Skip to main content
Full access
Other Articles
Published Online: 1 July 2001

Familiarity With and Social Distance From People Who Have Serious Mental Illness

Abstract

Objectives: This study examined the effects of familiarity with and social distance from persons who have serious mental illness on stigmatizing attitudes about mental illness. METHODS: A total of 208 community college students completed three written measures about familiarity, perception of dangerousness, fear, and social distance. Path analysis with manifest-variable structural modeling techniques was used to test a version of a model in which familiarity influences the perception of dangerousness, which in turn influences fear, which influences social distance from persons with serious mental illness. RESULTS: Most of the participants reported experience with mental illness. Scores on the three written measures largely supported the path model. Correlations between the perception of dangerousness and fear as well as between fear and social distance were particularly strong. CONCLUSIONS: Approaches to social change that increase the public's familiarity with serious mental illness will decrease stigma. Further studies are warranted that focus on how contact between members of the general public and persons who have serious mental illness may be facilitated.
Societal stigma significantly impedes the opportunities of persons who have serious mental illnesses such as schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. The 1999 Surgeon General's report (1) pointed to stigma as a key variable in understanding the course of illness and outcomes of people who have psychiatric disabilities. As we and others have reported elsewhere (2,3), the general public is less likely to hire persons who are labeled mentally ill, to lease apartments to them, or to freely interact with them. Moreover, persons who have mental illnesses are less likely to seek mental health services because of the accompanying stigma (4).
Two constructs are significantly related to the stigma of mental illness: familiarity and social distance. The effects of these constructs can be thought of as anchoring each end of a path describing the impact of stigma: familiarity influences stigma, which in turn influences social distance. Research suggests that members of the general public who are familiar with mental illness—those who have some contact with persons who have psychiatric disabilities—are less likely to endorse stigmatizing attitudes (5,6,7,8). Conversely, individuals who perpetuate stigma are likely to socially distance themselves from persons with mental illness; social distance may manifest itself in such discriminatory practices as not renting to or hiring people who have psychiatric disabilities (5,8,9,10). The purpose of this study was to directly test the three components of this path—familiarity, stigma, and social distance.
We have adopted a social psychological model for describing components of stigma that affect the outcomes of persons who have mental illness (2,11). According to that model, members of the general public hold stigmatizing attitudes about mental illness and react emotionally as a result of those attitudes—for example, "Persons who have mental illness are dangerous, and I fear them." This emotional reaction leads to a behavioral response—for example, "Because of my fear, I am going to avoid persons with mental illness." This attitude-emotion-behavior model is based on Weiner's (12) attribution theory, which has been tested in several studies (13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22).
As implied in these two examples, a perception of dangerousness is a crucial attitude that affects the components of the stigma model. According to this model, the assumption that an individual has personal responsibility for being violent leads to fear—for example, most people respond to violent threats with apprehension (23). Several studies have shown a specific relationship between perceiving persons who have mental illness as dangerous and fearing such persons (7,9,24,25). Fear, in turn, yields avoidant behaviors or social distance.
For example, one study showed that a fearful reaction to two attempts to assassinate politicians, attributed to persons who had schizophrenia, led to greater social distance between the public and individuals with mental illness (24). This historical finding has been supported by research showing that perceptions of dangerousness lead to social distance from persons with mental illness (9,10). Employers do not hire persons who have mental illness, because they want to keep such persons at a distance. Landlords do not permit people with psychiatric disabilities to move onto their properties.
Attitudes about mental illness in general, and dangerousness in particular, are influenced by an individual's familiarity with serious mental illness. Familiarity has been defined as knowledge of and experience with mental illness (6). It ranges from seeing a television portrayal of mental illness, to having a friend or coworker who has a mental illness, to having a family member who has a mental illness, to having a mental illness oneself. Previous research has shown that a lack of familiarity is associated with prejudicial attitudes about mental illness (6,7,8). We expected members of the general public who were more familiar with mental illness to be less likely to agree that persons who have psychiatric disabilities are dangerous, and we expected this lower perception of dangerousness in turn to be associated with less fear and less social distance.
Within the broader model of familiarity and social distance, we also examined the direct impact of familiarity on fear and social distance. In other words, does familiarity with mental illness directly diminish one's fear of persons who have mental illness as well as social distance from such persons?

