Introduction
The majority of therapists have cried during therapy with a client. A survey on ethical issues in psychological practice (
Pope, Tabachnick, & Keith-Spiegel, 1987) found that 56.5% of 456 respondents had cried in the presence of a client. An informal survey of 19 psychodynamic therapists (Nelson, 2007) found that “about two thirds” (p. 1) of therapists reported having cried in therapy. More recently,
Blume-Marcovici, Stolberg, & Khademi (2013) found that 72% of 684 psychologists and psychology trainees reported that they had cried during therapy with a client. Those who cry do so in an average of 7% of therapy sessions (
Blume-Marcovici, et al., 2013). A study conducted by
Trezza, Hastrup and Kim (1988) on patient crying found that patients cry in approximately 21% of therapy sessions. Based on these statistics, therapists cry approximately one third as frequently as their clients. A more recent study of client crying (
Robinson, Hill, & Kivlighan, 2015) found client tears in approximately 14% of sessions. Compared to this figure, therapists experience tearfulness about half as often as their clients (of course, not taking into account intesity or longevity of crying). However, very little research exists regarding this most basic expression of emotion by the therapist in therapy, i.e., therapists’ crying in therapy (hereafter, TCIT). When searching for the terms “psychologist crying” or “therapist tears” in the PsycInfo database, there is only one result (
Blume-Marcovici, et al., 2013). The words “therapist crying” produce one additional match: a 1995 qualitative dissertation by Jane Waldman focused on the crying experiences of ten psychodynamic psychotherapists.
A more in-depth search turns up three case studies on the topic of TCIT (
Counselman, 1997;
Owens, 2005;
Rhue, 2001). Based on the case studies, along with Waldman’s dissertation (1995), several basic themes regarding TCIT can be discerned. It appears, for instance, that TCIT may be most likely to occur when therapy session content focuses on grief and loss. In addition, each study highlights the important role of therapist genuineness in treatment, and the ways in which their own tears acted as messengers of authenticity and compassion. Aside from these few case examples, however, very little is known about the clinical contexts in which TCIT tends to occur, or about therapists’ understanding of their own tears.
While TCIT specifically has received little research attention, patient crying in psychotherapy has been the topic of some theoretical writings (
Labott, 2001;
Nelson, 2008;
Trezza, et al., 1988;
Van Heukelem, 1979). Trezza et al., (1988) conducted the only study to date which directly asked therapists about their experiences with client crying. In their study of clinical psychologists and social workers, respondents reported clients “watery eyes” in 21% of sessions, “some tears” in 15%, “many tears” in 9%, and “sobbing” in 3%. Labott (2001), in a chapter on patient crying in psychotherapy, reviews the scant research on client crying and theories of crying in psychotherapy, as well as describes a small amount of research devoted to the importance of patient crying to therapeutic outcome. Van Heukelem (1979), a pastoral counselor, wrote an article called “
Weep with Those Who Weep,” in which she encourages therapists to support and facilitate a client’s crying. In so doing, she briefly suggests that one possible response to client crying is a therapist’s own tears. In a chapter on patient crying in psychotherapy,
Nelson (2008) presents an attachment-based perspective on crying and discusses how therapists may use their clients’ tears as a point of clinical assessment and intervention. Like Van Heukelem, Nelson acknowledges that therapists may have the urge to cry themselves when working with a crying client, and goes on to explain that the therapist’s own internal response to patient crying, which may lead to TCIT, provides valuable information about the patient’s grief and attachment orientation. In this way, the therapists’ understanding of and reaction to the clients’ tears—including the therapists’ own urge to cry—have important diagnostic and treatment-planning implications. However, no research to date has investigated the clinical contexts in which TCIT specifically tends to occur, or therapists’ understanding of their own tears.
Given that the majority of psychologists and psychology trainees will experience TCIT in their careers (72% in
Blume-Marcovici, et al., 2013; 57% in
Pope, Tabachnick, & Keith-Spiegel, 1987), it is important to have information regarding typical scenarios in which TCIT occurs, as well as psychologists’ experience of TCIT when it does occur. The present study sought to investigate psychologists’ and psychology trainees’ experiences of TCIT by reviewing data from 411 accounts of respondents’ most recent experience of crying in therapy. Three broad research questions were posed:
(1)
What is the clinical context in which TCIT occurs?
