Skip to main content

Abstract

Longitudinal and family-based research suggests that conduct disorder, substance misuse, and ADHD involve both unique forms of dysfunction as well as more specific dysfunctions unique to each condition. Using direct measures of brain function, this study also found evidence in both unique and disorder-specific perturbations.

Abstract

Objective:

The authors sought to model the unique and common variance across conduct disorder, substance misuse, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and to investigate the neurocognitive factors that relate generally or uniquely to externalizing problems in adolescence.

Method:

Personality and behavioral measures and functional imaging responses to reward sensitivity and response inhibition tasks were assessed in 1,778 European adolescents at age 14 and, using structural equation modeling, were related to the unique and common variance across externalizing problems assessed and modeled at ages 14 and 16.

Results:

Externalizing problems best fit a general-specific model made up of a specific factor representing ADHD and conduct disorder symptoms, a specific factor representing substance misuse symptoms, and a common externalizing factor representing the variance shared among all symptoms. Common variance across externalizing problems was associated with high impulsivity and delay discounting as well as low blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response in the substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus but high BOLD response in the presupplementary motor area and precentral gyrus during successful inhibition. Unique variance for ADHD/conduct disorder was associated with impulsivity, poor response inhibition, and high delay discounting, as well as low BOLD response in frontal brain areas bilaterally during failed inhibition. In contrast, unique variance for substance misuse was associated with high sensation seeking and delay discounting, as well as differential brain response to reward anticipation: high BOLD response in the left orbitofrontal cortex but low BOLD response in the left inferior frontal gyrus.

Conclusions:

Personality, behavioral, and fMRI findings suggest that abnormalities in response inhibition, error processing, and reward processing are differentially implicated in underlying vulnerability specific to ADHD/conduct disorder and substance misuse and general to externalizing problems.
High rates of comorbidity between adult substance use and antisocial personality disorders on the one hand and childhood conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and substance misuse on the other suggest a shared etiology across such externalizing problems. These and other common comorbidities have inspired a trend in psychiatry to reformulate diagnostic categories from a dimensional and neuroscience perspective (e.g., the Research Domain Criteria [1]).
Accordingly, quantitative modeling of variance across externalizing problems (2, 3) suggests that much of the variance is common or shared and dimensional in nature (4). This research consistently supports a general-specific model of externalizing problems, with a latent general factor representing variance common to all externalizing symptoms and two specific factors representing variance that is unique to some conduct disorder symptoms (e.g., vandalism in adolescents) and substance misuse (e.g., binge drinking and frequency of drug use in adolescents). However, the role of ADHD, despite a high rate of comorbidity with conduct disorder and substance use disorders (5), was not investigated in these previous studies assessing the general-specific structure of externalizing problems.
Furthermore, while it is generally agreed that there is overlap in brain processes linked to substance misuse, conduct disorder, and ADHD, few studies have attempted to combine multivariate models of behavior with multivariate models of cognitive or brain function. There are several reasons for this, including small sample sizes and the fact that neuroimaging studies tend to work with a relatively simplistic phenotypic characterization (e.g., presence or absence of a particular diagnosis) and rarely account for other forms of comorbidity. In the investigation of the neurobiological correlates of externalizing problems, it is important that we accurately characterize the shared and the unique nature of these symptoms and investigate this sophisticated phenotypic characterization with state-of-the-art neuroimaging probes designed to engage the cortical and subcortical brain regions implicated in relevant brain functions. Doing so may reveal neurobiological profiles that explain underlying latent dimensions reflecting vulnerability to unique or broad psychiatric outcomes.
For example, while impulsivity has been identified as a risk factor for a variety of externalizing problems (6), it is known not to be a unitary construct (7, 8) but one that can be subdivided into at least two subfactors, one reflecting impulsive action (related to deficits in response inhibition) and another reflecting impulsive choice or decision making (often measured using a delay discounting task) (9). However, there is some debate as to whether impulsive choice involves two additional dissociable processes: 1) reward processing and 2) temporal perception or decision making (9). Deficits in impulsive action (poor response inhibition) have been reported in various externalizing problems, including ADHD (10), conduct disorder (11), and substance abuse (12), and are measured behaviorally using go/no-go and stop-signal tasks. Studies have shown that aggressive adolescent males and children with ADHD and conduct disorder make more commission errors in these tasks compared with control subjects (13, 14). The few studies that have investigated the neuropsychological correlates of externalizing problems from a dimensional perspective showed that self-reported impulsivity and poor response inhibition on a stop-signal task (impulsive action) prospectively predicted variance common to externalizing problems and specific to conduct disorder, whereas sensation seeking and reward sensitivity predicted variance specific to substance misuse (3, 13).
Functional neuroimaging studies involving response inhibition tasks implicate a number of brain regions involved in successfully inhibiting behavior, including frontal regions; frontal-striatal regions; left and right putamen; caudate; globus pallidus and thalamus; inferior frontal gyrus; insula and anterior cingulate; substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus; orbitofrontal and medial gyri; presupplementary motor area; and a parietal network (15). Adolescents with ADHD show reduced responses in frontal and striatal regions of the brain when performing go/no-go and stop-signal tasks (5, 14). A study that controlled for comorbid cases (14, 16) found that abnormal blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response patterns specific to ADHD are seen in the prefrontal cortex and abnormalities common to ADHD and conduct disorder are seen in regions such as the insula and cingulate on trials of failed inhibition. By contrast, delay discounting (and the simple task of thinking about the future) involves the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum (17). Functional MRI (fMRI) studies investigating individual differences in brain response to reward magnitude (such as the monetary incentive delay task) also implicate the ventral striatum, but more recent studies using newer methodologies, such as functional connectivity, also highlight the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and its connectivity with the ventral striatum in reward processing (e.g., 18).
While few neuroimaging studies of substance-misusing individuals have controlled for comorbid personality or attentional problems, there is emerging evidence that individuals specifically prone to substance misuse can be distinguished from other clinically disinhibited groups based on impulsive choice and reward sensitivity, rather than general deficits in response inhibition (impulsive action). For example, high-functioning drug users (19) and adolescents with pure substance misuse profiles (13) show impulsivity and behavioral activation only when anticipating reward, but are otherwise quite controlled. While several studies suggest a relationship between substance misuse generally and poor delay discounting (e.g., 20), to our knowledge no study to date has investigated how these processes are linked to specific sets of symptoms within complex models of psychopathology that include latent constructs of shared and specific vulnerability.
In this study, using a large sample of adolescents assessed longitudinally through the IMAGEN study (21), we sought to determine whether the pattern of shared and unique variance across externalizing problems is best represented by a general-specific hierarchical structure (2, 3), whether that structure has predictive validity, and whether these latent factors can be dissociated from a neurocognitive endophenotype perspective (6). Similarly, based on previous theoretical and empirical research demonstrating that impulsivity is a complex construct represented by at least three cognitive processes, we hypothesized that impulsive action, impulsive choice, and reward processing would dissociate the core dimensions of externalizing problems, with impulsive action (as measured by response inhibition and the brain areas most implicated in stopping behavior, particularly frontal networks [15]) being associated with the variance specific to ADHD and/or conduct disorder symptoms (that does not covary with substance use symptoms), and sensation seeking and reward processing (implicating the ventral striatum and the orbitofrontal cortex) being most associated with the variance specific to substance misuse. Impulsive choice and related brain functions were hypothesized to be implicated in variance common to all externalizing problems.

