Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 February 2001

Enhanced Verbal Memory in Nondemented Elderly Women Receiving Hormone-Replacement Therapy

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: This study examined the effects of hormone-replacement therapy on memory and other cognitive abilities in cognitively intact older women. METHOD: This prospective observational study in nondemented postmenopausal women aged 50–89 from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging involved study groups consisting of 103 women who were receiving oral or transdermal estrogen-replacement therapy (44 of whom were receiving adjuvant progesterone) and 81 women who had never received such therapy. Groups were naturally matched on education, health status, depressive symptoms, annual income, and general verbal ability. To restrict the study group to cognitively healthy women, prospective clinical data were used to exclude women who developed dementia up to 5 years after assessment. Data were cross-sectional. Multivariate analysis of variance and follow-up univariate analyses of variance were performed to compare those women who were receiving and those who had never received hormone-replacement therapy on measures of verbal memory, figural memory, mental rotations, attention, and working memory. RESULTS: The women receiving hormone-replacement therapy performed significantly better on measures of verbal learning and memory than did those who had never received hormones, but there were no significant differences in scores on other cognitive tests. Specific aspects of memory performance, including encoding and retrieval, were superior among the women receiving hormone therapy. CONCLUSIONS: These findings, based on groups of women who were receiving and had never received hormone-replacement therapy and who were naturally matched on health and cognitive status, suggest that hormone-replacement therapy may have a selective beneficial effect on verbal memory in older nondemented women.
Relatively recent reports suggest that hormone-replacement therapy may have a protective effect on the aging brain. Hormone-replacement therapy may lower the risk of Alzheimer’s disease (1) and prevent normal age-related decline in cognition (2). Neurobiological studies indicate a number of mechanisms through which estrogen might protect the hippocampus and other brain regions affected by dementia and cognitive aging (3). In contrast, findings from three recent randomized trials suggest no beneficial effect of hormone-replacement therapy on cognitive function in women with a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (46). Together, these studies suggest that hormone-replacement therapy may confer cognitive benefits only to women who are free of dementia. In this field of research, studies of cognition in women without dementia have produced the most conflicting results, with some studies showing no beneficial effects of hormone-replacement therapy on cognitive function (7, 8), others revealing a benefit to memory (913), and another showing a general cognitive benefit (14).
Factors contributing to the discrepancy in results across these studies include differences in the age of participants, the focus on current versus any use of hormone-replacement therapy, the cognitive domains studied, the specific tests administered, and the statistical approaches used to control for group differences in confounding variables. However, the most notable limitation of observational studies is the “healthy-user bias,” which is the tendency for women who receive hormone-replacement therapy to be healthier and better educated than nonrecipients (15, 16). Any cognitive advantage seen among recipients of hormone-replacement therapy in nonrandomized studies may be due to the characteristics of the women who elect to receive hormone-replacement therapy rather than the physiological properties of estrogen. To control for healthy-user bias, researchers commonly adjust cognitive test scores for group differences in education. This statistical correction may not adequately control for such bias or may result in overadjustments that mask a reliable but subtle hormonal effect. One approach to limiting the healthy-user bias in observational studies is to examine recipients and nonrecipients of hormone-replacement therapy in a cohort of women whose health and educational status is uniformly high. Study groups selected from such a cohort are more likely to be naturally matched on education, intelligence, and health status, thus eliminating the need for statistical correction. We followed this approach in the present study.
Our primary aims were 1) to compare cognitive function in nondemented older women receiving hormone-replacement therapy and women who had never received such therapy who were naturally matched on factors that have limited previous studies, 2) to investigate whether the apparent beneficial effect of hormone-replacement therapy on cognition is limited to memory or whether the benefits extend to other cognitive domains, and 3) to determine which aspects of memory might be most affected by hormone-replacement therapy. A better understanding of the effects of hormone-replacement therapy on specific aspects of memory processing—encoding, consolidation, and retrieval—might lend critical insights into the discrepancy in findings across several large-scale observational studies using different memory tests. To this end, we conducted a cross-sectional observational study comparing cognitive outcomes in two groups of women—active recipients of hormone-replacement therapy and women who had never received such therapy—that were naturally matched on cognitive and health status. The prospective nature of the study allowed us to exclude women who developed dementia within 5 years of test administration and thereby focus on the effects of hormone-replacement therapy on normal aging. We report evidence that hormone-replacement therapy is associated with an enhancement in cognitive function in nondemented women that is selective to verbal learning and memory.