(2)
What is the experience of TCIT from the therapists’ perspective?
(3)
What is the impact of TCIT on treatment?
In answering these exploratory questions, the authors intend to present a framework for understanding the therapeutic situations in which TCIT may be most likely to occur (i.e., client demographics, when in treatment TCIT happens, session content), therapists’ experiences of their own tears (i.e., emotions felt, comfort/discomfort with tears, regret), and how clinicians work with their own tears in session (i.e., regarding therapeutic rapport, discussing TCIT with clients).
Method
Design and Ethical Issues
The study employed a survey design, asking psychologists and psychology trainees retrospectively about their experiences with TCIT. The survey was completed online. The study was approved by the Research Ethic Board of Alliant International University.
Participants
Four hundred and eleven (N = 411) U.S. psychologists, postdoctoral psychology fellows and psychology graduate students who reported that they had previously cried in therapy at least one time were asked to complete an additional survey about their most recent experience of crying in therapy.
In terms of sex, 80% (n = 327) of respondents were female, 20% (n = 84) male (N = 411). Ethnically, 82% (n = 333) of respondents identified as non-Hispanic white, 5% (n = 21) as Asian or Asian American, 5% (n = 21) as Hispanic or Latino, and 2% (n = 9) as Black or African American. The rest of other ethnicities made up 6% (n = 23) of respondents. Respondents’ age ranged from 24 to 72 years, with a mean of 37 years and a median of 26 years (N = 409). At least 40 U.S. states were represented (N = 411).
In terms of professional status, 49% (n = 202) of respondents were graduate students in doctoral programs in psychology, 47% (n = 193) were licensed psychologists, and 4% (n = 16) were post-doctoral fellows or license-eligible psychologists (N = 411). Regarding theoretical orientation, 27% (n = 117) of respondents identified as cognitive-behavioral therapists, 25% (n = 102) as eclectic/integrative with a psychodynamic emphasis, 22% (n = 89) as eclectic/integrative without a psychodynamic emphasis, 12% (n = 48) as psychodynamic, 12% (n = 48) as “other,” and 2% (n = 7) as psychoanalytic (N = 411).
Measures
The present study was conducted as part of a larger research project on TCIT (
Blume-Marcovici, 2012): how often and when it occurs, demographic and clinical factors pertaining to which therapists cry, and perceived clinical impact of TCIT (Note:
crying was defined following the Adult Crying Inventory (
Vingerhoets & Cornelius, 2001): “tears in one’s eyes due to emotional reasons”). Participants from the larger study (
N = 684) completed the TCIT Survey 1, a 40-item survey created by the research team to gather information about whether or not—and which—therapists had cried in therapy (results reported elsewhere;
Blume-Marcovici, et al., 2013). Of these respondents, 72% reported previously having cried at least once in therapy. Those 72% of participants who reported having previously cried in therapy and who consented to complete a second survey (
N = 411), also completed TCIT Survey 2, a 49-item survey created by the research team to gather information about past experiences of TCIT. The TCIT Survey 2 had three primary subsections—Clinical Context of TCIT, Therapists’ Experiences of TCIT and TCIT’s Impact on Treatment—and yielded both quantitative and narrative data. The TCIT Survey 2 was piloted on a group of 20 psychology graduate students prior to the start of the study, and feedback was incorporated into final survey.
The TCIT Survey 2 included items utilizing 7-point Likert-type scales (e.g., “The client I worked with was aware that I was crying,” where 1 = strongly disagree, 4 = neither agree nor disagree, and 7 = strongly agree), and nominal scales (e.g., “Did you discuss your tears with your client?” to which the respondent could select “Yes” or “No”). Several items were in an unstructured, narrative format to which respondents could write in open-ended responses (e.g., “Describe non-identifying session content of the therapy session”). Two items used a checklist format (e.g., “Which emotion(s) did you feel when you cried in therapy? Twenty emotions were listed and the respondent could check as many as necessary. “Which emotion(s) do you believe your client felt when you cried?” which listed the same 20 emotions).