Method

Participants

A total of 2,232 participants across eight European sites were recruited through high schools. Parents gave written informed consent and adolescents gave written assent to the study procedures before enrollment. All procedures were approved by each local institutional ethics committee. Further details on the study design, sample, and recruitment procedure, as well as data storage and safety, may be found elsewhere (21; see also the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article).
After data quality control, complete and reliable data sets were available for 1,778 participants with a mean age of 14.4 years (SD=0.35) and a balanced gender ratio (51% of them girls [N=912]). Follow-up data on externalizing symptoms were gathered at age 16 for 1,210 (68%) of these participants and were used to test the stability of externalizing symptoms and their structure across adolescence. Participants who attended the assessments at age 16 did not differ significantly from those who did not in demographic, behavioral, or cognitive variables, except in language (English, German, and French; English speakers were more likely to participate in the follow-up: odds ratio=3.44, 95% CI=2.55–4.64) and self-reported ADHD symptoms (those scoring higher on ADHD symptoms were less likely to participate: odds ratio=0.91, 95% CI=0.85–0.98).
Of the 1,778 adolescents, 4.4% (N=78) were identified as having a diagnosis of conduct disorder (N=37), ADHD (N=30), or both (N=11) according to the Development and Well-Being Assessment interview (22); 3.7% (N=65) reported problematic alcohol use, and 10.6% (N=189) reported drug use. At age 16, 6.3% (N=66/1210) were identified as having a diagnosis of conduct disorder (N=25), ADHD (N=31), or both (N=10); 18.0% (N=218) reported problematic alcohol use, and 27.1% (N=328) reported drug use.

Measures

All measures were selected on the basis of brevity, age-appropriateness, and validity in their variant forms (English, German, and French). They are described briefly below; for a more detailed description, see the online data supplement.

Externalizing problems.

Self-report and parent-report behavioral and clinical measures were assessed through an online computer platform provided by Psytools (Delosis Research Technology, London) and administered at participants’ homes, as well as the Development and Well-Being Assessment interview (22; www.dawba.com) and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (23), administered at the research site. Substance misuse was assessed using the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (24) and the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Drugs (25). Only externalizing symptoms were assessed at both ages 14 and 16.
Personality traits were assessed with the impulsivity and sensation-seeking subscales of the self-reported Substance Use Risk Profile Scale (26). (For a discussion of the dissociation between impulsivity and sensation seeking and their psychometric properties, see references 3 and 13.)

IQ and behavioral measures.

To control for the general effects of intelligence on behavior and cognitive performance, estimates of intelligence were derived from the vocabulary and similarities subtests (verbal IQ) and block design and matrix reasoning subtests (performance IQ) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th Edition (27). Response inhibition was measured as number of commission errors in a go/no-go passive avoidance learning paradigm (28). In this task, participants are asked to learn, by trial and error, to respond to “correct” numbers and to withhold a response for “wrong” numbers by rewarding correct or punishing wrong go and no-go responses. Commission errors were used as an index of response inhibition. Delay discounting was assessed with the Kirby Delay Discounting Questionnaire (20), with larger k values indicating greater delay discounting of value for delayed options.

Neuroimaging tasks.