Method

Study Population

The participants were 184 nondemented postmenopausal women who were enrolled in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging at the National Institute on Aging (17). Participants were aged 50–89, had completed the California Verbal Learning Test, and either were receiving oral or transdermal estrogen or had never received hormone-replacement therapy at the time of assessment. From an original sample of 561 women in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging with California Verbal Learning Test data, we excluded 240 because they were not postmenopausal, 87 because they had received estrogen in the past but were not active recipients at the time of assessment, 28 because they used estrogen creams alone at the time of assessment, 19 because they received a diagnosis of suspected, possible, or probable dementia within 5 years of follow-up (11 who had never received hormone-replacement therapy and eight active recipients), one who had not received hormone-replacement therapy because she had radiation-induced menopause, and two because they demonstrated poor verbal knowledge by scoring three standard deviations below the mean (less than the first percentile) on the Primary Mental Abilities vocabulary test (18) (one who had never received hormone-replacement therapy and one active recipient). Thus the final study group consisted of 81 women who had never received hormone-replacement therapy and 103 active recipients. After a complete description of the study was given to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

Use of Hormone-Replacement Therapy

Current and past estrogen and progesterone use, duration of treatment, and type of treatment (i.e., oral, transdermal, or cream) were documented prospectively at each assessment by the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging by use of a questionnaire designed specifically to assess history of hormone-replacement therapy. The questionnaire data were supplemented at each visit by a complete inventory of all current and past medications. The two study groups of interest were women with no history of transdermal or oral hormone-replacement therapy and women receiving transdermal or oral hormone-replacement therapy at the time of cognitive assessment. To ensure the most reliable estimate of hormone-replacement therapy use, we reviewed each individual’s longitudinal data for self-reported hormone-replacement therapy use and, where necessary, recoded the data. The recoding was based on the assumption that accuracy in the recall of use of hormone-replacement therapy decreases with time since use and with advancing age. Women who at the time of the cognitive assessment reported never receiving estrogen but who indicated either past or current estrogen use at earlier visits (N=17) were coded as past recipients. This coding took advantage of the prospectively collected information and avoided artificially lowering the estimate of memory performance by those who had never received hormone-replacement therapy. The recoding minimized the likelihood of finding significant group differences where none existed. The duration of hormone-replacement therapy was coded on a 7-point scale: 1=less than 6 months, 2=6 months to less than 1 year, 3=1 year to less than 5 years, 4=5 years to less than 10 years, 5=10 years to less than 15 years, 6=15 years to less than 20 years, and 7=20 years or longer. Reliable information on dosage of hormone-replacement therapy was not available.

Tests and Outcome Measures

The California Verbal Learning Test (19) is a widely used assessment of verbal learning and memory composed of two 16-item shopping lists. Each list contains four items (e.g., grapes) from four semantic categories (e.g., fruits). Two of the categories overlap between the two lists, but the items are unique. On the learning trials, the 16 items are read aloud to the participant, who immediately tries to recall as many as possible. The order of administration is five learning trials with list A, a learning trial with list B, and a series of delayed trials with list A, including short-delay free recall, short-delay cued recall, long-delay free recall, long-delay cued recall, and long-delay recognition. California Verbal Learning Test outcome measures consisted of performance on the learning, recall, and recognition trials and several derived measures (see Results). The Benton Visual Retention Test (20) measures short-term figural memory. In each of 10 trials, participants briefly view a line drawing and then immediately try to draw it from memory. The outcome measure was the total number of errors. A modified version (21) of the Card Rotations Test (22) measured mental rotations and required discrimination between two- and three-dimensional rotations of simple geometric figures. The outcome measure was the number of correct responses minus the number of incorrect responses. The digit span test is a subtest of the WAIS-R (23) consisting of two tasks, forward and backward digit span tests, that measure attention and working memory. The outcome measure for each task was the number of correct responses. Self-reported depressive symptoms were measured with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (24), and self-reported health was measured by answer to the question, “Overall, how would you say your health is?” (1=excellent, 2=good, 3=fair, 4=poor).