Procedure
All data was collected through a single, cross-sectional, retrospective, self-report internet survey. A recruitment email, with a hyperlink to the survey, was sent to university program directors, psychology training sites, and psychology associations throughout the United States. Participants were asked to give their consent to participating by checking “yes” or “no” on a consent form. Participants were not compensated for their participation in the study, but could choose to donate five dollars. to one of two non-profit organizations (UNICEF or The Nature Conservancy) upon completion of the survey. Those participants who responded on TCIT Survey 1 that they had previously cried in therapy were prompted to complete TCIT Survey 2, which asked participants to answer questions about their most recent experience of TCIT. Quantitative and qualitative data was collected.
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics were used to determine frequency of responses regarding characteristics of clients with whom respondents cried in therapy, other aspects of the clinical context of TCIT, therapists’ experiences of TCIT and the impact of TCIT. Pearson’s Chi Square tests were run to analyze response trends in items related to the clinical context of TCIT (e.g., significant differences in sex of client, and therapy format in which TCIT occurred) and to determine any significant differences in whether respondents discussed their TCIT with their clients. One-way ANOVAs were calculated to determine any significant trends regarding groups of therapists who were more (or less) likely to discuss their TCIT with their clients. In exploratory analyses, Pearson correlations and Spearman rank-order correlations were calculated to determine significant relationship trends between variables regarding clinicians’ experiences of TCIT.
Content analysis was performed to analyze narrative responses to a question regarding session content during the respondents’ most recent TCIT episode. The responses were coded by the primary researcher for session content themes. These codes were reviewed by a psychology graduate-student research group to reach consensus about any unclear content codes and multiple/overlapping codes. Based upon the groups’ decision, themes were updated and divided into overarching content categories by the primary researcher. These overarching categories were reviewed and revised by two psychologists acting as advisors to the project. Frequency of content categories, as well as content themes within categories, was analyzed and reported.
Discussion
Typical TCIT Clinical Context
Based on data from the 411 most recent TCIT episodes collected and analyzed in the present study, it appears that TCIT occurs with a range of clients (including children, adolescents, and adults) who have a range of diagnoses and in a range of session content areas. Similar to the few prior studies on TCIT (
Counselman, 1997;
Rhue, 2001;
Waldman, 1995), content analysis of the present sample’s most recent TCIT episode session content determined that grief was the most common session topic in which TCIT occurred, followed by trauma and termination. However, the session topics that accompanied TCIT varied greatly (15% did not fit in any specific content category). The majority of most recent TCIT episodes were reported to be in individual therapy settings, with fewer cases reported in group, couples, and family treatment settings. Crying was most often described as instantaneous or brief (less than one minute in duration) and as occurring in the form of tears in the eyes (i.e., “tearing up”), though longer and more intense (i.e., tears on cheeks, sobbing) crying by the therapist was also reported. Therapist crying in therapy was reported to happen throughout the stages of treatment, from first meeting with a client (i.e., during an intake) to finally saying goodbye (i.e., during a termination session). However, TCIT was most likely to occur in “mid-treatment” or “late in treatment” than in other stage of therapy. This may be because of the inclusive nature of the categories; the labels “mid-treatment” and “late in treatment” encompassed more therapy sessions than the other more specific options (intake session, early in treatment, termination session) and, therefore by default, more respondents selected them. However, given that the label “early in treatment” was similarly inclusive, a more substantive hypothesis is that TCIT occurs more frequently once a strong bond between client and therapist has been established. In addition, as described above, termination was a session content that tended to accompany TCIT and discussion of termination likely occurs later in treatment, in preparation for saying good-bye. Therapist crying in therapy was least likely to be reported to occur in an intake session. This finding seems to support the results of an informal survey on TCIT conducted by Nelson (2007) in which respondents anecdotally reported that TCIT early in treatment would be more likely to have a detrimental impact on therapy.