Two MRI sessions were conducted, lasting 45 minutes each, each including a combination of structural and functional scans. Before each session, participants familiarized themselves with the scanner and tasks in a practice session. (Further information about the imaging procedure, with links to task specifications, is available at http://www.imagen-europe.com/en/Publications_and_SOP.php; see also reference 21.)
The stop-signal task (14) was used to measure brain response during successful and failed inhibition. Participants responded to regularly presented visual go stimuli (arrows pointing left or right) and were instructed to withhold their response when the go stimulus was followed (unpredictably) by a stop signal (arrow pointing upward). Stopping difficulty was manipulated across trials by varying the delay between the onset of the go arrow and the stop arrow (stop-signal delay) using a previously described tracking algorithm (14). There were two contrasts of interest, in which go trials were used as “implicit baseline”: successful inhibition–go trial, and failed inhibition (commission error)–go trial. A comprehensive report on this task by Whelan et al. (15) identified, through factor analysis, seven brain networks involved in stop success in this sample of adolescents (basal ganglia; parietal cortex; orbitofrontal cortex; medial orbitofrontal cortex; substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus; right frontal cortex; and presupplementary motor area) and six networks involved in stop failure (left and right frontal cortex; parietal cortex; posterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex; orbitofrontal cortex; substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus; and basal ganglia). Factor scores from that analysis were used in the present study.
A modified version of the monetary incentive delay task (29) was used to assess brain response to reward anticipation. In the anticipation phase of this task, participants were presented with cues (which varied between 4 and 4.5 seconds) signaling the amount of reward that could be won in a given trial (large reward, small reward, or no reward). We focused on three regions of interest, in which mean activity levels for each contrast were extracted during the reward anticipation phase involving the contrast of large reward anticipation versus no reward anticipation. The regions of interest selected were the left and right ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, and inferior frontal gyrus. As BOLD responses in the left and right ventral striatum were highly correlated, they were averaged to create a bilateral ventral striatum activation score.

Data Analysis

A series of a priori structural equation models on substance misuse, ADHD, and conduct disorder symptom scores (incorporating symptom severity and impact) were analyzed using MPlus, version 6.12 (www.statmodel.com). Based on previously reported analyses (2, 3), several models were assessed for goodness of fit.
1.
A single “externalizing problems” factor loading on all symptom scores.
2.
Two hierarchical models, in which variables assessing conduct disorder symptoms (scaled likelihood of diagnosis, parent- and self-reported symptoms, self-reported bullying behavior), ADHD symptoms (scaled likelihood of diagnosis, parent- and self-reported symptoms), and substance misuse variables (age at onset of drinking, number of drugs used, frequency of drunkenness, frequency of bingeing, the interaction of quantity and frequency of drinking, and drinking-related problems) loading onto specific conduct disorder, ADHD, and substance misuse subfactors, respectively, which then load onto a higher-order “externalizing problems” factor (model 2a) and a modified version of this model where conduct disorder and ADHD symptoms load onto the same specific ADHD/conduct disorder factor (model 2b).
3.
Two general-specific models, in which a general externalizing factor was added at the same level as the specific conduct disorder, ADHD, and substance misuse factors from the previous (2a) model (model 3a), or the specific ADHD/conduct disorder and substance misuse factors from the previous (2b) model (model 3b). In these last general-specific models, factors were not allowed to covary (see Figure 1)
FIGURE 1. General-Specific Model of Unique and Common Variance Across Externalizing Problems in Young Adolescencea
a ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Likelihood of ADHD and conduct disorder diagnoses refers to scaled likelihood of DSM-IV diagnosis according to band scores from the Development and Well-Being Assessment (22).
All models were fitted using a complex random-effects design to control for site as a cluster variable and using robust maximum-likelihood estimation. Unlike other studies on this topic, we incorporated variance in behavior across the entire population, not simply severity of symptoms in participants with diagnoses, which allowed us to investigate the extent to which continuous models of liability also represent variance with normal adolescent populations.
Once the best-fitting model was established, four sets of covariates (personality, cognitive indices, and brain response scores during the stop task and during the monetary incentive delay tasks) were each entered into the model separately.
Finally, stability of externalizing problems across adolescence was tested by fitting the model using follow-up data at age 16 (N=1,210) and assessing correlations between factor scores across time. All analyses controlled for gender, verbal and performance IQ, and language. The Benjamini-Hochberg procedure was used to correct for multiple testing in all analyses. See the online data supplement for further discussion of the analytic approach.