Data Selection

The California Verbal Learning Test was the primary measure of interest in the present investigation before analysis. Scores for the other tests were obtained for the visit at which the California Verbal Learning Test was first administered (1993 and on) and included scores on the Benton Visual Retention Test for comparison with data from a previous report (9). Participants had taken the Benton Visual Retention Test on average four times previously. In contrast, the Card Rotations Test and digit span tests were introduced into the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging at the same time as the California Verbal Learning Test; in almost every case, measures from those tests are also from the first test administration. The group sizes for some tests were slightly smaller than those for the California Verbal Learning Test measures.

Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted by using SPSS statistical software (version 4.1 for VAX/VMS, SPSS, Chicago). Analyses were conducted on age-corrected z scores. Because of significant differences in age between those who had never received hormone-replacement therapy and active recipients, we applied an age-banding technique for age correction by creating standard scores, a technique that does not assume linearity. This approach was preferred over an analysis of covariance, because the pattern of age-related changes varied across dependent measures, with some measures showing an accelerated increase after age 70 and others showing a more linear increase. To derive age-corrected scores, we first computed the mean and standard deviation for each test measure for each of the 10-year age bands (i.e., 50–59, 60–69, 70–79, and 80–89). For each test, we derived age-corrected z scores for each participant by computing the difference between her score and the average score of the other women in her age band and then dividing the difference by the standard deviation of her age band. These age-corrected z scores are in standardized units (i.e., a score of 0.25 indicates a score one-quarter standard deviation above the mean). The normative group used to compute the average scores included those who had never received hormone-replacement therapy, past recipients, and active recipients of any form of hormone-replacement therapy who met the study’s other inclusionary criteria (i.e., female, aged 50–89, nondemented, and postmenopausal). For those with multiple assessments by the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging, data used in creating age-corrected z scores were from the same test session at which the California Verbal Learning Test was first administered.
To examine the effects of hormone-replacement therapy (i.e., current versus never received) on cognition while controlling for multiple outcomes and intercorrelations among outcome measures, we conducted a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) on all scores simultaneously. When necessary, we entered the inverse value into the MANOVA so that higher values consistently indicated better performance. If the MANOVA revealed a significant group difference, we then conducted a series of univariate analyses to determine the outcome measures on which the groups differed (p<0.05, two-tailed). The same statistical approach was used to explore the effects of adjuvant progesterone use on cognitive performance in recipients of hormone-replacement therapy, although there was limited statistical power to detect such effects in the group of 103 recipients. Effects of the duration of hormone-replacement therapy on cognitive function were examined with Pearson’s product-moment correlations.

Results

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the participants. As expected from current prescribing practices, women who were receiving hormone-replacement therapy were significantly younger than those who had never received it. There were no significant differences between the two groups in education, score on the Primary Mental Abilities vocabulary test, self-reported depressive symptoms, number of diagnoses, self-reported health status, or annual income.

Recipients Versus Nonrecipients

The MANOVA on all scores simultaneously indicated enhanced cognition on all tests combined in recipients of hormone-replacement therapy compared to those who had never received such treatments (F=8.54, df=1, 169, p=0.004). Table 2 presents group means for outcome measures in age-corrected z scores, F and p values from univariate analyses of variance, magnitude of the group difference as an effect size in standardized units (i.e., z scores), and magnitude of the group difference in raw units (e.g., number of words). On the California Verbal Learning Test, recipients of hormone-replacement therapy compared with those who had never received such therapy showed superior performance on a number of measures of verbal learning and memory. These included immediate recall across the five learning trials, immediate recall of list B items, recall after both short and long delays, and recognition (i.e., discriminability, a measure of hits controlling for false positive responses). There was also a nonsignificant effect of recipients of hormone-replacement therapy to perform better on the short- and long-delay (category) cued recall trials. Derived measures of learning and memory lent insights into factors contributing to group differences in memory and learning. Recipients of hormone-replacement therapy showed more clustering of words into semantic categories during learning trials, indicating a more active learning style, and they showed better retention of items from trial 5 to the short-delay free recall trial. Retrieval difficulties among those who had never received hormone-replacement therapy were evident in their improved performance on the recognition trial compared to the long-delay free recall trial. There were no significant group differences on scores from the other cognitive tests, including the forward and backward digit span tests, Card Rotations Test, and Benton Visual Retention Test.

