In the 25 multiple-incidence autism families, 25 mothers and 23 fathers were eligible to participate; all of the mothers and fathers in the 30 Down’s syndrome families participated. A parent in a family with autism was included in the analysis only if he or she was the parent of two autistic children. Two mothers had autistic children with two different fathers, resulting in inclusion of only 23 autism fathers in this analysis. There was no significant difference between the families with autism and those with Down’s syndrome in father’s age (t=0.76, df=51, p>0.45), father’s level of education (χ
2=1.39, df=4, p=0.85), mother’s age (t=0.29, df=53, p=0.77), or mother’s level of education (χ
2=6.95, df=4, p=0.14). Father’s occupational level, as specified by the British Manual of the Classification of Occupations
(18), also did not differ significantly between the two groups (χ
2=6.23, df=4, p=0.18).
Psychiatric Disorder in Relatives
The results of a comparison of the autism and Down’s syndrome parents on rates of psychiatric disorders, as defined by the Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC), revealed that the autism parents had a significantly higher lifetime rate (33.3% versus 11.7%) of definite RDC major depressive disorder than did the Down’s syndrome parents (χ2=7.51, df=1, p=0.006). The parents of the autistic probands also had a significantly higher rate of social phobia (14.6% versus 3.3%) than the parents of the Down’s syndrome probands (χ2=4.54, df=1, p=0.03). There was no significant association between major depressive disorder and social phobia in the autism parents (χ2=0.32, df=1, p=0.57). No significant group differences were detected in the rates of the other axis I psychiatric disorders examined, including alcoholism (10.4% versus 3.3%), drug abuse (2.1% versus 1.7%), bipolar disorder (0.0% versus 0.0%), panic disorder (0.0% versus 1.7%), generalized anxiety disorder (4.2% versus 3.3%), simple phobia (4.2% versus 5.0%), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (2.1% versus 0.0%). Of the 16 autism parents with RDC major depressive disorder, 12 (75.0%) were female.
As in our previous study
(9), we sought to apply more stringent criteria to the diagnosis of major depressive disorder (recurrent disorder with symptoms of 4 weeks’ duration rather than the 2 weeks required by the RDC). In addition to increasing the required number of episodes (i.e., recurrent disorder) and duration of symptoms (i.e., 4 weeks), in this analysis we also excluded episodes that were temporally related to particularly stressful life events (e.g., occurred within 3 months of the death of a close friend or relative, the diagnosis of autism in a proband, or a divorce or marital separation or occurred in close association with a known organic etiology for depression, severe medical illness, or pregnancy). When these modified criteria for major depression were used, the autism parents continued to show a significantly higher rate of major depressive disorder (18.8% versus 1.7%) than the Down’s syndrome parents (χ
2=10.12, df=1, p=0.001). Eight out of the nine autism parents meeting this modified definition of major depressive disorder experienced his or her first depressive episode before the birth of either autistic child.
To further explore the evidence for an excess of depressive disorders in autism relatives, we examined family history data from the Family History Interview for Developmental Disorders of Cognition and Social Functioning for the probable or definite (i.e., 1 month of symptoms with evidence of impairment or treatment) presence of depressive or anxiety disorder in grandparents and in aunts or uncles. Too few siblings (in addition to the two autistic probands) were available in the multiple-incidence autism families for a meaningful comparison of rates in siblings. Also, the available data on first cousins were not thought to be sufficiently reliable for analysis. In sex-specific comparisons of the two groups of extended relatives (grandparents and aunts/uncles), there were no differences between the autism and Down’s syndrome families in the grandfathers’ age (t=0.66, df=10, p=0.76), grandmothers’ age (t=0.71, df=8, p=0.64), uncles’ age (t=0.58, df=13, p=0.63), or aunts’ age (t=0.37, df=9, p=0.77) or in the grandfathers’ level of education (χ2=3.33, df=7, p=0.59), grandmothers’ education (χ2=2.43, df=10, p=0.61), uncles’ education (χ2=2.77, df=10, p=0.59), or aunts’ education (χ2=5.41, df=7, p=0.85). The rate of depression or anxiety was significantly higher in the grandparents (χ2=4.26, df=1, p=0.04) and the aunts and uncles (χ2=6.0, df=1, p=0.01) of the autism probands. Probable or definite depression or anxiety was found in 17.7% of the grandparents in the autism families, compared to only 8.4% of the grandparents in the Down’s syndrome families, and in 13.2% of the aunts and uncles in the autism families, versus 5.4% of those in the Down’s syndrome families.
Psychiatric Disorder and the Broad Autism Phenotype
To explore the relationship between major depressive disorder and the broad autism phenotype and between social phobia and the broad autism phenotype (i.e., defined as characteristics that are qualitatively similar but milder than those that define autism—social and communication deficits and stereotyped-repetitive behaviors—and show familial aggregation in autism families), we first examined the relationship between the presence (score ≥2) or absence (score=0) of the broad autism phenotype, as already empirically defined (see Analysis section) and the presence of RDC major depressive disorder and social phobia. The broad phenotype status of the autism parents with a score of 1 was considered unknown, and these individuals were not included in this analysis. Using this approach, we found no evidence of a significant association between the presence of the broad autism phenotype and either major depressive disorder (χ2=0.87, df=1) or social phobia (χ2=0.02, df=1). Further analysis also revealed no evidence of a significant relationship between total score for the broad autism phenotype (i.e., a continuous variable rated 0 through 4) and the presence or absence of major depressive disorder (t=0.70, df=44) or social phobia (t=0.11, df=44). More fine-grained analysis also revealed no evidence of a significant relationship between the presence of major depressive disorder and components of the broad autism phenotype, including the personality characteristics aloof, rigid, and anxious, the scores on the Pragmatic Rating Scale and speech measures, and the friendship score, except for a significant relationship between hypersensitivity to criticism and major depressive disorder (χ2=4.91, df=1, p=0.03). Social phobia did show a significant relationship to the scores on the Pragmatic Rating Scale for pragmatic language (t=2.55, df=31, p=0.02 for unequal variances) and for speech (t=3.26, df=42, p=0.002 for unequal variances), i.e., significantly correlated with variables measuring pragmatic language deficits. However, the presence of social phobia was not significantly related to the other components of the broad autism phenotype examined, including friendship score and the personality characteristics aloof, anxious, and hypersensitivity to criticism.