared venlafaxine with SSRIs—fluoxetine, paroxetine, and fluvoxamine—Thase and colleagues (73) found a remission rate of 45 percent for patients treated with venlafaxine, compared with 35 percent for patients who received SSRIs and 25 percent for patients who received placebo. This difference between drugs has been attributed to the combined effects on serotonin and norepinephrine (69), although this point remains controversial (74). Given the data we have reviewed here, combining serotonergic and noradrenergic mechanisms might be expected to broaden the effects of the antidepressants by targeting both symptoms linked to the enhancement of pleasure and pleasurable engagement with the environment and symptoms linked to the experience of distress.
In support of this view is the finding that combining a noradrenergic antidepressant, such as desipramine (75) or bupropion (76,77), with an SSRI enhances the antidepressive effect among persons who do not respond to either SSRIs or NSRIs. This certainly suggests that some depressed patients who do not respond to selective agents taken alone will benefit from a combination of serotonin and norepinephrine. This suggests the possibility of a broader effect on both distress and positive motivational symptoms.
Do some patients fare better with SSRIs? One possible answer is that patients who have depression with atypical features achieve better outcomes. This depressive subtype is characterized by reversed vegetative features, including hypersomnia, hyperphagia, dense fatigue, and weight gain. However, other prominent features of atypical depression include anxiety and positive mood reactivity—that is, a tendency for mood to rebound briefly after a positive event (78,79,80). Atypical depression has been shown to respond more positively to MAOIs than to tricyclic antidepressants (78). More recent data suggest that SSRIs also are superior to tricyclic antidepressants (81) and equivalent to MAOIs (82). Alternatively, a recent large-scale trial found both imipramine and fluoxetine to be superior to placebo and equivalent to each other (83). As we have noted, desipramine, the more purely noradrenergic metabolite of imipramine, actually counteracted the antianxiety effects of imipramine among patients with anxiety (50). This observation suggests that the beneficial effects of imipramine on anxiety symptoms are mediated serotonergically (50).
In support of the serotonin distress mechanism for atypical depression, McGrath and colleagues (84) showed that gepirone, a 5-HT1A partial agonist agent similar to the antianxiety drug buspirone, was much more effective than placebo in a group of patients with atypical depression; the response rates were 62 percent and 20 percent, respectively. The collective data suggest that serotonergic antidepressants, including SSRIs, MAOIs, and perhaps imipramine and gepirone, have benefit in atypical depression.
Overall these data suggest that melancholia, typified by persistent anhedonia and positive vegetative features, may respond best to noradrenergic—or mixed noradrenergic and serotonergic—agents. Alternatively, atypical depression, with prominent anxiety and reactive mood, appears to respond well to serotonergic drugs. These findings will help lead us to a framework for managing depression.
In dealing with treatment-resistant depression, the key question is "What is the next step?" There are many possible choices, including use of alternative monotherapies and augmentation. These options were recently reviewed extensively (85,86,87,88). When depression has not yet resolved, what symptoms remain? Although many patients will have residual distress and anhedonia, often one feature or the other will predominate.
Liebowitz (89) has suggested that depression with prominent anxiety or atypical features responds well to serotonergic agents such as MAOIs and SSRIs. As we have noted, serotonergic treatments may be somewhat more important for symptoms of distress and may, under certain circumstances, actually worsen symptoms in the domains of pleasure and motivation. Alternatively, consistent with the effects of tricyclic antidepressants in melancholia, the catecholamines (norepinephrine and dopamine) may be more significant.
Although we cannot draw an absolute dichotomy here, these observations may provide a framework for making decisions about treatment. The heuristic, then, could be that if distress is the predominant residual symptom, pharmacological approaches that affect the serotonin system might be the best initial step. Alternatively, if the lack of appetitive motivation is the main problem, treatments that focus on the catecholamines might be chosen, as illustrated in Table 2. Even though dopamine may be more significant for appetitive motivation, the interplay between norepinephrine and dopaminergic systems suggests that noradrenergic agents will indirectly produce effects on functional dopamine activity.