Effects of Hormone-Replacement Therapy

Duration of hormone-replacement therapy did not correlate with any dependent measures (all r<0.18, df=182, all p>0.05). The use of adjuvant progesterone therapy did not have an effect on overall cognition (F=1.55, df=1, 94, p=0.22). However, among recipients of hormone-replacement therapy, the women receiving adjuvant progesterone (N=41 [data were missing for three subjects], mean=0.27, 95% confidence interval [CI]=0.02–0.53) performed significantly better than the women receiving estrogen only (N=55 [data were missing for four subjects], mean=–0.14, 95% CI=–0.38 to 0.09) on the Card Rotations Test (F=5.84, df=1, 95, p=0.02).

Discussion

In this study, women receiving hormone-replacement therapy showed significantly better learning and memory on a verbal memory test than women who never received hormone-replacement therapy. Hormone-replacement therapy was associated with better encoding and retrieval of words and better retention of words after an interference task. In contrast, recipients of hormone-replacement therapy and those who had never received such therapy performed similarly on tests of short-term figural memory, mental rotations, attention, and working memory. These data suggest that hormone-replacement therapy is associated with enhanced verbal memory in nondemented postmenopausal women.
Because this was not a randomized trial, it is possible that the observed group differences in verbal memory were not due to the physiological effects of hormone-replacement therapy on the brain but rather to one or more confounding factors distinguishing those receiving hormone-replacement therapy from those who had never received such therapy. However, we observed no differences between groups on any confound affecting cognition except age. Recipients of hormone-replacement therapy and those who had never received such treatment were selected from a sample of highly educated volunteers in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging who had above-average economic and health status and did not differ in years of education, number of diagnoses, household income, or vocabulary. The availability of naturally matched groups eliminated the need for statistical correction that may have over- or underadjusted cognitive test scores. Scores were adjusted for age, because recipients of hormone-replacement therapy were younger than those who had never received such treatment. That adjustment decreased the likelihood of significant group differences in cognition by lowering the scores of the younger recipients of hormone-replacement therapy and raising the scores of the older women who had never received such therapy. In summary, despite no measurable advantage in health status, education, mood, or economic status, recipients of hormone-replacement therapy showed significantly better verbal learning and memory than the group that had never received such treatment.
Contrary to previous findings in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging of a small but reliable effect of hormone-replacement therapy on short-term figural memory (9), we found no significant beneficial effect of hormone-replacement therapy on Benton Visual Retention Test scores in this group. The effect size in the present study was 0.19, which is slightly lower than the 0.25 effect size observed in the previous study in a somewhat larger sample. Follow-up analysis suggests that the difference in effect sizes is attributable to differences in exclusionary criteria used in the two studies. The previous study excluded only women with a diagnosis of dementia at the time of the cognitive assessment because we were interested in the possible links between the effects of hormone-replacement therapy in normal and abnormal cognitive aging, including cognitive decline in preclinical dementia. Analysis of the present data by using the previous study criteria resulted in an effect size of 0.24, comparable to our previous finding. Hormone-replacement therapy may benefit verbal memory more than figural memory; however, the present data cannot unequivocally address that issue because the Benton Visual Retention Test and California Verbal Learning Test do not provide comparable measures of memory. Future studies involving parallel measures of verbal and figural memory are needed to directly address whether hormone-replacement therapy differentially benefits verbal or figural memory. We did not observe significant effects of hormone-replacement therapy on scores on the digit span tests or the Card Rotations Test. Although we had sufficient power to detect an effect size of 0.30, we lacked the power to detect smaller effects that may be associated with working memory or visuospatial ability. Nevertheless, the data indicate greater effects of hormone-replacement therapy on verbal memory than on the other cognitive domains tested.
