Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

The authors assessed the functioning of mesolimbic and striatal areas involved in reward-based spatial learning in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD).

Method:

Functional MRI blood-oxygen-level-dependent response was compared in 33 unmedicated adults with OCD and 33 healthy, age-matched comparison subjects during a reward-based learning task that required learning to use extramaze cues to navigate a virtual eight-arm radial maze to find hidden rewards. The groups were compared in their patterns of brain activation associated with reward-based spatial learning versus a control condition in which rewards were unexpected because they were allotted pseudorandomly to experimentally prevent learning.

Results:

Both groups learned to navigate the maze to find hidden rewards, but group differences in neural activity during navigation and reward processing were detected in mesolimbic and striatal areas. During navigation, the OCD group, unlike the healthy comparison group, exhibited activation in the left posterior hippocampus. Unlike healthy subjects, participants in the OCD group did not show activation in the left ventral putamen and amygdala when anticipating rewards or in the left hippocampus, amygdala, and ventral putamen when receiving unexpected rewards (control condition). Signal in these regions decreased relative to baseline during unexpected reward receipt among those in the OCD group, and the degree of activation was inversely associated with doubt/checking symptoms.

Conclusions:

Participants in the OCD group displayed abnormal recruitment of mesolimbic and ventral striatal circuitry during reward-based spatial learning. Whereas healthy comparison subjects exhibited activation in this circuitry in response to the violation of reward expectations, unmedicated OCD participants did not and instead over-relied on the posterior hippocampus during learning. Thus, dopaminergic innervation of reward circuitry may be altered, and future study of anterior/posterior hippocampal dysfunction in OCD is warranted.
Functional abnormalities in fronto-striatal circuits underlie inhibitory control deficits and cognitive inflexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (1), while dysfunction in mesolimbic regions (e.g., the hippocampus and amygdala) underlies fear expression in patients with the disorder (2). Together with ventral fronto-striatal regions (e.g., the orbitofrontal cortex and ventral striatum), these mesolimbic regions comprise a reward-processing system (3) that allows us to anticipate, respond to, and learn from reward outcomes in quotidian life. Whereas the ventral/anterior hippocampus is intrinsically connected to the ventral striatum (4), processes reward-related information, and is preferentially involved in anxiety (5), the dorsal/posterior hippocampus preferentially processes spatial information (6). Using an ecologically valid task of reward-based spatial learning adapted from animal research, we sought to identify functional impairments in mesolimbic and ventral striatal circuitry that may contribute to OCD behaviors.
OCD patients perform poorly on tasks that require adjusting responses based on changing reward feedback (7), consistent with findings of aberrant processing of reward in the orbital frontal cortex and ventral striatum during reversal learning (8) and reward anticipation (9). Visuospatial impairment has also been described (10, 11), but the neural correlates of spatial learning have not been assessed in OCD. Together with anatomical findings of reduced gray matter in corticolimbic areas (12) involved in reward expectancy (13) and of smaller amygdala and hippocampal volumes in patients with refractory OCD, when compared with a comparison group (14), these data suggest that OCD patients have functional and structural abnormalities in the brain regions that support reward-based spatial learning.
Spatial learning is often assessed in rodents by having them navigate an eight-arm radial maze (15). We adapted this paradigm to a virtual reality environment for use with functional MRI (fMRI) (16). Both the animal and human tasks require learning to use extramaze cues to navigate and find hidden rewards. Healthy individuals activate temporoparietal areas when searching the maze, as occurs with other spatial navigation tasks (1720). Importantly, our task includes a control condition in which the use of spatial cues to find hidden rewards (i.e., spatial learning) is experimentally disabled, allowing us to assess the neural correlates of reward processing in the absence of spatial learning and disentangle the neural correlates of reward processing and learning. Healthy individuals activate the hippocampus and amygdala when receiving unpredicted rewards in the control condition compared with the learning condition, a finding that we speculated could be attributed to enhanced dopaminergic firing from ventral tegmental areas to the ventral striatum and these mesolimbic areas in response to unpredicted rewards (21).
In the present study, we used our translational fMRI task to assess the neural correlates of reward-based spatial learning in unmedicated individuals with OCD who were free of comorbid illnesses. Twenty-one of these individuals were treatment naive, and 12 were off psychotropic medications for at least 12 weeks. Given findings of functional deficits in reward-processing circuits (9) and compensatory hippocampal engagement during other learning tasks (22) in OCD, together with the differential roles of the posterior and anterior hippocampus in processing spatial and reward information, respectively (6), we made the following hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that whereas both the OCD and healthy comparison groups would engage tempoparietal areas while navigating the maze, OCD participants would overengage the posterior hippocampus during spatial learning. Second, we suspected that OCD participants would not engage the anterior hippocampus and ventral striatum to the same extent as healthy participants during reward anticipation or in response to reward receipt, especially when rewards were unpredicted in the control condition compared with the learning condition. We also explored associations of mesolimbic and temporoparietal activations with OCD severity and symptom dimensions.