Methods

Participants in this study were drawn from the student body of a local community college. We have done previous research with community college students (5,26), because they tend to be more demographically representative of the population as a whole than college sophomores from four-year universities. The participants provided demographic information and completed three written measures related to the constructs of familiarity, dangerousness, fear, and social distance.

Familiarity

Previous studies have measured familiarity categorically by asking participants, "Do you know someone with a mental illness?" (8). However, categorical measures have limited statistical power, so we used the Level of Contact Report (6), which we had developed previously. The Level of Contact Report lists 12 situations of varying degrees of intimacy that involve persons who have mental illness. These situations were adapted from other scales used in stigma research (8,27) and ranged from the least intimacy ("I have never observed a person that I was aware had a serious mental illness") to medium intimacy ("I have worked with a person who had a severe mental illness at my place of employment") to high intimacy ("I have a mental illness").
When the Level of Contact Report was developed, the 12 situations were ranked in terms of intimacy of contact. The mean of rank-order correlations summarizing interrater reliability was .83. The rank order, shown in Table 1, was validated in a subsequent sample of 100 research participants (6). The reliability and validity of the measure have been supported by two studies (5,6).
The 208 participants in this study were asked to check all of the situations on the 12-item list that they had experienced in their lifetime. The index of familiarity was the rank score of the most intimate situation the participant checked. For example, a participant who checked three situations— "A friend of the family has a serious mental illness" (rank order score, 9), "I have watched a documentary on television about mental illness" (rank order score, 4), and "My job includes providing services to persons with mental illness" (rank order score, 7)—would have a score of 9 because a friend of the family having a serious mental illness is the most intimate of the checked situations.

Perception of dangerousness and fear

A 21-item Attribution Questionnaire (28), modeled on the work of Reisenzein (16), was administered to participants. Three questionnaire items each test seven constructs that are relevant to stigma and mental illness: two attitudes—personal responsibility and dangerousness; three emotional reactions—anger, concern, and fear; and two behavioral responses—help and avoidance. An example of an item is, "How frightened of a person with mental illness would you feel?" Participants respond to the items by using a 9-point Likert scale, where 9 indicates "very much." Possible scores thus range from 21 to 189, with higher scores indicating higher levels of perception of dangerousness and fear. The reliability and factor structure for this test were supported by an earlier study (28). Factor scores corresponding with perceived danger and fear were used.

Social distance

Social distance from persons who have mental illness was assessed with the Social Distance Scale (8,27). This scale contains seven items that participants rate on a scale of 0 to 3, where 3 indicates "definitely unwilling." Possible scores thus range from 0 to 21, with higher scores representing a greater desire to distance oneself from persons who have mental illness. An example of an item is, "How would you feel about renting a room in your home to a person with serious mental illness?" The Social Distance Scale has excellent reliability and validity and is often used in stigma research as a proxy for behavioral indexes of discrimination against persons who have mental illness (6,8).

Results

A total of 216 students were informed of the study during their psychology or social science classes and were asked to participate. Of these, 208 agreed to participate and completed all the measures; the other eight students either refused to participate or returned incomplete measures. Demographic data for the participants are shown in Table 1. A total of 172 participants, or 83 percent, were female, and 160 participants, or 77 percent, were white.
Also shown in Table 1 are the mean scores and standard deviations for the measures used in the path analysis, along with the frequencies with which the participants checked each of the items on the Level of Contact Report. These frequencies reveal some interesting trends. For example, fewer than 10 percent of the participants reported that they had no experience with mental illness.
This high rate of exposure to persons with mental illness was partly a result of exposure through contemporary media sources. More than 90 percent of the participants had been exposed to images of mental illness in films, and another two-thirds reported seeing more objective presentations of mental illness in documentaries. About a quarter of the participants reported working alongside someone who had a mental illness. A third of the participants had a friend of the family with a mental illness, and a quarter had a relative with a mental illness. Fewer than 5 percent of the participants lived with a person who had mental illness, and only about 2 percent identified themselves as having a serious mental illness.