Most studies of the effects of hormone-replacement therapy on memory have involved measures of immediate and delayed recall that are similar to the measures reported in our study. Such measures speak to whether hormone-replacement therapy affects memory, but they do not specify which aspects of memory are influenced by hormone-replacement therapy. The more comprehensive outcome measures of the California Verbal Learning Test suggest that hormone-replacement therapy enhances encoding, certain aspects of retention, and retrieval. Specifically, recipients of hormone-replacement therapy showed superior encoding of information, as indicated by better learning of lists A and B and better clustering of study items into categories. Recipients also showed better retention of items between trial 5 and the short-delay recall trial than did those who had never received such therapy; this is likely because the former were less susceptible to interference from the intervening list B task. There was no group difference in retention between the short- and long-delay free recall trials, suggesting no effect of hormone-replacement therapy on retention in the absence of a recent interference task. Finally, the memory performance of the group that had never received hormone-replacement therapy improved more from the long-delay free recall trial to the long-delay recognition trial than did that of recipients of hormone-replacement therapy (although recipients showed superior recognition). This indicates that those who had never received such treatment had not forgotten the items initially learned but rather that they had difficulty retrieving them. These findings have important implications for the interpretation of previous studies and the design of future investigations. For example, some studies include memory tests that involve additional rehearsal of unrecalled items before the delay trial (7, 8). Notably, these tasks may mask the beneficial effect of hormone-replacement therapy on verbal encoding and explain why the largest studies of the effects of estrogen on cognition found no beneficial effect on verbal recall.
Neurobiological studies in animals and humans suggest the biological plausibility of enhanced memory with hormone-replacement therapy (3), more so than with any other cognitive domain. Studies of the neurobiological effects of estrogen in the hippocampus and other brain regions subserving memory have primarily been conducted in ovariectomized rats given estradiol or in rats across the estrous cycle. Many studies focus on the effects of estrogen on cholinergic transmission. Although the density of estrogen receptors in the hippocampus and basal forebrain is much lower than that in the hypothalamus, changes in estrogen levels are associated with dramatic structural and functional changes in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. For example, administration of estradiol in ovariectomized rats leads to increases in choline acetyltransferase activity (26), prolongation of long-term potentiation (27), and recovery from ovariectomy-induced decreases in dendritic spine density (28). Such changes have been linked to changes in memory (2931). For example, estrogen reverses ovariectomy-induced memory deficits and decreases cholinergic uptake and choline acetyltransferase activity in the hippocampus and frontal cortex (32). Insights into functional brain changes associated with hormone-replacement therapy in humans have come from relatively recent neuroimaging studies showing changes related to hormone-replacement therapy in brain areas subserving cognitive functions (3336). All together, animal studies have suggested that estrogen may enhance memory by promoting survival and growth of neurons in the hippocampus, and human studies have indicated that hormone-replacement therapy modulates the activity of a number of brain regions that subserve memory processes.
One limitation of the present study was that we lacked sufficient statistical power to compare cognitive function in subgroups using specific types of hormone-replacement therapy (estrogen alone versus estrogen plus progesterone) to those who had never received hormone-replacement therapy. Such a comparison would increase our understanding of the relative effects of estrogen and combined therapy on cognitive aging. Our study also lacked sufficient statistical power to examine the effects of duration and type of hormone-replacement therapy on cognitive function among recipients of hormone-replacement therapy. In this small group of recipients (N=103), there was no correlation between duration of hormone-replacement therapy and any cognitive measure. Concurrent progesterone administration was associated with enhanced performance on a test of mental rotations but did not affect performance on measures of memory. It is unclear whether this was a chance finding or whether adjuvant progesterone therapy actually has a relatively large effect on visuospatial functioning. Studies of larger groups are needed to better address the effects of duration and adjuvant progesterone administration on cognitive function.
In summary, the present data suggest that hormone-replacement therapy may protect against normal age-related changes in memory by enhancing specific aspects of memory processing. Among observational studies, the present one is notable for involving groups of recipients of hormone-replacement therapy and those who had never received such therapy that were naturally matched on cognitive and health variables. Nevertheless, we cannot rule out the possibility that other unidentified confounding factors contributed to enhanced memory among recipients of hormone-replacement therapy. To address the limitations of observational studies while building on their findings, we recently initiated the Women’s Health Initiative Study of Cognitive Aging (N>2,000), a 6-year investigation of cognitive aging in a large subsample of women enrolled in the Randomized Hormone Replacement Trial of the Women’s Health Initiative. These and other ongoing intervention trials will provide critical insights into the effects of sex steroid hormones on the aging brain.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2