Method

Participants

Unmedicated adults with OCD (N=33) and healthy comparison subjects (N=33), group-matched by age, sex, and ethno-racial groups, were recruited through flyers, Internet advertisements, and word-of-mouth. The institutional review board of the New York State Psychiatric Institute approved this study. Participants provided written, informed consent prior to entering the study.
Details of the exclusion criteria, clinical assessments, and behavioral analyses are described in the data supplement accompanying the online version of this article.

Reward-Based Spatial Learning Paradigm

Our reward-based spatial learning paradigm has been described elsewhere (16, 23). The virtual reality environment consisted of an eight-arm radial maze surrounded by a naturalistic landscape (e.g., mountains, trees, and flowers) that constituted the extramaze cues that could be used for spatial navigation (Figure 1). Prior to scanning, participants practiced navigating a similar maze on a desktop computer.
FIGURE 1. The Virtual Reality Environmenta
a Panel A shows the schematic of the virtual maze depicting the four events modeled: 1) “searching,” 2) “reward anticipation,” and the two types of reward feedback, 3) “reward” and 4) “no-reward.” Panel B shows some of the naturalistic spatial cues in the virtual reality maze. Panel C depicts participants’ view of the virtual reality maze. Panel D shows the baited area at the end of an arm, with “$” indicating successful receipt of reward.
Stimuli during scanning were presented through nonmagnetic goggles. Participants used an MRI-compatible joystick (Current Designs, Philadelphia) to navigate the maze. Before scanning, participants were informed that they would be in the center of a maze with eight identical arms extending outwards and that hidden rewards (monetary) would be available at the end of the arms. They were instructed to navigate the maze to collect the rewards and that they could keep any money they found, but they would lose money if they revisited an arm. They were told that they would complete several sessions of the task but not that the sessions differed from one another. Therefore, they believed that they would perform the same task multiple times.
The paradigm included an active-learning and a control condition. In the learning condition, all eight arms were baited with rewards. As participants navigated the maze, they had to learn the spatial layout of the extramaze cues to avoid revisiting arms. After each arm visit (trial), participants reappeared at the center of the maze with their viewing perspective randomly reoriented to prevent use of strategies, such as chaining (systematically selecting neighboring arms). After collecting all eight rewards, the learning condition was terminated.
Next, participants were presented with a screen indicating that a new session was beginning. In this control condition, identical extramaze cues used in the learning condition were randomized among locations after each trial to destroy any possibility of using the spatial layout of the cues (spatial learning). To control for the reward/punishment frequency in the learning condition, participants were rewarded at the same frequency but without regard to their actual performance. This control condition thus shared all salient features with the learning condition, including lower-order stimulus features and higher-order task features. This condition terminated following the number of trials that a given participant needed to obtain all eight rewards in the learning condition. If a participant required 18 trials to find all eight rewards in the learning condition (i.e., eight correct and 10 error trials), they would be given 10 unbaited trials randomly in the control condition. Thus, contrasting neural activity in the learning condition (during spatial learning) and the control condition (in which spatial learning is impossible) reveals the neural correlates of reward-based spatial learning.
Participants underwent two runs of each condition. The learning condition always preceded the control condition to establish the number of trials and reward frequency for the control condition. Thus, the paradigm contained 32 rewarded navigation events (8 rewards × 2 conditions × 2 runs), but the number of unrewarded events varied for each participant. Following completion of the paradigm, participants were provided the same amount of money regardless of performance.