Path analysis

We used path analysis with manifest-variable structural modeling techniques to test our proposed model, because such an analysis examines both the size and the direction of associations among variables. The model was an overidentified recursive model with manifest variables. Intercorrelations among the dependent variables (familiarity, dangerousness, fear, and social distance) are summarized in Table 2. This intercorrelational matrix was used as the foundation for the remainder of the analyses, which were conducted with use of the SAS system's CALIS procedure. The analyses used the maximum-likelihood method of parameter estimation; all analyses were performed on the variance-covariance matrix (29).
In terms of goodness of fit, the chi square statistic was used to test the null hypothesis that the reproduced covariance matrix would have the specified model structure—that is, that the model would fit the data. We included two additional goodness-of-fit indexes: the normed fit index (30) and the comparative fit index (31). The normed fit index ranges from 0 to 1, where 0 represents the goodness of fit associated with the null model—a model in which all variables are uncorrelated—and 1 represents the goodness of fit associated with a saturated model—a model with 0 degrees of freedom that perfectly reproduces the original covariance matrix. The comparative fit index is a variation on the normed fit index that has been shown to be less biased than the normed fit index in small samples (31). For each index, a value above .9 suggests an acceptable fit between the model and the data.
Figure 1 shows the relationship between familiarity, perceived dangerousness, fear, and social distance. Estimations produced a significant chi square value that did not support a fit between the model and the data (χ2=17.67, df=1, p<.001). Technically, when the proper assumptions are met, the chi square statistic may be used to test the null hypothesis that the model fits the data. However, in practice the statistic is very sensitive to sample size and departures from multivariate normality and will often result in the rejection of a well-fitting model. One alternative way to examine the size of the chi square statistic for the theoretical model is to compare it with the chi square value for the null model. A chi square difference test showed that this model fit the data (p<.001) much better than the null model did (χ2=368.81, df=6). Moreover, the normed fit index and the comparative fit index were .953 and .951, respectively, supporting the fit between the model and the data.
All but one of the t values for the path coefficients in Figure 1 were significant (p<.05). As expected, familiarity inversely predicted the perception of dangerousness: the more familiar a study participant was with mental illness, the less dangerous that individual believed persons with mental illness to be. Familiarity was also inversely associated with social distance: participants who were familiar with mental illness were not likely to be socially distant from these individuals. No significant relationship was found between familiarity and fear. As predicted, a strong relationship was found between expectations of dangerousness and fear. In addition, fear was found to positively predict social distance: participants who feared persons with mental illness were more likely to avoid such persons.

Discussion and conclusions

The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between familiarity, social distance, and stigmatizing attitudes about mental illness. The results seem to support the causal path we proposed. That is, members of the general public who are relatively familiar with serious mental illness are less likely to believe that persons who have psychiatric disabilities are dangerous. Relatively weaker perceptions of dangerousness corresponded with less fear of persons with mental illness, which in turn was associated with less social distance.
These findings seem to support the role of familiarity in the stigma surrounding mental illness. Members of the general public who have greater knowledge about or experience with mental illness are less likely to stigmatize, at least in terms of stereotypes of dangerousness. Moreover, these individuals are less likely to discriminate against persons who have serious mental illness by avoiding them. The effects of familiarity were somewhat pervasive in our model: participants who reported more familiarity demonstrated less social distance.
This finding may be a measurement artifact, because in some ways familiarity and social distance are different sides of the same coin. The Social Distance Scale has been described as a proxy for discriminatory behavior (8,27,32). Individuals report only whether they think they would withhold opportunities from people who belong to a minority group—in this case, persons who have mental illness. Additional research is needed to clarify the relationship between familiarity and social distance by using actual behavioral indexes of avoidance and discrimination.
Frequency analyses of familiarity items from the Level of Contact Report provided some useful information that challenges stigma. One naive notion of serious mental illness is that these disorders are rarely encountered because few persons are seriously mentally ill, and most of these people are confined to remote wards (33). Contrary to this notion, fewer than 10 percent of the participants in our study reported having no experience with mental illness, whereas more than 90 percent reported learning about mental illness from movies. Unfortunately, much of the information from the film industry promotes stigma. Media analyses have shown that on the whole, people who have mental illness are represented in movies as being dangerous (34).
Our study also showed that members of the general public have fairly intimate contact with people who have mental illness. More than a quarter of the participants reported working with someone who had a mental illness, and about a third reported that a friend of the family identified him- or herself as being mentally ill. It is likely that these numbers are artificially low, because people who have mental illness learn that keeping their history from coworkers and friends can protect them from public disapproval (33). Hence members of the general public are likely to have far more coworkers and associates who have mental illness than they are aware of. The important message here is that mental illness is not a rare disorder that locks most of its sufferers away in hospitals: interactions with persons who have mental illness are common.
One concern about this study is whether the sample was representative of the general public. The participants were diverse in race, sex, age, and educational background, partly because they were recruited from certificate and vocational programs at a community college. In addition, the sample was sufficiently large to enable all analyses to be completed with good statistical power (29). However, the study did not include the kind of probability sampling and stratification strategies that are needed to ensure good external validity. Nationwide survey research of this nature is typically more labor intensive and costly than the approaches used in our study (35). Our results support our fundamental assumptions and therefore suggest that a broader survey of a representative sample is justified.
Our findings have implications for strategies to address stigma. Approaches to social change that increase the public's familiarity with serious mental illness will decrease stigma. Social psychologists have examined several variables that are relevant to ethnic prejudice and that could be adapted for research on contact with and stigma surrounding persons who have mental illness. One important variable that affects contact is opportunity: members of the majority must have opportunities to interact with members of minority groups if stigma is to be reduced (36). Thus persons who have serious mental illnesses must have formal opportunities to contact and interact with the general public. Other factors that augment the effects of interpersonal contact include treatment and perception of the participants as equals by members of the public (37,38), cooperative interaction (41,42), institutional support for contact (41,42), frequent contact with individuals who mildly disconfirm the stereotype of mental illness (43,44), a high level of intimacy (45,46,47), and real opportunities to interact with members of minority groups (36). Each of these factors suggests specific hypotheses on how contact between members of the general public and persons who have serious mental illness can be facilitated.
Figure 1. Relationship between familiarity, perception of dangerousness, fear, and social distance in a sample of 208 participants in a study on attitudes about mental illness1
1 The values are the standardized path coefficients. The residual errors were .990 for perception of dangerousness, .498 for fear, and .886 for social distance.
Table 1. Demographic data and frequencies of questionnaire scores for 208 participants in a study on familiarity with and social distance from persons who have mental illness
Table 2. Intercorrelations among familiarity, perceived dangerousness, fear, and social distance for a sample of 208 participants in a study of attitudes about mental illness