Footnote

Presented in part at the 29th annual meeting of the Institute of Medicine, Washington, D.C., Oct. 11, 1999; the third annual meeting of the Society for Behavioral Neuroendocrinology, Charlottesville, Va., June 26–30, 1999; and the 37th annual meeting of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Las Croabas, P.R., Dec. 14–18, 1998. Received Nov. 3, 1999; revision received Feb. 25, 2000; accepted Mar. 24, 2000. From the Gerontology Research Center, National Institute on Aging. Address reprint requests to Dr. Maki, Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, Gerontology Research Center, Box 3, NIA, 5600 Nathan Shock Dr., Baltimore, MD 21224; [email protected] (e-mail). The authors thank Drs. Jill B. Rich and Scott Moffat for comments on the manuscript, Dr. Claudia Kawas for determination of dementia status, Melissa Kitner-Triolo, Elizabeth Burke, and Carolyn Bacal for technical assistance; and participants in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.

References

1.
Henderson VW: The epidemiology of estrogen replacement therapy and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1997; 48(suppl 7):S27–S35
2.
Sherwin BB: Estrogen effects on cognition in menopausal women. Neurology 1997; 48(suppl 7):S21–S26
3.
McEwen BS, Alves SE, Bulloch K, Weiland NG: Ovarian steroids and the brain: implications for cognition and aging. Neurology 1997; 48(suppl 7):S8–S15
4.
Mulnard RA, Cotman CW, Kawas C, van Dyck CH, Sano M, Doody R, Koss E, Pfeiffer E, Jin S, Gamst A, Grundman M, Thomas R, Thal LJ: Estrogen replacement therapy for treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2000; 283:1007–1015
5.
Wang PN, Liao SQ, Liu RS, Liu CY, Chao HT, Lu SR, Yu HY, Wang SJ, Liu HC: Effects of estrogen on cognition, mood, and cerebral blood flow in AD: a controlled study. Neurology 2000; 54:2061–2066
6.
Henderson VW, Paganini-Hill A, Miller BL, Elble RJ, Reyes PF, Shoupe D, McCleary CA, Klein RA, Hake AM, Farlow MR: Estrogen for Alzheimer’s disease in women: randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Neurology 2000; 54:295–301
7.
Barrett-Connor E, Kritz-Silverstein D: Estrogen replacement therapy and cognitive function in older women. JAMA 1993; 269:2637–2641
8.
Szklo M, Cerhan J, Diez-Roux AV, Chambless L, Cooper L, Folsom AR, Fried LP, Knopman D, Nieto FJ: Estrogen replacement therapy and cognitive functioning in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Am J Epidemiol 1996; 144:1048–1057
9.
Resnick SM, Metter EJ, Zonderman AB: Estrogen replacement therapy and longitudinal decline in visual memory: a possible protective effect? Neurology 1997; 49:1491–1497
10.
Sherwin BB, Phillips S: Estrogen and cognitive functioning in surgically menopausal women. Ann NY Acad Sci 1990; 592:474–475
11.
Phillips SM, Sherwin BB: Effects of estrogen on memory function in surgically menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1992; 17:485–495
12.
Robinson D, Friedman L, Marcus R, Tinklenberg J, Yesavage J: Estrogen replacement therapy and memory in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc 1994; 42:919–922
13.
Kampen DL, Sherwin BB: Estrogen use and verbal memory in healthy postmenopausal women. Obstet Gynecol 1994; 83:979–983
14.
Jacobs DM, Tang MX, Stern Y, Sano M, Marder K, Bell KL, Schofield P, Dooneief G, Gurland B, Mayeux R: Cognitive function in nondemented older women who took estrogen after menopause. Neurology 1998; 50:368–373
15.
Matthews KA, Kuller LH, Wing RR, Meilahn EN, Plantinga P: Prior to use of estrogen replacement therapy, are users healthier than nonusers? Am J Epidemiol 1996; 143:971–978
16.
Egeland GM, Kuller LH, Matthews KA, Kelsey SF, Cauley J, Guzick D: Premenopausal determinants of menopausal estrogen use. Prevent Med 1991; 20:343–349
17.