Image Acquisition and Processing

A General Electric Signa 3T-LX scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee) and a standard quadrature General Electric head coil were used for image acquisition. Axial functional images were positioned parallel to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line using a T1-weighted sagittal localizing scan. Functional images were obtained using a T2*-sensitive gradient-recalled single-shot echo-planar pulse sequence (time to repeat=2,800 msec; echo time=25 msec; 90° flip angle; single excitation per image; field of view=24×24 cm; matrix=64×64; 43 slices 3 mm thick, no gap, and covering the entire brain). The number of echoplanar imaging volumes collected was determined by the performance of each participant in the learning condition, with a maximum of 322 volumes per run.
As described elsewhere (16), image preprocessing procedures were run in batch mode using MATLAB 7.9 (Mathworks, Natick, Mass.) and implemented in SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London) and FSL (FMRIB Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk). Preprocessing consisted of slice-time correction, using a windowed Fourier interpolation to minimize dependence on the reference slice, motion correction, and realignment to the middle image of the middle scanning run (24). Images with estimates for peak motion exceeding 3-mm (one voxel) translation were repaired with ArtRepair (25). Runs with more than 15% of such images were discarded for poor quality (26). Motion-corrected functional images of each participant were coregistered to the corresponding three-dimensional spoiled gradient recall anatomical image, which was spatially normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (avg152T1 template brain) with a voxel size of 2×2×2 mm3. Normalization parameters warped the functional images into the same MNI space as the spoiled gradient echo image. Normalized images were spatially smoothed using a Gaussian-kernel filter with a full width at half maximum of 8 mm. Spatially smoothed fMRI time series were temporally high-pass filtered with a cutoff frequency of 1/128 Hz through a discrete cosine transform to remove low-frequency noise (e.g., scanner drift).

Image Analysis

Extraction of subject-level signal differences across the learning and control conditions of the spatial learning task was conducted using general linear models in SPM8. Four regressors corresponding to specific events that occurred during each trial of each condition were defined (Figure 1A). “Searching” was defined from the start of a trial until an arm was selected and committed to (and 10% of its length was traversed). Reward “anticipation” began after the first 10% of an arm was traversed and extended until its baited area was reached. The two types of reward feedback possible at an arm’s terminus were defined as “reward,” when a monetary reward was won, and “no-reward,” when no monetary reward was won. These regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function, with the durations of the regressors for each participant modeled according to the durations of these events during performance in the learning condition. For these regressors, a t contrast vector was applied to the parameters (beta_j) estimated for each voxel j producing four contrast images for each participant representing each regressor/event (searching, anticipation, reward, no reward) compared across the two conditions (learning, control).
A random-effects “omnibus” analysis (F test in SPM8) was used to test the significance of interactions between group (OCD, healthy comparison), condition (learning, control), and event (searching, anticipation, reward, no reward) across the whole brain, covarying for sex. To correct for multiple comparisons, we applied a cluster extent threshold with an a priori significance threshold set at a p value of 0.01. The cluster extent threshold was obtained with Monte Carlo simulations (10,000 iterations) implemented in custom software written in Matlab. Group composite activation maps generated for each contrast were used to examine the interactions resulting from the omnibus test; voxels identified using a p value threshold <0.01, together with a cluster extent threshold of 25, are reported. Subject-level fMRI signal differences across the learning and control conditions and an implicit baseline (consisting of the unmodeled components of the task) were extracted to derive parameter estimates for individual participants at specific peaks of the statistical map for that contrast. These post hoc tests determined group differences in activation associated with the learning and control conditions for each event.