As noted, many antidepressants act via serotonin-dependent mechanisms, and choosing an antidepressant with serotonin reuptake blocking properties or serotonin receptor antagonist properties can be expected to reduce symptoms of distress (53). However, augmentation strategies that emphasize actions in the serotonin system also may be useful when distress symptoms predominate in the clinical picture. Therefore, a switch to a more potent serotonergic agent or the use of augmenting agents such as lithium (90,91,92) and buspirone (93) that have known serotonergic effects may be the preferred initial choices for treating a highly distressed patient.
Many patients experience residual problems with energy and appetitive motivation. For these patients, the model suggests using a catecholaminergic antidepressant as monotherapy. However, the use of augmenting drugs that act through catecholaminergic mechanisms, such as desipramine and bupropion (94,95), or psychostimulant drugs, such as methylphenidate and methamphetamine (96), in combination with a serotonergic agent also could prove useful. More recently, the atypical antipsychotics have been shown to be an effective augmenting strategy when used with SSRIs to treat refractory unipolar depression among patients who are not psychotic (97).
For example, Ostroff and Nelson (98) reported that the addition of risperidone to an SSRI was an effective augmentation strategy for eight treatment-resistant patients. Shelton and colleagues (97) conducted a clinical trial of augmentation of fluoxetine with olanzapine among patients with treatment-resistant depression. The participants in that study had not responded to either NSRIs or SSRIs, and they received a prospective run-in with 40 to 60 mg of fluoxetine a day. Nonresponders were randomly assigned to receive continuation fluoxetine plus placebo, 5 to 20 mg of olanzapine a day plus placebo, or the olanzapine-fluoxetine combination. The combined treatment produced a rapid response that was sustained over a 16-week follow-up period in about half of the patients, which was not seen in the other groups.
In a related study, Zhang and associates (99) showed that when fluoxetine in combination with olanzapine or risperidone was administered to rats, a marked elevation was observed in frontal norepinephrine and dopamine without a concomitant change in serotonin. This finding suggests that the combination treatment benefits a residual lack of energy and appetitive motivation.
It would be remiss of us to suggest that pharmacotherapies should be the primary approach to resistant depression. Psychotherapy, particularly cognitive-behavioral therapy and interpersonal psychotherapy, have been shown to be beneficial in the treatment of depression. Psychotherapy should certainly be considered as an adjunct in the treatment of patients who have not achieved full remission with antidepressants. More recently, an adaptation of the cognitive therapy model called the cognitive-behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy (100,101) was shown to be effective when used in combination with an antidepressant among chronically depressed patients.
Given the variety of pharmacological and psychotherapeutic options currently available, recovery seems to be an achievable goal for most patients who have depression. However, to reach this end, we must have tools that will enable us to know when the goal has not been achieved and to determine what steps to take next. We hope that the concepts in this article will contribute to a move in that direction.
It is important to keep in mind that this is a working model only and that alternative choices, such as augmentation with lithium for an anergic patient and desipramine for an anxious one, may still be useful in some circumstances. Many of the studies of mood dimensions that we have reviewed drew from populations of healthy volunteers or patients from research clinics. Thus the results may be only weakly generalizable to patients encountered in practice. Furthermore, this model does not suggest that treatment should be altered in the case of a patient who is doing well, regardless of the patient's previous symptoms. However, given our current state of knowledge, this conceptual approach may be helpful for planning initial treatment strategies. It is likely that a more complex understanding of specific phenotypes of mood disorders will emerge in the future and will help to guide selection of treatment.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by grant MH-01741 to Dr. Shelton and grant MH-497590 to Dr. Tomarken from the National Institute of Mental Health.
Table 1. Items from the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ) that exemplify the tripartite model of mood1
1 Adapted with permission (14,15)A copy of the full MASQ can be obtained from [email protected] or [email protected].
Table 2. A therapeutic heuristic for predicting treatment effects on the basis of mechanism of action

Footnote

The authors are affiliated with Vanderbilt University Medical Center in Nashville, Tennessee. Send correspondence to Dr. Shelton at 1500 21st Avenue, South, Suite 2200, Nashville, Tennessee 37212 (e-mail, [email protected]).

References

1.
Robins LN, Helzer JE, Weissman MM, et al: Lifetime prevalence of specific psychiatric disorders in three sites. Archives of General Psychiatry 41:949-958, 1984
2.
Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, et al: Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry 51:8-19, 1994
3.
Zis AP, Goodwin FK: Major affective disorder as a recurrent illness: a critical review. Archives of General Psychiatry 36:835-839, 1979
4.
Consensus Development Panel: NIMH/NIH consensus development conference statement on mood disorders: pharmacological prevention of recurrences. American Journal of Psychiatry 142:469-476, 1985
5.
Klerman GL, Weissman MM: The course, morbidity, and costs of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 49:831-834, 1992
6.
Keller MB, Lavori PW, Mueller TI, et al: Time to recovery, chronicity, and levels of psychopathology in major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 49:809-816, 1992
7.
Hirschfeld RM, Montgomery SA, Keller MB, et al: Social functioning in depression: a review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 61:268-275, 2000
8.
Murray CJL, Lopez AD (eds): Global Burden of Disease and Injury. Cambridge, Mass, Harvard University Press, 1996
9.
Lopez AD, Murray CC: The global burden of disease, 1990-2020. Nature Medicine 4:1241-1243, 1998
10.
Ustin TB: The global burden of mental disorders. American Journal of Public Health 89:1315-1318, 1999
11.
Salomone DA, Keller MB, Leon AC, et al: Recovery from major depression: a 10-year prospective follow-up across multiple episodes. Archives of General Psychiatry 54:1001-1006, 1997
12.
Frank E, Prien RF, Jarrett RB, et al: Conceptualization and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder: remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence. Archives of General Psychiatry 48:851-855, 1991
13.
Healey D, Healey H: The clinical pharmacologic profile of reboxetine: does it involve the putative neurobiological substrates of well being? Journal of Affective Disorders 51:313-322, 1998
14.
Watson D, Weber K, Assenheimer JS, et al: Testing a tripartite model: I. evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom subscales. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1:3-14, 1995
15.
Watson D, Clark LA, Weber K, et al: Testing a tripartite model: II. exploring the symptom structure of anxiety and depression in student, adult, and patient samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104:15-25, 1995
16.
Brown TA, Chorpita BF, Barlow DH: Structural relationships among dimensions of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders and dimensions of negative affect, positive affect, and autonomic arousal. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107:179-192, 1998
17.
Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN: Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry 44:1248-1263, 1998
18.
Lang PJ, Davis M, Lehman A: Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders 61:137-159, 2000
19.
Clark LA, Watson D: Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100:316-336, 1991
20.
Mineka S, Watson D, Clark LA: Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annual Review of Psychology 49:377-412, 1998
21.
Shelton RC, Brown L: Mechanisms of action in the treatment of anxiety. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 62(suppl 12):10-15, 2000
22.
Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, et al: Major depression and generalized anxiety disorder: same genes, (partly) different environments? Archives of General Psychiatry 49:1622, 1992
23.
Neale MC, Kendler KS: Models of comorbidity for multifactorial disorders. American Journal of Human Genetics 57:935-953, 1995
24.
Kendler KS: Major depression and generalised anxiety disorder: same genes, (partly) different environments—revisited. British Journal of Psychiatry 30(suppl):68-75, 1996
25.
Kendler KS, Davis CG, Kessler RC: The familial aggregation of common psychiatric and substance use disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey: a family history study. British Journal of Psychiatry 170:541-548, 1997
26.
Hewitt JK, Silberg JL, Rutter M, et al: Genetics and developmental psychopathology: I. phenotypic assessment in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 38:943-963, 1997
27.
Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA: Stressful life events and major depression: risk period, long-term contextual threat, and diagnostic specificity. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 186:661-669, 1998
28.
Boyer P: Do anxiety and depression have a common pathophysiological mechanism? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplement 406:24-29, 2000
29.
Watson D, Tellegen A: Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychology Bulletin 98:219-235, 1985
30.
Petty F, Davis LL, Kabel D, et al: Serotonin dysfunction disorders: a behavioral neurochemistry perspective. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 57(suppl 8):11-16, 1996
31.
Berendsen HH: Interactions between 5-hydroxytryptamine receptor subtypes: is a disturbed receptor balance contributing to the symptomatology of depression in humans? Pharmacology and Therapeutics 66:17-37, 1995
32.