Footnote

Dr. Corrigan, Ms. Green, and Mr. Lundin are affiliated with the University of Chicago Center for Psychiatric Rehabilitation, 7230 Arbor Drive, Tinley Park, Illinois 60477 (e-mail, [email protected]). Ms. Kubiak is with the psychology department of Prairie State College in Chicago Heights, Illinois. Dr. Penn is with the psychology department of the University of North Carolina in Chapel Hill.

References

1.
Satcher D: Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon General. Washington, DC, Department of Health and Human Services, 1999
2.
Corrigan PW, Penn DL: Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. American Psychologist 54:765-776, 1999
3.
Link BG: Understanding labeling effects in the area of mental disorders: an assessment of the effects of expectations of rejection. American Sociological Review 52:96-112, 1987
4.
Holmes EP, River LP: Individual strategies for coping with the stigma of severe mental illness. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 5:231-239, 1998
5.
Corrigan PW, Backs A, Green A, et al: Prejudice, social distance, and familiarity with severe mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, in press
6.
Holmes EP, Corrigan PW, Williams P, et al: Changing attitudes about schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:447-456, 1999
7.
Link BG, Cullen FT: Contact with the mentally ill and perceptions of how dangerous they are. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 27:289-303, 1986
8.
Penn DL, Guynan K, Daily T, et al: Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 20:567-578, 1994
9.
Levey S, Howells K: Dangerousness, unpredictability, and the fear of people with schizophrenia. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry 6:19-39, 1995
10.
Madianos MG, Madianou D, Vlachonikolis J, et al: Attitudes towards mental illness in the Athens area: implications for community mental health intervention. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 75:158-165, 1987
11.
Corrigan PW: Mental health stigma as social attribution: implications for research methods and attitude change. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7:48-67, 2000
12.
Weiner B: Judgements of Responsibility: A Foundation for a Theory of Social Conduct. New York, Guilford, 1995
13.
Dooley PA: Perceptions of the onset controllability of AIDS and helping judgements: an attributional analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 25:858-869, 1995
14.
Graham S, Weiner B, Zucker GS: An attributional analysis of punishment goals and public reactions to OJ Simpson. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23:331-346, 1997
15.
Menec VH, Perry RP: Reactions to stigmas among Canadian students: testing attribution-affect-help judgement model. Journal of Social Psychology 138:443-453, 1998
16.
Reisenzein R: A structural equation analysis of Weiner's attribution-affect model of helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50:1123-1133, 1986
17.
Rush LL: Affective reactions to multiple social stigmas. Journal of Social Psychology 138:421-430, 1998
18.
Schmidt G, Weiner B: An attribution-affect-action theory of behavior: replications of judgements of help-giving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 14:610-621, 1988
19.
Steins G, Weiner B: The influence of perceived responsibility and personality characteristics on the emotional and behavioral reactions to people with AIDS. Journal of Social Psychology 139:487-495, 1999
20.
Weiner B, Graham S, Chandler C: Pity, anger, and guilt: an attributional analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 8:226-232, 1982
21.
Weiner B, Perry RP, Magnusson J: An attributional analysis of reactions to stigmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55:738-748, 1988
22.
Zucker GS, Weiner B: Conservatism and perceptions of poverty: an attributional analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 23:925-943, 1993
23.
Johnson-Dalzine P, Dalzine L, Martin-Stanley C: Fear of criminal violence and the African American elderly: assessment of a crime prevention strategy. Journal of Negro Education 65:462-469, 1996