Results

Participants

Thirty-three participants with OCD and 33 healthy comparison subjects were scanned. The groups were matched on demographic characteristics (Table 1). Participants in the OCD group were free of psychotropic medications (treatment-naive, N=21; free of any psychotropic medications for 109 weeks [SD=127], N=12 [see Table S1 in the online data supplement]), as well as of current comorbid axis I disorders, and nine had a lifetime history of a depressive disorder. OCD symptoms were distributed across the five symptom dimensions (27). The two groups did not differ significantly in measures of head motion within the scanner (for further details, see the online data supplement).
TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Participants With Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) and Matched Healthy Comparison Subjects
CharacteristicOCD Participants (N=33)Healthy Comparison Participants (N=33)Analysis
 MeanSDMeanSDtdfp
Age (years)29.48.1929.427.98–0.12640.99
Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence score (full–4)110.4513.1111.2613.2–0.23540.81
Duration of illness (years)14.808.60     
Age of OCD onset (years)15.057.33     
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale scores4.833.50     
Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale total score25.653.69     
 Obsessions12.281.98     
 Compulsions13.372.12     
 N%N%   
Sex       
 Male1957.601957.60   
 Female1442.401442.40   
Handedness       
 Right2884.843090.90   
 Left515.62310.00   
Race/ethnicity       
 Asian13.0013.00   
 African American515.20824.20   
 Caucasian2575.752163.60   
 Hispanic618.18618.18   
 Other26.1039.10   

Behavioral Performance

Both groups demonstrated significant improvement in performance speed and completed fewer trials in the learning condition from run 1 to run 2 (main effects of run: all p values ≤0.01 [see Table 2 and Table S2 in the data supplement). However, the OCD group completed more trials to obtain all eight rewards in run 1, contributing to a significant group-by-run interaction. In addition, performance speed in the learning condition correlated positively with OCD severity ratings on the doubt/checking dimension (p=0.03). Performance differences across conditions (analogous to the blood-oxygen-level-dependent [BOLD] contrast between the learning and control conditions) revealed a significant main effect of group for the time taken to complete both conditions in run 1 (p=0.03), deriving from the slower performance speed of OCD participants (see Table S3 in the data supplement).
TABLE 2. Group Differences in Reward-Based Spatial Learninga
ComparisonHealthy Comparison GroupOCD GroupAnalysis
Performance speed (seconds)         
 MeanSD MeanSD    
Run 1125.9100 184.2132    
Run 2100.559 85.739    
 tdfptdfp   
Run 1 versus run 21.35320.194.3532<0.01   
       Fdfp
Main effect of group      0.441, 590.51
Main effect of run      15.151, 59<0.01
Group-by-run interaction      3.611, 590.06
Number of trials         
 MeanSD MeanSD    
Run 114.96.4 20.78.5    
Run 214.810.2 14.27.9    
 tdfptdfp   
Run 1 versus run 20.16320.873.2532<0.01   
       Fdfp
Main effect of group      1.611, 590.21
Main effect of run      5.821, 590.02
Group-by-run interaction      4.791, 590.03
a
Data in bold denote statistically significant findings.

Omnibus Test of Neural Activity

The omnibus analysis revealed a significant three-way interaction (group-by-condition-by-event) in a large left hemisphere cluster (MNI coordinates x, y, z: −27, −7, −11; 454 voxels [756 mm3]; F=8.74, df=4, 252, p<0.05, corrected), encompassing the anterior and posterior hippocampus, amygdala, and ventral putamen (Figure 2). Group composite maps were then used to examine group-by-condition interactions within these regions separately for each event.
FIGURE 2. Whole-Brain Analysis Indicating Three-Way Interactions (Diagnosis-by-Condition-by-Event)a
a Interactions were detected in a left hemisphere cluster comprising the ventral putamen, amygdala, and hippocampus (maximum peak, Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates [x, y, z]: −27, −7, −11; 454 voxels [756 mm3]; F=8.74, df=4, 252, p<0.05, corrected).

Neural activity during spatial navigation.

