The 100-msec latency auditory evoked field component, termed M100, has shown sex-specific anomalous asymmetry in actively psychotic patients with paranoid schizophrenia (
1). In normal adults the neuroanatomical source of the M100 component is further anterior in the right hemisphere, but this asymmetry is less pronounced in women than in men (
2–
4). Actively psychotic men with DSM-III-R paranoid schizophrenia demonstrate less asymmetry (source appears both relatively more anterior in the left hemisphere and less anterior in the right hemisphere) (
5,
6), whereas actively psychotic women appear to demonstrate greater asymmetry (source is further anterior in the right hemisphere) (
1). The M100 component appears to be generated on or near the transverse gyrus of Heschl on the superior temporal gyrus (
7–
10), with possible contributions from auditory association areas surrounding Heschl's gyrus (
11,
12). The M100 is considered by some a physiological index of echoic, or auditory, sensory memory (
11,
13,
14).
A logical question, then, is does the anomalous asymmetry of M100 in schizophrenia reflect abnormal placement of Heschl's gyrus on the superior temporal gyrus, indicate abnormal placement of the superior temporal gyrus within the brain, or rather, suggest that cortical regions other than Heschl's gyrus produce this auditory evoked field component in patients with schizophrenia? Sensory cortical reorganization in schizophrenia has yet to be empirically demonstrated, although substantial evidence supports intracortical cytoarchitectural abnormalities (
15–
19). This question, as well as the prominence of auditory-system-related symptoms (i.e., hallucinations) in schizophrenia and the central relationship of Heschl's gyrus to the auditory system, led us to the present study.
This study was undertaken to clarify what abnormalities, if any, are present with respect to the volume, surface area, and location of Heschl's gyri in persons with schizophrenia and also to directly compare the anatomical findings with the functional information provided by magnetoencephalographic source localization of the M100 component. We therefore examined the location of Heschl's gyrus on the superior temporal gyrus and in relation to the whole brain in patients with schizophrenia. We also determined the superior temporal gyrus's position within the brains of the subjects, and we examined the location of the M100 generator source in relation to the aforementioned structures. We hypothesized that the sex-specific abnormalities in physiological function reflected in the M100 of patients with schizophrenia would not be reflected in the M100's anatomical substrate, as a preliminary test of our theoretical position concerning functional reorganization of the auditory cortex in schizophrenia (
1).
METHOD
Subjects
Forty-five subjects participated in this study. Twenty-one were diagnosed with paranoid schizophrenia (10 women), and 24 nonschizophrenic persons served as comparison subjects (11 women). All subjects were strongly right-handed; i.e., they had scores of 0.50 or higher on the Annett handedness inventory (
20). Demographic data on the comparison and patient groups are provided in
table 1.
The patients were volunteers recruited either from outpatient clinics in the Denver metropolitan area or from the Psychiatry Service at the Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center in Denver. Patient diagnosis was made by DSM-III-R criteria on the basis of standardized coding of a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R—Patient Version (SCID-P) (
21) performed by one of us (M.R.) and a review of medical records. Each patient had a history of neuroleptic medication, and 18 were medicated at the time of the study. For the patients (16 of 18) taking medications for which phenothiazine equivalency has been established (
22,
23), the mean chlorpromazine-equivalent dose was 360.60 mg/day (SD=392.47). The patients with schizophrenia were all actively psychotic, i.e., they had had active hallucinatory activity at least within the past month. The nonschizophrenic comparison subjects had never been mentally ill according to the Research Diagnostic Criteria (
24). All subjects were given a complete description of the study, after which they signed informed consent statements. The project was approved by the Colorado Multiple Institutional Review Board human subjects committee.
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed at the University of Colorado hospital by using a GE Signa 1.5-T system. Images of the head were acquired with a standard head coil by using a spoiled-gradient echo acquisition. This resulted in 1.7-mm-thick T1-weighted images with a TR of 40 msec, a TE of 5 msec, and a 40° flip angle. The 256×256 matrix acquisition resulted in 0.94×0.94×1.7-mm voxel dimensions. The entire head was encompassed within 124 slices. The MRI images were then transferred to a personal computer and stored on optical disks for further analyses.