Respondents overall reported crying more frequently with female clients. However, when taking into account the sex of the therapist, it appears that female therapists were more likely to report crying with female clients, while male therapists were equally likely to report crying with male and female clients. In a dissertation on self-disclosure,
Hansen (2008) described a similar finding. She found that male therapists disclosed with both male and female clients while female therapists disclosed less with male clients. It is, thus, possible that the gender trends found in therapist self-disclosure may apply to trends in TCIT. Future research may benefit from investigating this further.
More often than not, the client was aware of the clinician’s tears (by the therapist’s report). It is important to note that this was not inherently negative or therapeutically detrimental. In fact, clinicians whose clients were aware of their tears reported feeling more comfortable with TCIT than clinicians whose clients were not aware of their therapist’s crying. It may be that the latter (i.e., clinicians whose clients did not notice TCIT) were preoccupied with hiding their tears and, thus, the experience of TCIT was more uncomfortable due to fear—and anticipated shame—of being exposed. It is also possible that clinicians who were more comfortable with TCIT were less likely to hide their tears and more likely to explicitly “use” their own tears in a therapeutic sense. Indeed, clinicians who reported that their client was aware of their tears were also more likely to have reported discussing their tears with their client.
Therapists’ Experiences of TCIT
The most common emotions that accompanied TCIT for the clinician were sadness and feeling “touched” or moved by emotion, with more than half of clinicians indicating that they felt these emotions in their most recent TCIT episode. Most of the time clinicians reported feeling comfortable with their own TCIT, their tears were authentic, and their tears were related to their client’s situation (as opposed to the therapist’s personal life circumstances). When the latter was true, however, (i.e., TCIT was related to therapist’s own personal situation, as it was 16% of the time in the present study) clinicians were more likely to report feeling uncomfortable with their tears and to regret TCIT. Regret about TCIT in general, however, was relatively uncommon, with less than 10% of respondents in the present study indicating that they wished they had not cried in their most recent TCIT episode.
Impact of TCIT
Most of the time (i.e., in 84.5% of cases) TCIT was not a clinical technique consciously utilized by a clinician, suggesting that TCIT typically occurs without clinical motivation. Based on research by
Cornelius (1981;
1997), who wrote that the social purpose of tears is to communicate sympathy, and that they can only communicate sympathy if they are seen as involuntary, we surmise that the involuntary nature of TCIT may be important to transmitting genuine compassion. While TCIT appears not to be a conscious intervention or technique, it can lead to new insights, as was the case in 28% of most recent TCIT episodes reported. Further, TCIT itself may be the impetus for an intervention, such as discussing the new insight or discussing the TCIT itself.
To Discuss or Not to Discuss
When TCIT occurs, the clinician is in a position to decide whether or not to address it with the client. The majority of respondents in the present sample did not discuss TCIT with their clients (61%); however, 39% did discuss their tears, and those who did were significantly more likely to report improvement in the relationship with the client. It may be that discussion of TCIT leads to increased rapport because it allows the client to fully appreciate the therapists’ empathy.
Orlinksky & Howard (1986), for instance, explain that only client-perceived empathy is related to therapeutic outcome. Whatever the mechanism, it appears that discussing TCIT with a client may be a way to make it productive and positive within the therapy.
The intensity of a clinician’s tears may have been a factor in whether or not a clinician discussed TCIT with their client. Clinicians with more intense tears (i.e., tears on cheeks and sobbing versus tears in eyes only) were significantly more likely to report discussing TCIT with their client. Since discussion of TCIT was related to reported improvement in rapport, and discussing TCIT occurred more often with more intense tears, it may be particularly advisable to discuss TCIT with a client when a clinician’s tears are more intense or more visible to the client.
The present study did not capture details regarding how clinicians discussed their tears with their clients. While discussing tears was associated with relationship improvement, numerous respondents who did not discuss their tears still reported relationship improvement. Indeed, in 66% of TCIT episodes, respondents reported that their tears communicated something to their client that words could not express.