A significant group-by-condition interaction in the left posterior hippocampus derived from activation in the OCD group but not in the healthy comparison group when navigating the maze and searching for rewards in the learning compared with control conditions (Table 3, Figures 3A and B). Both groups showed activation in the temporal and parietal regions, including the bilateral superior temporal gyrus and the lateral inferoparietal cortex (Figure 3A).
TABLE 3. Group Differences in Neural Activity Associated With Spatial Navigation and Reward Processing in the Learning Compared With Control Condition
Activated RegionsLocationSignificance Testing
HemisphereNumber of VoxelsMontreal Neurological Institute Coordinates (x, y, z)Ftdfp
Omnibus test       
 Left hemisphere clusterLeft454–27, –7, –118.74 4, 252<0.05 (corrected)
Spatial navigation (searching)       
 Posterior hippocampusLeft53–33, –32, –13 3.1564<0.001
Reward anticipation       
 Ventral putamenLeft429–21, –6, –10 3.0564<0.01
 AmygdalaLeft429–27, –7, –11 3.4764<0.01
Reward receipt       
 Anterior hippocampusLeft248–31, –16, –20 3.0464<0.01
 Ventral putamenLeft248–21, –6, –10 3.4364<0.01
 AmygdalaLeft63–27, –7, –11 2.4364<0.05
FIGURE 3. Neural Activity During Spatial Navigationa
a The left image (in panel A) shows group differences in brain activations associated with searching the maze in the learning versus the control condition detected in the left posterior hippocampus. The center and right images (in panel A) show group average brain activations for the obsessive compulsive-disorder (OCD) and healthy comparison (HC) participants with increases in signal during searching in the learning versus control condition (red) and increases during searching in the control versus active condition (blue). These maps are thresholded at our a priori significance threshold (p=0.01, cluster filter of 28). The graph (panel B) shows parameter estimates at the labeled left hippocampal cluster (Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates [x, y, z]: –33, –32, –13) in both conditions and for both groups. Error bars represent standard deviations.

Neural activity during reward anticipation.

Significant group-by-condition interactions in the left amygdala and the ventral putamen (Figure 4A) derived from activation during reward anticipation in the learning condition and deactivation in the control condition in the healthy comparison group, in contrast to activation in the control condition and deactivation (left amygdala) in the learning condition in the OCD group (Figure 4B).
FIGURE 4. Neural Activity During Reward Processinga
a The left image (in panel A) shows group differences in activations associated with reward anticipation in the learning versus control conditions detected in the left putamen and amygdala. The center and right columns (in panels A and C) show group average brain activations for the obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and healthy comparison (HC) participants with increases in signal associated with reward anticipation (panel A) and receipt (panel C) in the learning versus control condition (red) and increases in the control versus active condition (blue). In panel B, the graphs show parameter estimates at the labeled putaminal (Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates [x, y, z]: −21, 6, −10) and amygdala (−27, −7, −11) clusters in both conditions for both groups. Also shown are group differences (panel D, left column) in activations associated with reward receipt in the learning versus control conditions in the left anterior and posterior hippocampus, putamen, and amygdala, with parameter estimates (panel D) at the labeled hippocampal (−33, −32, −13), putaminal (−21, 6, −10), and amygdala (−27, 7, −11) clusters in both conditions and for both groups. These maps are thresholded at a p value of 0.025, with a cluster filter of 30. Error bars represent standard deviations.

Neural activity during reward receipt.

Significant group-by-condition interactions in the left anterior hippocampus, amygdala, and ventral putamen were detected in response to receiving expected (learning condition) and unexpected (control condition) rewards (Figure 4C). In healthy participants, activation in these regions in response to receiving unexpected rewards was accompanied by deactivation in response to receiving expected rewards. In contrast, in OCD participants, activation in these same regions in response to receiving expected rewards was accompanied by deactivation in response to receiving unexpected rewards in the control condition (Figure 4D).

Performance correlates.

Performance speed in the OCD group correlated positively with activation in the left posterior hippocampus during navigation in the learning condition (p=0.01) and inversely with activation in the left anterior hippocampus, amygdala, and putamen during reward receipt in that condition (all p values ≤0.01).