Petty F, Kramer GL, Wu J: Serotonergic modulation of learned helplessness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 821:538-541, 1997
33.
Silverstone PH, Rue JE, Franklin M, et al: The effects of administration of mCPP on psychological, cognitive, cardiovascular, hormonal, and MHPG measurements in human volunteers. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 9:173-178, 1994
34.
Cervo L, Mocaer E, Bertaglia A, et al: Roles of 5-HT1A receptors in the dorsal raphe and dorsal hippocampus in anxiety assessed by the behavioral effects of 8-OH-DPAT and S 15535 in a modified Geller-Seifter conflict model. Neuropharmacology 39:1037-1043, 2000
35.
Gorman JM, Kent JM: SSRIs and SMRIs: broad spectrum of efficacy beyond major depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 60(suppl 4):33-38, 1999
36.
Van Praag HM, Kahn R, Asnis GM, et al: Therapeutic indications for serotonin-potentiating compounds: a hypothesis. Biological Psychiatry 22:205-212, 1987
37.
Knutson B, Wolkowitz OM, Cole SW, et al: Selective alteration of personality and social behavior by serotonergic innervation. American Journal of Psychiatry 155:137-145, 1997
38.
Bodkin JA, Lasser RA, Wines JD Jr, et al: Combining serotonin reuptake inhibitors and bupropion in partial responders to antidepressant monotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 58:137-145, 1997
39.
Katz MM, Koslow SH, Maas JW, et al: The timing, specificity, and clinical prediction of tricyclic drug effects in depression. Psychological Medicine 17:297-309, 1987
40.
Depue RA, Spoont MR: Conceptualizing a serotonin trait: a behavioral dimension of constraint. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 487:47-62, 1986
41.
Zald DH, Depue RA: Serotonergic functioning correlates with positive and negative affect in psychiatrically healthy males. Personality and Individual Differences 30:71-86, 2001
42.
Charney DS, Redmond DE Jr: Neurobiological mechanisms in human anxiety: evidence supporting central noradrenergic hyperactivity. Neuropharmacology 22:1531-1536, 1983
43.
Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, et al: Noradrenergic mechanisms in stress and anxiety: I. preclinical studies. Synapse 23:28-38, 1996
44.
Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, et al: Noradrenergic mechanisms in stress and anxiety: II. clinical studies. Synapse 23:39-51, 1996
45.
Sullivan GM, Coplan JD, Kent JM, et al: The noradrenergic system in pathological anxiety: a focus on panic with relevance to generalized anxiety and phobias. Biological Psychiatry 46:1205-1218, 1999
46.
Tanaka M, Yoshida M, Emoto H, et al: Noradrenaline systems in the hypothalamus, amygdala, and locus coeruleus are involved in the provocation of anxiety: basic studies. European Journal of Pharmacology 405:397-406, 2000
47.
Rosenbaum JF: Treatment of outpatients with desipramine. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 45:17-22, 1984
48.
Kalus O, Asnis GM, Rubinson E, et al: Desipramine treatment in panic disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 21:239-244, 1991
49.
Pohl R, Yeragani VK, Balon R, et al: The jitteriness syndrome in panic disorder patients treated with antidepressants. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2:72-73, 1989
50.
McLeod DR, Hoehn-Saric R, Porges SW, et al: Therapeutic effects of imipramine are counteracted by its metabolite, desipramine, in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 20:615-621, 2000
51.
Depue RA, Collins P: Neurobiology and the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:491-569, 1999
52.
Depue RA, Luciana M, Arbisi P, et al: Dopamine and the structure of personality: relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67:485-498, 1994
53.
Stahl SM: Selecting an antidepressant by using mechanism of action to enhance efficacy and avoid side effects. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59(suppl 18):23-29, 1998
54.
Reith ME, Li MY, Yan QS: Extracellular dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin in the ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats during intracerebral dialysis following systemic administration of cocaine and other uptake blockers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 134:309-317, 1997
55.
Hajnal A, Mark GP, Rada PV, et al: Norepinephrine microinjections in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus increase extracellular dopamine and decrease acetylcholine in the nucleus accumbens: relevance to feeding reinforcement. Journal of Neurochemistry 68:667-674, 1997
56.