Image Reconstruction and Definitions of Regions of Interest
With custom image-processing software routines designed in Interactive Data Language 3.1.1 (Research Systems, Boulder, Colo.), each set of brain images was segmented by an experienced investigator (J. Sheeder) who was blind to the diagnosis and sex of the subject. Segmentation was accomplished by using a combination of automated pixel thresholding and manual segmentation. From the segmented brain images, total brain volume was calculated. This volume-computation algorithm has been tested on regular and irregular phantoms of known volumes and found to be accurate to within ±1%. Manual segmentation and volume computations (by M.R., who was also blind to sex and diagnosis) were also performed for the superior temporal gyrus and Heschl's gyrus for both hemispheres of each brain. The superior temporal gyrus included all clearly visible slices of the superior temporal gyrus anterior to the fornix (inclusive). Total brain and superior temporal gyrus volumes for 40 of these subjects have been reported elsewhere (
1) and are used in this report only for geometric comparisons with Heschl's gyri. We are unaware of any published volumetric criteria concerning Heschl's gyrus data and therefore present the method used in this project in more detail.
Heschl's gyrus traverses an anterior-lateral to posterior-medial course on the superior surface of the superior temporal gyrus. Its most anterior anterior-lateral boundary is indistinct and may fuse with the anterior superior temporal gyrus (
4). Its posterior-medial course and junction with the insula are distinct. Scanning the entire coronal series, we first identified Heschl's gyrus at its prominent midsuperior temporal gyrus position, where its medial and lateral boundaries are unequivocal. We next moved anteriorly until the white matter stem, and the associated gray matter accentuation, merged indistinguishably with the anterior superior temporal gyrus. This defined the most anterior slice. We then moved posteriorly until Heschl's gyrus gray matter was no longer distinct from the insular origin at the circular sulcus. The slice anterior to this defined the posterior boundary of Heschl's gyrus. We have found that surface renderings of the superior temporal gyrus viewed from the superior perspective can be helpful in identifying useful landmarks for segmenting Heschl's gyrus images. Reconstructions of the superior temporal gyrus are adequate for this purpose. They allow for the identification of the anterior and posterior transverse temporal sulci (Heschl's sulcus), which are useful in conjunction with the segmentation criteria described.
It has been reported that many superior temporal gyri appear to contain two (or more) Heschl's gyri, on the basis of the dorsal surface view of the superior temporal gyrus. Galaburda and Sanides (
25) suggested that only the most anterior transverse gyrus should be considered Heschl's gyrus, and any more-posterior transverse gyrus should be considered part of the temporal planum. This is supported by a study by Rademacher et al. (
26), who reported that in cases where two Heschl's gyri were present, auditory koniocortex (area 41 of Brodmann [27]) was restricted to the more anterior gyrus. We also found additional transverse gyri, but in no case (in 96 hemispheres) did we find a second transverse gyrus extending the full anterior-lateral to posterior-medial extent of the superior temporal gyrus. There were cases in which a second (posterior) transverse gyrus merged with Heschl's gyrus (defined as described in the preceding) before the Heschl's gyrus merged with the insula. In those cases we included the volume of the second (posterior) transverse gyrus at the point where the two gyri merged to include a single common white matter stem, before it merged with the insula. The portion of the second (posterior) transverse gyrus lateral to the anterior transverse gyrus that was supported by a separate white matter stem was not included in the volume. Our segmented volumes for the Heschl's gyrus included both the white matter stem and gray matter of the gyrus.
To provide a basis for comparison with previous reports (e.g., Kulynych et al. [28]), surface areas were also determined for each of the Heschl's gyri. These were obtained by calculating the length of a line (in millimeters) traced on the interface of the gray matter and fluid of the gyrus in each coronal image, then multiplying the result by the slice thickness.