Symptom severity correlates.

OCD severity ratings on the doubt/checking dimension correlated positively with activation in the left posterior hippocampus during navigation (p=0.01) and inversely with activation in the left anterior hippocampus and ventral putamen during reward receipt in the control condition (all p values ≤0.01). Thus, the OCD participants with the most severe doubt/checking symptoms showed activation in the left hippocampus the most during navigation and in both the left hippocampus and ventral putamen the least during the receipt of unexpected rewards.

Discussion

We used a translational paradigm to investigate the neural correlates of reward-based spatial learning in unmedicated individuals with OCD. Participants had to use extramaze cues to navigate the maze and find rewards in the learning condition, but randomization of the scene prevented use of the cues to learn the reward locations; thus, spatial learning was experimentally prevented in the control condition. Both OCD and healthy participants demonstrated spatial learning, taking less time and fewer trials to find all eight rewards in the second scan run compared with the first scan run. Group differences in neural activity associated with searching the maze, anticipating, and receiving rewards were detected in a left hemisphere cluster encompassing the hippocampus, amygdala, and ventral putamen. Although both groups engaged temporoparietal areas typically engaged by healthy individuals during spatial navigation (1720), only participants in the OCD group engaged the left posterior hippocampus. Additionally, healthy participants exhibited activation in the left anterior hippocampus, amygdala, and ventral putamen when receiving unexpected rewards in the control condition, consistent with our previous findings with this task in another sample of healthy individuals (16). In contrast, in OCD participants, signal in these mesolimbic regions decreased relative to baseline in response to receiving unexpected rewards; activation was instead detected in response to receiving expected rewards in the learning condition. Finally, only healthy participants showed activation in the left ventral putamen and amygdala when anticipating rewards in the learning condition. These findings suggest abnormal functioning of mesolimbic and ventral striatal circuitry in OCD during reward-based spatial learning.
Healthy participants did not show activation in the posterior hippocampus when searching the maze, a finding we previously interpreted as evidence that the (posterior) hippocampus works with other medial temporal regions to create a map of the environment (28). In contrast, participants in the OCD group exhibited activation in the left posterior hippocampus when searching and receiving rewards in the learning condition, suggesting that they required additional neural processing resources to learn/remember the spatial layout of the cues, consistent with their needing more trials (attempts) than healthy participants to obtain all eight rewards in run 1 and with the role of the left hippocampus in episodic memory (29). OCD participants took more time to find all rewards in run 1, and their performance speed correlated positively with activation in the left posterior hippocampus during navigation. Perhaps their greater engagement of this region contributed to their greater improvement (compared with healthy participants) in performance (speed and number of trials) from run 1 to run 2. Greater reliance on the hippocampus is consistent with findings of compensatory hippocampal engagement in OCD participants during performance of other learning tasks (22). Both performance speed and activation in the left posterior hippocampus during navigation were positively associated with doubt/checking symptoms, suggesting that the OCD participants who endorsed more of these symptoms required the most time and greatest reliance on the posterior hippocampus to find all rewards.
Unlike healthy participants, unmedicated OCD participants did not show activation in the ventral striatum in response to receiving unexpected rewards in the control condition. Lesion, neurophysiological, and fMRI studies typically implicate the ventral striatum, specifically the nucleus accumbens, in processing reward prediction errors (30). The fMRI data from healthy individuals suggest that ventral striatal activation increases with positive prediction errors (i.e., when reinforcement is greater than expected [31, 32]). Our findings suggest that the receipt of unexpected rewards is the prediction error signal that activates the ventral striatum on this task in healthy participants. However, in OCD participants, the receipt of unexpected rewards was associated with decreased BOLD signal relative to baseline in the ventral putamen, an effect typically associated with omitted rewards in healthy individuals (32, 33). Abnormal ventral striatal function when processing rewards is consistent with findings from studies using a monetary incentive delay task of reward processing in OCD patients (9, 34). Our finding of attenuated ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation in OCD participants is also consistent with these previous data (9).