Results

Externalizing Problems Model Fit

Of the models tested, the general-specific model with the combined ADHD/conduct disorder and substance misuse specific factors (model 3b) fit the data best (Table 1). Factor loadings for this model are presented in Table 2. The substance misuse factor reflected mostly variance unique to drinking. The ADHD/conduct disorder factor captured variance unique to ADHD with some loading from parent-reported conduct disorder symptoms. With the exception of parent-reported ADHD symptoms, all substance misuse, ADHD, and conduct disorder variables loaded significantly on the general externalizing factor, with conduct disorder variables loading the strongest on this factor.
TABLE 1. Fit Indices for Successive Structural Equation Models of Externalizing Problems in Young Adolescencea
Modelχ2dfSCFCFIRMSEASRMRAICBICΔχ2Δdfp
Models without covariates           
Model 1: one factor1952.20651.390.590.130.1175489758461158.513<0.001
Model 2a: hierarchical, three subfactors389.57621.530.920.060.04733727374551.710<0.001
Model 2b: hierarchical, two subfactors567.57641.550.880.070.067365874020157.412<0.001
Model 3a: general-specific (three specific)336.88521.550.940.060.047331773745   
Model 3b: general-specific (two specific)341.25521.250.940.050.037322173648   
Models with covariates           
Model 3b and IQ covariatesb374.3175 0.940.050.03     
Model 3b and personality covariatesb487.7297 0.930.050.04     
Model 3b and behavioral covariates (go/no-go and delay discounting task data)b418.0197 0.940.040.03     
Model 3b and stop-signal task datab679.91232 0.920.030.03     
Model 3b and monetary incentive delay task data (reward anticipation)b554.08136 0.920.040.03     
a
Tests of goodness of fit include the chi-square and comparative fit indices (CFI), the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Hu and Bentler (30) suggest that acceptable model fit is indicated by SRMR values close to or below 0.08, RMSEA values close to or below 0.06, and CFI values close to or above 0.90. Models were compared using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), frequently used to compare nonnested models. Smaller values on both these measures indicate a better-fitting model while penalizing for increasing model complexity. Nested models were compared with the Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square difference test for maximum likelihood parameter estimation using scaled correction factors (SCF): hierarchical subfactor models were compared with their corresponding general-specific model—that is, model 2b was compared with model 3b, while model 2a was compared with model 3a; the one-factor model was compared with both models 3a and 3b with similar results, but only the chi-square difference tests for the comparison with model 3b are presented. All chi-square difference tests indicated that the one-factor model and the hierarchical subfactors models fit significantly worse than the general-specific models. The general-specific model (model 3b, two specific factors) was deemed to represent the data best not only because it was the best-fitting model but also because ADHD and conduct disorder symptoms were strongly correlated (r=0.71), and once the common variance between ADHD, conduct disorder, and substance misuse was accounted for, variance for the specific conduct disorder factor did not reach significance.
b
Models included gender, language, and IQ as covariates.
TABLE 2. Factor Loadings for General-Specific Model 3b at Age 14 (N=1,778)a
 Factor
 ADHD/Conduct DisorderSubstance MisuseGeneral Externalizing Behavior
PredictorLoadploadpLoadp
Likelihood of conduct disorder diagnosis0.27<0.001  0.55<0.001
Conduct disorder symptoms, self-reported0.140.303  0.68<0.001
Conduct disorder symptoms, parent-reported0.46<0.001  0.41<0.001
Likelihood of ADHD diagnosis0.70<0.001  0.190.039
ADHD symptoms, self-reported0.39<0.001  0.350.016
ADHD symptoms, parent-reported0.79<0.001  0.210.191
Bullying behavior, self-reported0.080.002  0.26<0.001
Age at drinking onset  0.36<0.0010.33<0.001
Number of drugs used  0.30<0.0010.30<0.001
Drunkenness frequency  0.70<0.0010.32<0.001
Bingeing frequency  0.77<0.0010.33<0.001
Drinking quantity by frequency  0.73<0.0010.36<0.001
Problem drinking symptoms  0.45<0.0010.31<0.001
a
ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; load=estimated standardized factor loadings. Likelihood of conduct disorder and ADHD diagnoses refers to scaled likelihood of DSM-IV diagnosis according to band scores from the Development and Well-Being Assessment (22).