Pacholcyzk T, Blakely RD, Amara SG: Expression cloning of a cocaine- and antidepressant-sensitive human noradrenaline transporter. Nature 350:350-354, 1991
57.
Karson CN: Oculomotor signs in a psychiatric population: a preliminary report. American Journal of Psychiatry 136:1057-1060, 1979
58.
Aston-Jones G, Rajkowski J, Cohen J: Role of locus coeruleus in attention and behavioral flexibility. Biological Psychiatry 46:1309-1320, 1999
59.
De Deurwaerdere P, Spampinato U: Role of serotonin2A and serotonin2B/2C receptor subtypes in the control of accumbal and striatal dopamine release elicited in vivo by dorsal raphe nucleus electrical stimulation. Journal of Neurochemistry 73:1033-1042, 1999
60.
Lucas G, Spampinato U: Role of striatal serotonin2A and serotonin2C receptor subtypes in the control of in vivo dopamine outflow in the rat striatum. Journal of Neurochemistry 74:693-701, 2000
61.
Lucas G, De Deurwaerdere P, Caccia S, et al: The effect of serotonergic agents on haloperidol-induced striatal dopamine release in vivo: opposite role of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor subtypes and significance of the haloperidol dose used. Neuropharmacology 39:1053-1063, 2000
62.
Prisco S, Esposito E: Differential effects of acute and chronic fluoxetine administration on the spontaneous activity of dopaminergic neurones in the ventral tegmental area. British Journal of Pharmacology 116:1923-1931, 1995
63.
Ichikawa J, Kuroki T, Meltzer HY: Differential effects of chronic imipramine and fluoxetine on basal and amphetamine-induced extracellular dopamine levels in rat nucleus accumbens. European Journal of Pharmacology 350:159-164, 1998
64.
Katz RJ, Carroll BJ: Intracranial reward after Lilly 110140 (fluoxetine HCl): evidence for an inhibitory role for serotonin. Psychopharmacology 1:189-193, 1977
65.
Cazala P: Effects of Lilly 110140 (fluoxetine) on self-stimulation behavior in the dorsal and ventral regions of the lateral hypothalamus in the mouse. Psychopharmacology 71:143-146, 1980
66.
Lee K, Kornetsky C: Acute and chronic fluoxetine treatment decreases the sensitivity of rats to rewarding brain stimulation. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 60:539-544, 1998
67.
Weissman MM: Beyond symptoms: social functioning and the new antidepressants. Journal of Psychopharmacology 11(suppl 4):S5-S8, 1997
68.
Bosc M, Dubini A, Polin V: Development and validation of a social functioning scale, the Social Adaptation Self-Evaluation Scale. European Neuropsychopharmacology 7:S57-S70, 1997
69.
Stahl SM: The 7 habits of highly effective psychopharmacologists: II. begin with the end in mind. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 61:327-328, 2000
70.
Roose SP, Glassman AH, Attia E, et al: Comparative efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and tricyclics in the treatment of melancholia. American Journal of Psychiatry 151:1735-1739, 1994
71.
Perry PJ: Pharmacotherapy for major depression with melancholic features: relative efficacy of tricyclic versus selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants. Journal of Affective Disorders 39:1-6, 1996
72.
Stahl SM: Are two antidepressant mechanisms better than one? Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 58:339-340, 1997
73.
Thase ME, Entsuah AR, Rudolph RL: Remission rates during treatment with venlafaxine or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. British Journal of Psychiatry 178:234-241, 2001
74.
Shader RI, Fogelman SM, Greenblatt DJ: Newer antidepressants: hypotheses and evidence. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 17:1-3, 1997
75.
Nelson JC, Mazure CM, Bowers MB Jr, et al: A preliminary, open study of the combination of fluoxetine and desipramine for rapid treatment of major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 48:303-307, 1991
76.
Sprenger D: Buspirone augmentation of a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor: efficacy in two depressed patients with comorbid anxiety and type I alcohol dependence. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 17:425-426, 1997
77.
Marshall RD, Liebowitz MR: Paroxetine/bupropion combination treatment for refractory depression. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 16:80-81, 1996
78.