Together, these findings suggest ventral striatal dysfunction in reward signaling in OCD pathophysiology, perhaps contributing, in part, to the inflexible control over behaviors. Blunted reward signaling, for example, might decrease the rewarding relief that should normally result from a behavior, thereby contributing to difficulty controlling the urge to repeat it. These findings can also be interpreted in terms of the dopaminergic system, since dopamine is associated with reward-based learning (21). Neurophysiological findings suggest that ventral striatal dopaminergic neurons fire in response to unpredicted rewards (35). If ventral striatal activation reflects the normal phasic activity of dopaminergic neurons in response to reward unpredictability, then our fMRI findings suggest abnormal phasic activity of striatal dopaminergic neurons in OCD, consistent with positron emission tomography data (36).
In healthy participants, activation in the left ventral putamen, along with the anterior hippocampus and amygdala, was detected in response to receiving unexpected rewards. The ventral striatum is intrinsically connected to the anterior hippocampus (4), which has a preferential role over the posterior hippocampus in processing reward information (6), and to the amygdala, which is also involved in reward prediction error signaling (37). In OCD participants, signal in these regions decreased relative to baseline in response to receiving unexpected rewards, suggesting that the processing of reward prediction errors is abnormal in OCD. However, given the role of the anterior hippocampus in anxiety (5), this lack of activation may also represent greater baseline activity within these connected regions in persons with OCD, particularly in the right hippocampus, since our findings of group differences were localized to the left hemisphere. Thus, baseline psychophysiological measures of anxiety should be incorporated into future studies.
In OCD participants, activation in the left ventral putamen and anterior hippocampus during reward receipt in the control condition was inversely associated with severity ratings on the doubt/checking dimension, suggesting the least activation in those who endorsed the most doubt/checking symptoms. Mesolimbic dysfunction specific to this symptom dimension may, in part, be a result of reduced gray matter volume in mesolimbic areas in OCD patients with prominent checking compulsions (12, 38). Electrophysiological (39) and fMRI (40) data indicate that the anterior hippocampus encodes uncertainty, consistent with our findings of anterior hippocampal activation in response to unexpected reward in healthy individuals. Thus, the processing of uncertainty within these regions is likely altered in OCD, consistent with evidence that OCD patients—especially those with checking compulsions—are highly intolerant of uncertainty (41).
This study is limited by the modest sample size and spatial resolution of fMRI that does not allow differentiation of detailed hippocampal subregions that may contribute differently to reward-based learning. We also cannot exclude the possibility that group differences in brain activations were due, in part, to group differences in visuospatial processing or affective responses to receiving/not receiving rewards. Finally, searching and reward-related activations might be less distinct than we suggest, given the timing of the task and slowness of the hemodynamic response function.
In conclusion, this is the first study, to our knowledge, to assess the neural correlates of reward-based spatial learning using a translational fMRI paradigm in unmedicated participants with OCD. Our data point to mesolimbic and ventral striatal dysfunction associated with reward-based spatial learning in OCD, confirm findings of hippocampal compensation (22), and suggest that the neural processing of unpredictable rewards is abnormal in OCD. Future studies will determine whether these functional abnormalities precede clinical expression of OCD (and could be biomarkers of risk) or whether these abnormalities follow the clinical expression of OCD (and could be targets for treatment).