Personality, Cognitive, and Neural Covariates

All predictor models with covariates showed good model fit (Table 1). Table 3 presents regression paths between covariates and the general-specific factors of externalizing problems from model 3b, showing that lower verbal and performance IQ were significantly related to the ADHD/conduct disorder factor only. Impulsivity was significantly associated with the ADHD/conduct disorder and general externalizing factors, but not substance misuse, while sensation seeking was significantly associated with the substance misuse factor only. Commission errors on the go/no-go task were associated with the ADHD/conduct disorder factor only, while steeper delay discounting was associated with all factors. In terms of regional brain response variables, the ADHD/conduct disorder factor was associated with lower BOLD response in frontal areas bilaterally during failed inhibition, the substance misuse factor was not significantly associated with BOLD response during this task, and the general externalizing factor was associated with lower BOLD response during successful inhibition in the substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus, and in the presupplementary motor area and precentral gyrus. Only the substance misuse factor was associated with differential BOLD response during reward anticipation on the monetary incentive delay task: higher BOLD response in the left orbitofrontal cortex and lower BOLD response in the left inferior frontal gyrus.
TABLE 3. Standardized Parameter Estimates for Concurrent Associations Between Covariates and ADHD/Conduct Disorder, Substance Misuse, and General Externalizing Behavior Factors as Established in General-Specific Model 3b at Age 14 (N=1,778)a
 Factor
 ADHD/Conduct DisorderSubstance MisuseGeneral Externalizing Behavior
PredictorEstimatepEstimatepEstimatep
IQ      
 Verbal–0.17<0.001–0.020.3760.030.756
 Performance–0.16<0.001–0.100.023–0.040.345
Personality measures      
 Impulsivity0.27<0.001–0.030.5490.53<0.001
 Sensation seeking0.010.7460.110.0090.060.241
Behavioral measures      
 Delay discounting task (k values)0.06<0.0010.070.0060.110.001
 Go/no-go task (commission errors)0.090.0070.020.4100.040.230
fMRI region-of-interest factors for stop-signal task success and failure      
 Stop success      
  Basal ganglia–0.020.826–0.060.132–0.080.100
  Parietal cortex–0.010.7830.000.9360.010.849
  Orbitofrontal cortex0.030.3940.010.767–0.010.787
  Medial orbitofrontal cortex–0.020.4670.020.7000.050.038
  Substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus0.080.0430.070.062–0.12<0.001
  Right frontal cortexb0.040.2730.020.7090.050.260
  Presupplementary motor area, precentral gyrusb–0.010.8390.060.0200.140.001
 Stop failure      
  Basal ganglia0.090.1210.010.8920.060.475
  Orbitofrontal cortex–0.010.804–0.010.8060.050.098
  Precentral cortex, medial orbitofrontal cortex0.040.398–0.050.1800.030.317
  Parietal cortexb–0.020.5610.030.3480.020.489
  Left and right frontal cortexb–0.090.0030.010.861–0.050.446
  Substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus–0.010.8810.050.345–0.010.412
fMRI regions of interest for reward anticipation (monetary incentive delay task)      
 Left and right ventral striatum–0.090.017–0.020.4290.000.945
 Right orbitofrontal cortex0.020.5950.020.573–0.010.844
 Left orbitofrontal cortex–0.020.6530.050.0100.000.987
 Right inferior frontal gyrus0.040.4170.010.7700.040.293
 Left inferior frontal gyrus–0.030.534–0.09<0.0010.040.305
a
ADHD=attention and hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Values in boldface are those that remained significant after controlling for multiple testing using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure; these associations were also significant when using bootstrapping. All models include gender, language, and IQ as covariates and are clustered within study site. When all covariates (personality, behavioral, and imaging) were entered simultaneously into the same model, the significant effects indicated in boldface remained significant (and of the same size), with the exception of the effect of delay discounting on the ADHD/conduct disorder factor (β=0.05, p=0.067) and the general externalizing factor (β=0.06, p=0.152) and the effect of commission errors on the ADHD/conduct disorder factor (β=0.06, p=0.061). All covariates explained a total of 19%, 5%, and 34% of the variance in the ADHD/conduct disorder, substance misuse, and general externalizing factors, respectively, with behavioral and imaging measures explaining 4%, 3%, and 5% of the variance, respectively (based on R2 values once the effects of personality and IQ where accounted for).
b
As loadings for these factors were negative (see Whelan et al. [15]), the direction of association has been inversed so that positive associations indicate higher BOLD response.

Predicting Externalizing Problems at Age 16

Externalizing problems data at age 16 best fit a general-specific model as well, represented by ADHD/conduct disorder-specific, substance misuse-specific, and common externalizing factors, which were correlated strongly with corresponding factors at age 14 (see Table 4 for fit indices, factor loadings, and 2-year test-retest correlations between factors). When covariates at age 14, including externalizing problem factor scores, were added to the model at age 16, impulsivity at age 14 predicted the general externalizing factor at age 16 (b=0.15, p<0.05), sensation-seeking at age 14 predicted substance misuse at age 16 (b=0.07, p<0.01), and go/no-go commission errors as well as lower BOLD response in frontal areas bilaterally during failed inhibition at age 14 predicted ADHD/conduct disorder at age 16 (for both, b=0.09, p<0.001). No other significant associations were observed between covariates (cognitive or brain imaging) at age 14 and externalizing problem factors at age 16.
TABLE 4. Factor Loadings for General-Specific Model 3b at Age 16 (N=1,210)a
 Factor
PredictorADHD/Conduct Disorder at Age 16Substance Misuse at Age 16General Externalizing Behavior at Age 16
 LoadpLoadpLoadp
Likelihood of conduct disorder diagnosis0.190.316  0.39<0.001
Conduct disorder symptoms, self-reported–0.150.647  0.82<0.001
Conduct disorder symptoms, parent-reported0.350.001  0.48<0.001
Likelihood of ADHD diagnosis0.44<0.001  0.350.007
ADHD symptoms, self-reported0.090.446  0.52<0.001
ADHD symptoms, parent-reported0.64<0.001  0.490.004
Bullying behavior, self-reported0.030.393  0.18<0.001
Age at drinking onset  0.36<0.0010.23<0.001
Drug use frequency  0.51<0.0010.23<0.001
Drunkenness frequency  0.85<0.0010.22<0.001
Bingeing frequency  0.83<0.0010.26<0.001
Drinking quantity by frequency  0.84<0.0010.24<0.001
Problem drinking symptoms  0.56<0.0010.16<0.001
 rprprp
Correlations between factors      
 ADHD/conduct disorder at age 140.56<0.001–0.010.8000.210.004
 Substance misuse at age 140.000.8140.45<0.001–0.030.392
 General externalizing behavior at age 14–0.090.178–0.070.0620.61<0.001
a
ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; load=estimated standardized factor loadings. Likelihood of conduct disorder and ADHD diagnoses refers to scaled likelihood of DSM-IV diagnosis according to band scores from the Development and Well-Being Assessment (22). Model fit of general-specific model at age 16: χ2=170.11, df=52, 1210, p<0.001; comparative fit index=0.96; root mean square error of approximation=0.04; standardized root mean square residual=0.03.