Quitkin FM, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, et al: Columbia atypical depression: a subgroup of depressives with better response to MAOI than to tricyclic antidepressants or placebo. British Journal of Psychiatry Supplement 21:30-34, 1993
79.
Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Rabkin JG, et al: Atypical depression: a valid clinical entity? Psychiatric Clinics of North America 16:479-495, 1993
80.
Cohen LJ: Rational drug use in the treatment of depression. Pharmacotherapy 17:45-61, 1997
81.
Strata P, Bolino F, Cupillari M, et al: A double-blind parallel study comparing fluoxetine with imipramine in the treatment of atypical depression. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 6:193-196, 1991
82.
Pande AC, Birkett M, Fechner-Bates S, et al: Fluoxetine versus phenelzine in atypical depression. Biological Psychiatry 40:1017-1020, 1996
83.
McGrath PJ, Stewart JW, Janal MN, et al: A placebo-controlled study of fluoxetine versus imipramine in the acute treatment of atypical depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 157:344-350, 2000
84.
McGrath PJ, Stewart JW, Quitkin FM, et al: Gepirone treatment of atypical depression: preliminary evidence of serotonergic involvement. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 14:347-352, 1994
85.
Janicak PG, Martin B: Strategies for treatment-resistant depression. Clinical Cornerstone 1:58-71, 1999
86.
Shelton RC: Treatment options for refractory depression. Journal of Clinical Psychi-atry 60(suppl 4):57-63, 1999
87.
Nelson JC: Augmentation strategies in depression 2000. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 61(suppl 2):13-19, 2000
88.
Fava M: New approaches to the treatment of refractory depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 61(suppl 1):26-32, 2000
89.
Liebowitz MR: Depression with anxiety and atypical depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 54(suppl):10-14, 1993
90.
Carli M, Afkhami-Dastjerdian S, Reader TA: Effects of a chronic lithium treatment on cortical serotonin uptake sites and 5-HT1A receptors. Neurochemical Research 22:427-435, 1997
91.
Massot O, Rousselle JC, Fillion MP, et al:5-HT1B receptors: a novel target for lithium: possible involvement in mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 21:530-541, 1999
92.
Haddjeri N, Szabo ST, de Montigny C, et al: Increased tonic activation of rat forebrain 5-HT1A receptors by lithium addition to antidepressant treatments. Neuropsychopharmacology 22:346-356, 2000
93.
Bleier P, Bergeron R, de Montigny C: Selective activation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors induces rapid antidepressant response. Neuropsychopharmacology 16:333-338, 1997
94.
Nelson JC: Augmentation strategies with serotonergic-noradrenergic combinations. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59(suppl 5):65-68, 1998
95.
Fatemi SH, Emamian ES, Kist DA: Venlafaxine and bupropion combination therapy in a case of treatment-resistant depression. Annals of Pharmacotherapy 33:701-703, 1999
96.
Nierenberg AA, Dougherty D, Rosenbaum JF: Dopaminergic agents and stimulants as antidepressant augmentation strategies. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59(suppl 5):60-63, 1998
97.
Shelton R, Tollefson GD, Tohen M, et al: A novel augmentation strategy for treatment-resistant major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 158:131-134, 2001
98.
Ostroff RB, Nelson JC: Risperidone augmentation of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in major depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 60:256-259, 1999
99.
Zhang W, Perry KW, Wong DT, et al: Synergistic effects of olanzapine and other antipsychotic agents in combination with fluoxetine on norepinephrine and dopamine release in rat prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 23:250-262, 2000
100.
McCullough JP: Cognitive-behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy: an interactional treatment approach for dysthymic disorder. Psychiatry 47:234-250, 1984
101.
Keller MB, McCullough JP, Klein DN, et al: A comparison of nefazodone, the cognitive behavioral-analysis system of psychotherapy, and their combination for the treatment of chronic depression. New England Journal of Medicine 342:1462-1470, 2000

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services
Psychiatric Services
Pages: 1469 - 1478
PubMed: 11684742

History

Published online: 1 November 2001
Published in print: November 2001

Authors

Details

Richard C. Shelton, M.D.
Andrew J. Tomarken, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share