Reliability of MRI Measurements
The reliability of the measurements of total brain volume has been discussed in another paper (
1). To determine the reliability of the measurements of Heschl's gyrus and surface area, 10 participants were randomly selected from the original 45, for a total of 20 Heschl's gyri for reliability analyses. The images of all 20 gyri were segmented by the first author (D.C.R.), who then calculated Heschl's gyrus volume and surface area as already described. Interrater reliability was determined by comparing these measurements with the volumetric measurements and surface area measurements produced by the last author (M.R.); the intraclass correlation coefficients for Heschl's gyrus volume and surface area were ICC=0.90 and ICC=0.72, respectively. Each volume was then redetermined by the first author 1 week from the original segmentation; the estimates of intrarater reliability were ICC=0.93 and ICC=0.94 for Heschl's gyrus volume and surface area, respectively.
Centroid Measurements
We also wished to determine the absolute three-dimensional location of Heschl's gyrus within the brain to permit direct comparison of the M100 source generator location and its gross anatomical substrate. We therefore calculated the centroid (geometric center of mass, with the assumption of uniform pixel density) of each slice in the regions of interest (total brain, Heschl's gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus) to assess the location of the region of interest within the skeletally based coordinate system. This required coregistration of magnetoencephalographic and MRI coordinate systems. For this purpose, before MRI was performed vitamin A capsules were attached to the nasion and two preauricular points. The skin-capsule interface points are small with respect to the capsule dimensions and are easily identified visually in the MRI. The MRI coordinates of the centroid units were converted into skeletal coordinates by a standard transformation (
29). A geometric summation of the centroids of individual two-dimensional slices provided a single centroid measure for each Heschl's gyrus and each superior temporal gyrus in each hemisphere, as well as the geometric center of mass for the entire brain.
Figure 1 illustrates the skeletally based coordinate system and resulting surface reconstructions of the superior temporal gyrus, Heschl's gyrus, and centroids for four representative subjects.
Neuromagnetic Recordings and M100 Source Localization
Data on M100 lateralization from 39 of the participants have been reported elsewhere (
1) and will not be discussed in this paper. Data for six additional subjects were recorded for this project. Details of the recording procedures and equipment have been reported in several previous publications (
1,
4,
10). Briefly, we recorded neuromagnetic activity from each hemisphere in response to 1-kHz tone pips, at a 90-dB sound pressure level, delivered to the contralateral ear with a random interstimulus interval between 1.2 and 1.8 sec. Recordings were obtained from an area sufficient to encompass both outgoing and ingoing magnetic extrema (approximately 35 locations per hemisphere). Auditory evoked field signals within a 20-msec poststimulus time window around the M100 peak latency (identified as the largest signal between 60 and 120 msec poststimulus) were used in an inverse solution algorithm (
10) to estimate M100 source locations with reference to the skeletal coordinate system. M100 data for two subjects (one male comparison subject and one male patient) were excluded from further analyses because the equivalent current dipole locations were viewed as anatomically unreasonable (equivalent current dipole location ±95% spherical confidence region of 7-mm radius was not contained on the superior temporal gyrus), possibly because of digitization error or excessive noise in the magnetoencephalographic signals.
For both hemispheres in each subject, a set of x, y, and z difference vectors (X, Y, and Z) was computed as the distance from the three-dimensional location of the centroid of Heschl's gyrus to the three-dimensional M100 source location (e.g., Y=yHeschl's–yM100). These vectors provide measurements of the in-plane directional distance between functional source activity (the M100) and the putative neuroanatomical generator (Heschl's gyrus). We also computed the straight-line distance between the two points by taking the square root of the sums of the squares of the three vectors. We realize that there is no a priori reason to expect the M100 source to be absolutely co-located with the centroid of Heschl's gyrus, although we assume that the tonotopic representation of a 1-kHz sound on the auditory cortex has a regular, if yet undefined, relationship to the center of Heschl's gyrus. Therefore, the difference vectors provide only the distance from the center of the gyrus that represents the anatomical correlate of the primary auditory cortex to the source of neurophysiological activity putatively generated in that cytoarchitectural region. The same set of vector computations was also performed to ascertain the relationship between Heschl's gyrus and the superior temporal gyrus and the relationship of the superior temporal gyrus to the entire brain.