Discussion

This study provides further support for the latent trait model of externalizing problems previously proposed by Krueger et al. (2) and validated in an adolescent sample by Castellanos-Ryan and Conrod (3), and it extends the model to include early-adolescent ADHD symptoms (as also recently demonstrated by Callagher et al. in adults [4]) and to demonstrate developmental stability. When such problems are modeled hierarchically, personality, cognitive, and neural measures of disinhibition dissociate in their relationship to the latent variables generated by the model, extending the validity and utility of the dimensional-spectrum conceptualization of externalizing problems. The findings suggest that while risk-taking behavior is observed in normal adolescents, personality and neurocognitive abnormalities appear to be associated with the tendency to commit these behaviors across multiple behavioral indicators, contexts, and time points, as reflected by the latent dimensions and their stability across time. At least three neurocognitive profiles were shown to dissociate dimensions of externalizing problems: self-reported sensation seeking (and reward sensitivity), impulsive action tendencies (poor response inhibition), and impulsive choice (delay discounting). Understanding a young person’s dominant learning and motivational profile from this perspective might help in individualizing treatments and have an impact on a variety of behavioral outcomes.
The latent factor previously identified by Castellanos-Ryan and Conrod (3) as representing variance unique to conduct disorder was shown in this study to also include ADHD symptoms, and it was correlated with self-reported impulsivity scores, lower verbal and performance IQ, poor response inhibition, higher delay discounting, and a weaker BOLD response in the frontal cortex bilaterally (including the anterior cingulate, rostral caudate, and inferior frontal gyrus) during failed inhibition in a stop task. These findings are all consistent with a profile of poor frontal executive control involving impairments in both impulsive action and impulsive choice. Our findings are similar to those reported by Rubia et al. (31) showing that adolescents with a pure ADHD symptom profile had low BOLD response in prefrontal cortical brain regions during failed inhibition but not during successful inhibition. While a number of studies have also reported reduced right inferior frontal cortex activation in ADHD patients during successful stop trials (32), these previous findings may reflect co-occurrence of general externalizing tendencies, rather than the variance specific to ADHD/conduct disorder. This clinical profile appears best characterized by deficits in motor impulsivity regulated by prefrontal brain circuits, rather than reward sensitivity, and related subcortical brain circuits.
The general externalizing factor, which accounted for much of the variance in externalizing problems, was also strongly associated with self-reported impulsivity but was dissociated from the other externalizing dimensions on cognitive and neuroimaging measures of poor impulsive choice, low BOLD response in the substantia nigra and subthalamic nucleus, and high BOLD response in the presupplementary motor area/precentral gyrus during successful inhibition. These brain regions have been implicated in inhibitory control (33), with some findings suggesting that both networks have a more specific role in the motivation of action and the capacity to slow down to evaluate conflicting choices (34). The presupplementary motor area is thought to be associated with motivation for movement initiation by linking expected reward with specific actions, rather than controlling whether an action should be made (35). Findings from the present study add to previous findings by showing that high BOLD response in the presupplementary motor area/precentral gyrus may be associated with vulnerability to externalizing problems in general, rather than substance use specifically (15).
The substance misuse-specific factor, reflecting mostly early onset and frequency and quantity of drinking, was associated with self-reported sensation seeking, high delay discounting, and high left orbitofrontal response and low left inferior frontal gyrus response when anticipating reward. These findings of a double dissociation in the neurocognitive profiles of ADHD/conduct disorder and substance misuse are consistent with previous reports (13) showing that sensation seeking and individual differences in reward response (previously measured using behavioral tasks) specifically predict vulnerability to binge drinking in adolescence. The orbitofrontal cortex has consistently been identified as a key part of the reward-processing network (36), with high BOLD response in this brain area being associated with heightened attention to reward (37). The present findings are consistent with reports of frontolimbic (including the orbitofrontal cortex and inferior frontal gyrus) reward-processing deficits involved in alcohol problems (38) and further suggest a reward-related deficit in adolescents prone to early-onset frequent alcohol misuse, independent of other externalizing problems.
This study also demonstrates that the latent dimensions of externalizing problems are moderately stable and that personality, cognitive, and neural correlates of such dimensions also have longitudinal predictive validity. Considering the age of our participants at baseline (age 14) and their relatively minor substance use histories, these findings likely reflect underlying etiologic mechanisms rather than the long-term effects of substance use and suggest that prevention efforts directed at reducing underlying motivational and trait dimensions of risk (39, 40) could benefit from incorporating training components that target these brain functions (41). Furthermore, in line with recent proposals to shift our research strategy in psychopathology toward the study of cognitive and neural endophenotypes (1), our findings suggest that the dissociation of impulsive action, impulsive choice, and reward sensitivity is relevant to understanding the common and shared variance across externalizing problems. Furthermore, these findings suggest that new intervention strategies targeting these endophenotypes of risk, either at the personality, cognitive, or neural level, have the potential to affect a number of clinical outcomes concurrently. The fact that they can be measured before the emergence of psychiatric symptoms suggests that they might also be good candidates for novel prevention strategies in mental health, such as has been demonstrated by the personality-targeted approach to drug and alcohol prevention (40), which recently was also shown to concurrently reduce emotional and behavioral problems in youths (39).