Shock NW, Greulich RC, Andres R, Arenberg D, Costa PT Jr, Lakatta E, Tobin JD: Normal Human Aging: The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Washington, DC, US Government Printing Office, 1984
18.
DeFries JC, Vandenberg SG, McClearn GE, Kuse AR, Wilson JR, Ashton GC, Johnson RC: Near identity of cognitive structure in two ethnic groups. Science 1974; 183:338–339
19.
Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA: California Verbal Learning Test—Research Edition. New York, Psychological Corp, 1987
20.
Benton AL: The Revised Visual Retention Test: Clinical and Experimental Applications, 3rd ed. New York, Psychological Corp, 1963
21.
Wilson J, DeFries J, McLearn G, Vandenberg S, Johnson R, Rashad M: Cognitive abilities: use of family data as a control to assess sex and age differences in two ethnic groups. Int J Aging Hum Dev 1975; 6:261–276
22.
Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH: Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton, NJ, Educational Testing Service, 1976
23.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York, Psychological Corp, 1981
24.
Radloff LS: The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. J Applied Psychol Measurement 1977; 1:385–401
25.
Hays WL: Statistics for the Social Sciences. New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1973
26.
Luine VN: Estradiol increases choline acetyltransferase activity in specific basal forebrain nuclei and projection areas of female rats. Exp Neurol 1985; 89:484–490
27.
Wong M, Moss R: Long-term and short-term electrophysiological effects of estrogen on the synaptic properties of hippocampal CA1 neurons. J Neurosci 1992; 12:3217–3225
28.
Gould E, Woolley CS, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS: Gonadal steroids regulate dendritic spine density in hippocampal pyramidal cells in adulthood. J Neurosci 1990; 10:1286–1291
29.
Packard MG: Posttraining estrogen and memory modulation. Horm Behav 1998; 34:126–139
30.
Gibbs RB, Burke AM, Johnson DA: Estrogen replacement attenuates effects of scopolamine and lorazepam on memory acquisition and retention. Horm Behav 1998; 34:112–125
31.
Fader AJ, Hendricson AW, Dohanic DP: Estrogen improves performance of reinforced T-maze alternation and prevents amnestic effects of scopolamine administered systemically or intrahippocampally. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1998; 69:225–240
32.
Singh M, Meyer EM, Millard WJ, Simpkins JW: Ovarian steroid deprivation results in reversible learning impairment and compromised cholinergic function in female Sprague-Dawley rats. Brain Res 1994; 644:305–312
33.
Resnick SM, Maki PM, Golski S, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB: Estrogen effects on PET cerebral blood flow and neuropsychological performance. Horm Behav 1998; 34:171–184
34.
Berman KF, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR, Dananceau MA, Van Horn JD, Esposito G, Ostrem JL, Weinberger DR: Modulation of cognition-specific cortical activity by gonadal steroids: a positron-emission tomography study in women. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997; 94:8836–8841
35.
Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, Mencl WE, Constable RT, Naftolin F, Palter SF, Marchione KE, Katz L, Shankweiler DP, Fletcher JM, Lacadie C, Keltz M, Gore JC: Effect of estrogen on brain activation patterns in postmenopausal women during working memory tasks. JAMA 1999; 281:1197–1202
36.
Maki PM, Resnick SM: Longitudinal effects of estrogen replacement therapy on PET cerebral blood flow and cognition. Neurobiol Aging 2000; 21:373–383

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 227 - 233
PubMed: 11156805

History

Published online: 1 February 2001
Published in print: February 2001

Authors

Details

Pauline M. Maki, Ph.D.
Alan B. Zonderman, Ph.D.
Susan M. Resnick, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share