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were performed by using Statistica 5.0 (StatSoft, Tulsa, Okla.). All analyses were two-tailed and evaluated for significance at the 0.05 alpha level. Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) sums of squares were computed with controls for partial correlations with other effects in the design (i.e., type III sums of squares).
To test for possible differences in demographic variables of interest, 2×2 ANOVAs (sex by diagnosis) were performed for handedness, education level, and age. Independent t tests of differences between male and female patients were performed for age at onset, illness duration, global assessment of functioning score from the SCID-P, and phenothiazine-equivalent doses. Subsequently, Pearson coefficients (r) were computed for the correlations between any demographic variable with a significant effect and all other dependent variables to ascertain potential confounding effects.
The volume of Heschl's gyrus was analyzed with a 2×2×2 (sex by diagnosis by hemisphere) mixed-design analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with hemisphere treated as a within-subjects variable and total brain volume used as the single covariate. Total brain volume was used as a covariate in this analysis because we have previously reported a trend for the brains of women with schizophrenia to be smaller than those of healthy female comparison subjects (
1). Therefore, the usual correction for interindividual differences in brain size would have been inappropriate, since Heschl's gyrus may have appeared abnormally large in the female schizophrenic patients given their smaller brain volumes. The surface area of Heschl's gyrus was also subjected to an equivalent ANOVA, uncorrected for brain size since there is no appropriate brain surface area metric to use in the correction. In addition, correlations between surface area measurements and volumetric measurements for Heschl's gyrus were computed.
To examine the relationships between the M100 source and Heschl's gyrus, between Heschl's gyrus and the superior temporal gyrus, and between the superior temporal gyrus and the total brain, the vector computations X, Y, and Z were used as dependent variables in separate 2×2×2 mixed ANOVAs (sex by diagnosis by hemisphere). The distances between the three-dimensional locations of the M100 and Heschl's gyrus were also analyzed this way.
DISCUSSION
Several interesting findings emerged from this study. First, volumetric abnormalities in Heschl's gyrus appear to be sex specific. In our study group, only the men with schizophrenia had smaller Heschl's gyri than comparison subjects of the same sex. These findings are consonant with other data from our laboratory on superior temporal gyrus volumes, indicating that male but not female patients with schizophrenia have smaller than normal superior temporal gyrus volumes (
1). The observation that the women with schizophrenia had slightly larger Heschl's gyrus volumes than those of the female comparison subjects is unique but not without precedent. Larger than normal volumes in schizophrenia have previously been reported for the caudate nucleus and putamen (
30).
We are not aware of previous studies that have addressed the volumes of Heschl's gyri in people with schizophrenia, although at least two studies of Heschl's gyrus surface area showed no group differences between persons with schizophrenia and comparison subjects (
28,
31). Our surface area measurements were consistent with the ones from these previous reports, showing no difference between patients and comparison subjects. Several issues may relate to the disparity between the volumetric and surface area observations: a) the surface area measurements were not corrected for total brain surface area, complicating their interpretation in the face of interindividual differences in brain size; b) surface area would appear to primarily reflect gray matter, whereas our volume measure included both gray and white matter; and finally, c) surface area measures may imperfectly represent deeper gray matter, particularly between opposed gyri where the actual surface does not follow the apparent gray matter invagination (i.e., where the gyri appear to be in close apposition, with no real sulcus). This might tend to decrease the sensitivity of the measure in a manner akin to decreased signal-to-noise ratio.