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2014.13111499.ds001.pdf)
File (appi.ajp.2014.13111499_ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Cuthbert BN, Insel TR: Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med 2013; 11:126
2.
Krueger RF, Markon KE, Patrick CJ, Benning SD, Kramer MD: Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: an integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. J Abnorm Psychol 2007; 116:645–666
3.
Castellanos-Ryan N, Conrod PJ: Personality correlates of the common and unique variance across conduct disorder and substance misuse symptoms in adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2011; 39:563–576
4.
Callagher N, Krueger RF, Eaton NR, Markon KE, Keyes KM, Blanco C, Saha TD, Hasin DS: ADHD and the externalizing spectrum: direct comparison of categorical, continuous, and hybrid models of liability in a nationally representative sample. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol (Epub ahead of print, Oct 1, 2013)
5.
Banaschewski T, Hollis C, Oosterlaan J, Roeyers H, Rubia K, Willcutt E, Taylor E: Towards an understanding of unique and shared pathways in the psychopathophysiology of ADHD. Dev Sci 2005; 8:132–140
6.
Robbins TW, Gillan CM, Smith DG, de Wit S, Ersche KD: Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16:81–91
7.
Evenden JL: Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999; 146:348–361
8.
Reynolds B, Ortengren A, Richards JB, de Wit H: Dimensions of impulsive behavior: personality and behavioral measures. Pers Individ Dif 2006; 40:305–315
9.
Garcia A, Kirkpatrick K: Impulsive choice behavior in four strains of rats: evaluation of possible models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Res 2013; 238:10–22
10.
Lijffijt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H: A meta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory motor control? J Abnorm Psychol 2005; 114:216–222
11.
Hobson CW, Scott S, Rubia K: Investigation of cool and hot executive function in ODD/CD independently of ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52:1035–1043
12.
Iacono WG, Malone SM, McGue M: Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early-onset addiction: common and specific influences. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2008; 4:325–348
13.
Castellanos-Ryan N, Rubia K, Conrod PJ: Response inhibition and reward response bias mediate the predictive relationships between impulsivity and sensation seeking and common and unique variance in conduct disorder and substance misuse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2011; 35:140–155
14.
Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Toone B, Taylor E: Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1067–1075
15.
Whelan R, Conrod PJ, Poline JB, Lourdusamy A, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Bellgrove MA, Büchel C, Byrne M, Cummins TD, Fauth-Bühler M, Flor H, Gallinat J, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Mann K, Martinot JL, Lalor EC, Lathrop M, Loth E, Nees F, Paus T, Rietschel M, Smolka MN, Spanagel R, Stephens DN, Struve M, Thyreau B, Vollstaedt-Klein S, Robbins TW, Schumann G, Garavan H; IMAGEN Consortium: Adolescent impulsivity phenotypes characterized by distinct brain networks. Nat Neurosci 2012; 15:920–925
16.
Rubia K, Smith AB, Halari R, Matsukura F, Mohammad M, Taylor E, Brammer MJ: Disorder-specific dissociation of orbitofrontal dysfunction in boys with pure conduct disorder during reward and ventrolateral prefrontal dysfunction in boys with pure ADHD during sustained attention. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:83–94
17.
Cooper N, Kable JW, Kim BK, Zauberman G: Brain activity in valuation regions while thinking about the future predicts individual discount rates. J Neurosci 2013; 33:13150–13156
18.
Costumero V, Barrós-Loscertales A, Bustamante JC, Ventura-Campos N, Fuentes P, Ávila C: Reward sensitivity modulates connectivity among reward brain areas during processing of anticipatory reward cues. Eur J Neurosci 2013; 38:2399–2407
19.
Yechiam E, Stout JC, Busemeyer JR, Rock SL, Finn PR: Individual differences in the response to forgone payoffs: an examination of high functioning drug abusers. J Behav Decis Mak 2005; 18:97–110
20.
Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK: Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen 1999; 128:78–87
21.
Schumann G, Loth E, Banaschewski T, Barbot A, Barker G, Büchel C, Conrod PJ, Dalley JW, Flor H, Gallinat J, Garavan H, Heinz A, Itterman B, Lathrop M, Mallik C, Mann K, Martinot JL, Paus T, Poline JB, Robbins TW, Rietschel M, Reed L, Smolka M, Spanagel R, Speiser C, Stephens DN, Ströhle A, Struve M; IMAGEN consortium: The IMAGEN study: reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology. Mol Psychiatry 2010; 15:1128–1139
22.
Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, Gatward R, Meltzer H: The Development and Well-Being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000; 41:645–655
23.
Goodman R, Renfrew D, Mullick M: Predicting type of psychiatric disorder from Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) scores in child mental health clinics in London and Dhaka. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 9:129–134
24.
Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M: Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons With Harmful Alcohol Consumption–II. Addiction 1993; 88:791–804
25.
Hibell B, Guttormsson U, Ahlstrom S, Balakireva O, Bjarnason T, Kokkevi A, Kraus L: The 2007 ESPAD Report: Substance Use Among Students in 35 European Countries. Stockholm, Swedish Council for Information on Alcohol and Other Drugs, 2009
26.
Woicik PA, Stewart SH, Pihl RO, Conrod PJ: The Substance Use Risk Profile Scale: a scale measuring traits linked to reinforcement-specific substance use profiles. Addict Behav 2009; 34:1042–1055
27.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th ed. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 2003
28.
Newman J, Wallace J: Diverse pathways to deficient self-regulation: implications for disinhibitory psychopathology in children. Clin Psychol Rev 1993; 13:699–720
29.
Nees F, Vollstädt-Klein S, Fauth-Bühler M, Steiner S, Mann K, Poustka L, Banaschewski T, Büchel C, Conrod PJ, Garavan H, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Artiges E, Paus T, Pausova Z, Rietschel M, Smolka MN, Struve M, Loth E, Schumann G, Flor H; IMAGEN Consortium: A target sample of adolescents and reward processing: same neural and behavioral correlates engaged in common paradigms? Exp Brain Res 2012; 223:429–439
30.
Hu L, Bentler PM: Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling 1999; 6:1–55
31.
Rubia K, Halari R, Mohammad AM, Taylor E, Brammer M: Methylphenidate normalizes frontocingulate underactivation during error processing in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70:255–262
32.
Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K: Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:185–198
33.
Obeso I, Robles N, Marrón EM, Redolar-Ripoll D: Dissociating the role of the pre-SMA in response inhibition and switching: a combined online and offline TMS approach. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:150
34.
Frank MJ, Samanta J, Moustafa AA, Sherman SJ: Hold your horses: impulsivity, deep brain stimulation, and medication in parkinsonism. Science 2007; 318:1309–1312
35.
Scangos KW, Stuphorn V: Medial frontal cortex motivates but does not control movement initiation in the countermanding task. J Neurosci 2010; 30:1968–1982
36.
Liu X, Hairston J, Schrier M, Fan J: Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011; 35:1219–1236
37.
Elliott R, Newman JL, Longe OA, Deakin JF: Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex to financial reward in humans: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2003; 23:303–307
38.
Claus ED, Kiehl KA, Hutchison KE: Neural and behavioral mechanisms of impulsive choice in alcohol use disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2011; 35:1209–1219
39.
O’Leary-Barrett M, Topper L, Al-Khudhairy N, Pihl RO, Castellanos-Ryan N, Mackie CJ, Conrod PJ: Two-year impact of personality-targeted, teacher-delivered interventions on youth internalizing and externalizing problems: a cluster-randomized trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 52:911–920
40.
Conrod PJ, O’Leary-Barrett M, Newton N, Topper L, Castellanos-Ryan N, Mackie C, Girard A: Effectiveness of a selective, personality-targeted prevention program for adolescent alcohol use and misuse: a cluster randomized controlled trial. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:334–342
41.
Westerberg H, Klingberg T: Changes in cortical activity after training of working memory: a single-subject analysis. Physiol Behav 2007; 92:186–192