24.
Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H: The effect of violent attacks by schizophrenic persons on the attitude of the public towards the mentally ill. Social Science and Medicine 43:1721-1728, 1996
25.
Wolff G, Pathare S, Craig T, et al: Community knowledge of mental illness and reaction to mentally ill people. British Journal of Psychiatry 168:191-198, 1996
26.
Corrigan PW, River LP, Lundin RK, et al: Predictors of participation in campaigns against mental illness stigma. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 187:378-380, 1999
27.
Link BG, Cullen FT, Frank J, et al: The social rejection of former mental patients: understanding why labels matter. American Journal of Sociology 92:1461-1500, 1987
28.
Corrigan PW, Rowan D, Green A, et al: Challenging two mental illness stigmas: personal responsibility and dangerousness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, in press
29.
Hatcher L: A Step-by-Step Approach to Using the SAS System for Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling. Cary, NC, SAS Institute Inc, 1994
30.
Bentler PM, Bonett DG: Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin 88:588-606, 1980
31.
Bentler PM: EQS Structural Equations Program Manual. Los Angeles, BMDP Statistical Software, 1989
32.
Penn DL, Kommana S, Mansfield M, et al: Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia: II. the impact of information on dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:437-446, 1999
33.
Corrigan PW, Lundin RK: Don't Call Me Nuts! Coping With the Stigma of Mental Illness. Tinley Park, Ill, Recovery Press, 2000
34.
Wahl OF: Media Madness: Public Images of Mental Illness. New Brunswick, NJ, Rutgers University Press, 1995
35.
Pescosolido BA, Monahan J, Link BG, et al: The public's view of the competence, dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of persons with mental health problems. American Journal of Public Health 89:1339-1345, 1999
36.
Sigelman L, Welch S: The contact hypothesis revisited: black-white interaction and positive racial attitudes. Social Forces 71:781-795, 1993
37.
Cook SW: Experimenting on social issues: the case of school desegregation. American Psychologist 40:452-460, 1985
38.
Riordan C: Equal-status interracial contact: a review and revision of the concept. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 2:161-185, 1978
39.
Johnson DW, Johnson R, Maruyama G: Goal interdependence and interpersonal attraction in heterogeneous classrooms, in Groups in Contact. Edited by Miller N, Brewer MB. New York, Academic Press, 1984
40.
Worchel S: The role of cooperation in reducing intergroup conflict, in Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Edited by Worchel S, Austin WG. Chicago, Nelson-Hall, 1986
41.
Adlerfer CP: Problems of changing white males' behavior and beliefs concerning race relations, in Change in Organizations. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass, 1982
42.
Williams RM: Mutual Accommodation: Ethnic Conflict and Cooperation. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press, 1977
43.
Johnston L, Hewstone M: Cognitive models of stereotype change:3. subtyping and the perceived typicality of disconfirming group members. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 28:360-386, 1992
44.
Weber R, Crocker J: Cognitive processes in the revision of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45:961-977, 1983
45.
Amir Y: Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psychological Bulletin 71:319-342, 1969
46.
Brown RJ, Turner JC: Interpersonal and intergroup behavior, in Intergroup Behavior. Edited by Turner JC, Giles H. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1981
47.
Ellison CG, Powers DA: The contact hypothesis and racial attitudes among black Americans. Social Science Quarterly 75:385-400, 1994

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services
Psychiatric Services
Pages: 953 - 958
PubMed: 11433114

History

Published online: 1 July 2001
Published in print: July 2001

Authors

Details

Patrick W. Corrigan, Psy.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share