The clinician may want to reflect on whether new information was learned about the client or the therapy due to TCIT when deciding whether to discuss it further. If new information or a new insight about the client or the therapeutic relationship was gleaned due to TCIT (as 28% of therapists in the present study reported), the therapist may want to consider whether this information would be beneficial to share with the client. A TCIT case example was described in the Results section (TCIT and insight) in which a clinician described crying when she felt protective and angered on behalf of her client. As the respondent noted, the TCIT may have provided her with new information about her client’s pattern of relating. The clinician was then in a position to decide whether or not to discuss her tears with her client as a means of sharing this new understanding of the client’s relational style (particularly if the therapist had other evidence to support this interpretation and believes it would be beneficial to the treatment).
As reported in the Results section, 27% of clients were not crying during the most recent TCIT episode, and content analysis revealed a session content theme of the client’s lack of emotion accompanying TCIT in a subset of cases. This suggests that therapists’ tears may, at times, be a sign of unprocessed or unexpressed emotion by the client. If the therapist believes TCIT has occurred due to “projection,” the clinician has, by virtue of this understanding, gained new information about the client’s defensive structure, as well as information about what emotions are not tolerable to the client. This may be useful information to share with the client. If the clinician becomes aware of a strong emotion (sadness, feeling touched, warmth, loss, powerlessness, gratitude and joy were the most commonly reported) causing the TCIT (possibly through projection), the clinician may consider interpreting or reflecting this emotion through discussion of his/her own TCIT, particularly if it appears that the client is not aware of feeling such an emotion.
Keeping TCIT Client-Centered
Based on optional comments, a theme emerged in which respondents expressed a concern that TCIT could shift the therapy to become therapist centered. Several participants cautioned that it is “important to keep the focus on the client.” We recommend that if a clinician chooses to discuss his/her tears with the client, it the focus should be on the client’s experience of TCIT, or on the clinicians’ understanding of how TCIT relates to the client’s treatment. The therapist may verbalize to a client who expresses no affect that his/her story is in fact sad, discuss the importance of emotional expression or containment, express the sense of connection the therapist feels to a client who may struggle to feel connected or validate an emotion to a client who has been consistently invalidated.
If a therapist believes that the cause of TCIT was caused by personal circumstances or emotions (e.g., grief about the therapist’s own loss, TCIT related to therapist burnout, etc.), the therapist be particularly thoughtful about the choice to discuss TCIT with the client. If the therapist chooses to discuss TCIT, it should be with the well-being and benefit of the client in mind, not in an effort to make an excuse, give a personal explanation, or to find further emotional release. Similar guidelines have been applied to therapist self-disclosure (
Goldfried, Burckell, & Eubanks-Carter, 2003;
Knox & Hill, 2003).
Limitations
The present study surveyed only psychologists and psychology trainees. No data was collected from clients or from outside observers, thus, the perspectives in this study are exclusively those of practitioners. Future research would do well to incorporate the important perspective of clients regarding TCIT. Additionally, the present study asked participants to focus on their most recent experience with TCIT as a means of increasing the likelihood of accurate recall and eliciting a “typical” scenario for TCIT; however, it may be that most recent TCIT experiences were not “typical.” They also may not have been as meaningful or powerful as clinicians’ other experiences of TCIT (as several participants commented at the end of the survey). Future researchers may consider focusing on eliciting respondents’ most profound experiences of TCIT, as a means of capturing the full potential positive and/or negative impact of TCIT on treatment.
Other important limitations are that this study was based on a sample that was largely Caucasian (82%) and was of a limited size (
N = 411), both of which impact generalizability. In addition, the self-report survey method asked respondents to recall events from memory, and such autobiographical events may be poorly remembered; further, memory distortions are more pronounced when the events being recalled are emotional in nature (
Bradburn, Rips, & Shevell, 1987).
An additional noteworthy study limitation (that is also a direction for future research) is the omission of details regarding how therapists discussed their TCIT with clients. This may include a switch from a retrospective study (such as the present study) to a more process-oriented one. In addition, the content analysis conducted in the present study was aimed specifically at determining session content (as this was one of the original research questions). More open-ended, qualitative research approaches to broader-based narrative accounts of TCIT and/or interviews with practitioners and clients may allow new knowledge about TCIT to emerge. Future research may also focus on therapist countertransference as well as understanding the specific stages of therapy in which therapists cry.