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1310 - 1319
PubMed: 25073448

History

Received: 14 November 2013
Revision received: 3 March 2014
Revision received: 22 May 2014
Accepted: 2 June 2014
Published ahead of print: 31 October 2014
Published online: 1 December 2014
Published in print: December 01, 2014

Authors

Details

Natalie Castellanos-Ryan, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Maren Struve, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Robert Whelan, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Tobias Banaschewski, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Gareth J. Barker, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Arun L.W. Bokde, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Uli Bromberg, Dipl.Psych.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Christian Büchel, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Herta Flor, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Mira Fauth-Bühler, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Vincent Frouin, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Juergen Gallinat, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Penny Gowland, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Andreas Heinz, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Claire Lawrence, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Jean-Luc Martinot, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Frauke Nees, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Tomas Paus, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Zdenka Pausova, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Marcella Rietschel, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Trevor W. Robbins, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Michael N. Smolka, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Gunter Schumann, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Hugh Garavan, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
Patricia J. Conrod, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.
The IMAGEN Consortium
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, and CHU Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montreal; the Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience and the Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington; the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; the Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; Neurospin, Atomic Energy and Alternative Energies Commission, Paris; the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité-Universitätsmedizin, Berlin; the School of Psychology and the School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, U.K.; Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany; Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) Unit 1000, Imaging and Psychiatry, SHFJ CEA, University of Paris South, Orsay, France; AP-HP Department of Adolescent Psychopathology and Medicine, Maison de Solenn, University Paris Descartes, Paris; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal; Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto; Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany; and MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Conrod ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Dr. Banaschewski has served as an adviser to or consultant for Eli Lilly, Hexal Pharma, Medice, Novartis, Otsuka, Oxford Outcomes, PCM Scientific, Shire, and Viforpharma and has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Eli Lilly, Shire, Viforpharma; he has received conference attendance support and conference support or received speaking fees from Eli Lilly, Medice, Novartis, and Shire. Dr. Barker has received teaching honoraria from General Electric Healthcare and has served as a consultant for IXICO. Dr. Robbins has served as a consultant for Cambridge Cognition, Chempartners, Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, Shire, and Teva, has received research support from Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, and Lundbeck, has received royalties from Cambridge Cognition, and has received editorial honoraria from Elsevier and Springer-Verlag. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.Supported by the European Union-funded FP6 Integrated Project IMAGEN (Reinforcement- Related Behaviour in Normal Brain Function and Psychopathology; LSHM-CT- 2007-037286), the FP7 project IMAGEMEND (Imaging Genetics for Mental Disorders) and the Innovative Medicine Initiative Project EU-AIMS (115300-2), the Medical Research Council Programme Grant “Developmental Pathways Into Adolescent Substance Abuse” (93558), and the Swedish funding agency FORMAS. Further support was provided by the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF grants 01GS08152 and 01EV0711), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) Reinhart-Koselleck Award (SP 383/5-1), and DFG grants SM80/5-2, SM 80/7-1, SFB 940/1. This research was also supported by the German Ministry of Education and Research (grant 01EV0711). Dr. Castellanos-Ryan’s and Dr. Conrod’s salaries are awarded from the Fonds de Recherche du Québec–Santé.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share