Second, we found no evidence of a sex-specific abnormality in axial-plane location of Heschl's gyrus centroids in our subjects with schizophrenia. While the Heschl's gyrus centroids were more anterior in the right hemisphere, this effect was not significantly related to either sex or diagnosis. Thus, the previously reported anomalous asymmetry of M100 source location in schizophrenia does not appear to reflect an anatomical shift of Heschl's gyrus. This raises the possibility that the cortex involved in generating the M100 has a different relationship to Heschl's gyrus than is the case for persons without schizophrenia. Should later studies support this conclusion, two explanations might be entertained. First, this could reflect a developmental anomaly in which the auditory cytoarchitectural region supporting echoic memory (the M100 source) is diverted from its normal development with respect to Heschl's gyrus. Alternatively, the symptoms of schizophrenia (e.g., auditory hallucinatory activity) might induce a plastic reorganization of cortical functions whose effects are reflected in altered M100 location (e.g., see reference 1), such that a different auditory region generates the M100 in hallucinating men. Both possibilities have precedents in the literature. With respect to the first, magnetoencephalography has shown evidence of plastic reorganization of digit representation in musicians (
32). Directly related to the second, Tiihonen et al. (
33) found alteration in M100 latency in a single subject with schizophrenia who was recorded while hallucinating.
In addition, when we directly compared the location of the M100 auditory evoked field source to the anatomical location of Heschl's gyrus (by means of the difference vector), we found a slight reversal of the axial-plane relationship between structure and function specific to the left hemisphere of men with schizophrenia, suggesting that the functional displacement in men with the disorder may be restricted to the left hemisphere. This finding is consistent with our first report of the phenomenon (
5) and with anatomical studies of the superior temporal gyrus that suggest left-hemisphere-specific low volume in men (
34,
35). We have also recently reported neuromagnetic evidence suggesting that men with schizophrenia have a deficit in auditory short-term memory scanning and retrieval specific to the left hemisphere, possibly also reflecting this anomaly in cortical function (
36).
Many neuroimaging studies have used only male patients diagnosed with the disease (
28,
34,
35,
37,
38), and thus the generalizability of their findings may be limited. Others (
31,
39) have studied male and female subjects combined in the same group, clouding any potential sex-related neuroanatomical differences. The increasingly likely possibility of sex-related differences in superior temporal gyrus anatomy (
40,
41) and function (
1) in schizophrenia is potentially critical to our understanding of the disorder, however. Although the potential for sex differences in neuroanatomical morphology has received relatively little attention in the schizophrenia literature (
42), and findings are sometimes conflicting (as reviewed by Cowell et al. [43]), there are well-described differences in the clinical features of the disorder (
44–
46). Our finding of sex-specific anomalous representation of auditory cortical activity in the superior temporal gyrus in schizophrenia is therefore intriguing and may relate to the sex differences in clinical features of the disorder. The broader issue of whether relative preservation of brain morphology in women with schizophrenia relates to their overall better treatment response and outcome and whether this may reflect in part a neuroprotective effect of sex hormones on these structures awaits future study.
The results of this study may be somewhat limited in that we used a relatively homogeneous group of subjects with schizophrenia (i.e., those with primarily positive symptoms). Studies will need to be undertaken to ascertain to what extent, if any, these findings generalize to other schizophrenia subtypes. Finally, the scope of this study with respect to sex specificity is limited by lack of data concerning clinically observable sex differences in the study group. Future efforts will need to include comprehensive evaluation of symptoms, as well as brain structure and function, to determine the relevance of these sex-specific findings to the disorder.
Our findings are essentially consistent with the view that schizophrenia is a neurodevelopmental disorder (
47,
48); the evidence points to different neuroanatomical expression in the two sexes. On the basis of these findings and those from other studies of auditory evoked fields (
1,
5,
36), we believe it is possible that some functional reorganization of the primary auditory cortex has occurred in these patients, such that there is relative dissociation of that functional region from Heschl's gyrus. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that either medication or the time elapsed since the onset of the disorder is responsible for these results. To our knowledge, the effects of neuroleptic medication on the measures included in this study are not known, and they could theoretically be a considerable confound. Therefore, there is a still a need for studies of this type with first-episode or never-medicated patients with schizophrenia and for longitudinal studies of children at risk for developing the disorder.