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2014.13121700.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Marsh R, Horga G, Parashar N, et al: Altered activation in fronto-striatal circuits during sequential processing of conflict in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 75:615–622
2.
Milad MR, Rauch SL: Obsessive-compulsive disorder: beyond segregated cortico-striatal pathways. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16:43–51
3.
Haber SN, Knutson B: The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35:4–26
4.
Kahn I, Shohamy D: Intrinsic connectivity between the hippocampus, nucleus accumbens, and ventral tegmental area in humans. Hippocampus 2013; 23:187–192
5.
Bannerman DM, Rawlins JN, McHugh SB, et al: Regional dissociations within the hippocampus—memory and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004; 28:273–283
6.
Behrendt RP: Conscious experience and episodic memory: hippocampus at the crossroads. Front Psychol 2013; 4:304
7.
Gillan CM, Papmeyer M, Morein-Zamir S, et al: Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168:718–726
8.
Remijnse PL, Nielen MM, van Balkom AJ, et al: Reduced orbitofrontal-striatal activity on a reversal learning task in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:1225–1236
9.
Figee M, Vink M, de Geus F, et al: Dysfunctional reward circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 69:867–874
10.
Abramovitch A, Dar R, Hermesh H, et al: Comparative neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: implications for a novel executive overload model of OCD. J Neuropsychol 2012; 6:161–191
11.
Morein-Zamir S, Craig KJ, Ersche KD, et al: Impaired visuospatial associative memory and attention in obsessive compulsive disorder but no evidence for differential dopaminergic modulation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010; 212:357–367
12.
Pujol J, Soriano-Mas C, Alonso P, et al: Mapping structural brain alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:720–730
13.
Schultz W: Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 2002; 36:241–263
14.
Atmaca M, Yildirim H, Ozdemir H, et al: Hippocampus and amygdalar volumes in patients with refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2008; 32:1283–1286
15.
Packard MG, Hirsh R, White NM: Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems. J Neurosci 1989; 9:1465–1472
16.
Marsh R, Hao X, Xu D, et al: A virtual reality-based fMRI study of reward-based spatial learning. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:2912–2921
17.
Iaria G, Chen JK, Guariglia C, et al: Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps. Eur J Neurosci 2007; 25:890–899
18.
Hartley T, Maguire EA, Spiers HJ, et al: The well-worn route and the path less traveled: distinct neural bases of route following and wayfinding in humans. Neuron 2003; 37:877–888
19.
Spiers HJ, Maguire EA: A navigational guidance system in the human brain. Hippocampus 2007; 17:618–626
20.
Aguirre GK, Detre JA, Alsop DC, et al: The parahippocampus subserves topographical learning in man. Cereb Cortex 1996; 6:823–829
21.
Wise RA: Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004; 5:483–494
22.
Rauch SL, Wedig MM, Wright CI, et al: Functional magnetic resonance imaging study of regional brain activation during implicit sequence learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:330–336
23.
Brooks SJ, O’Daly O, Uher R, et al: Thinking about eating food activates visual cortex with reduced bilateral cerebellar activation in females with anorexia nervosa: an fMRI study. PLoS ONE 2012; 7:e34000
24.
Jazzard P, Matthews PM, Smith SM: Functional MRI: An Introduction to Methods. New York, Oxford University Press, 2002
25.
Mazaika P, Hoeft F, Glover GH, et al: Methods and Software for fMRI Analysis for Clinical Subjects. Human Brain Mapping Conference, San Francisco, 2009
26.
Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, et al: Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn Reson Med 1996; 35:346–355
27.
Pinto A, Greenberg BD, Grados MA, et al: Further development of YBOCS dimensions in the OCD Collaborative Genetics study: symptoms vs categories. Psychiatry Res 2008; 160:83–93
28.
Sharp PE: Complimentary roles for hippocampal versus subicular/entorhinal place cells in coding place, context, and events. Hippocampus 1999; 9:432–443
29.
Denys D, Mantione M, Figee M, et al: Deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens for treatment-refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:1061–1068
30.
Delgado MR: Reward-related responses in the human striatum. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1104:70–88
31.
O’Doherty JP, Dayan P, Friston K, et al: Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 2003; 38:329–337
32.
McClure SM, Berns GS, Montague PR: Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron 2003; 38:339–346
33.
Ramnani N, Elliott R, Athwal BS, et al: Prediction error for free monetary reward in the human prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 2004; 23:777–786
34.
Jung WH, Kang DH, Han JY, et al: Aberrant ventral striatal responses during incentive processing in unmedicated patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011; 123:376–386
35.
Schultz W: Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci 2007; 30:203–210
36.
McAnarney ER, Zarcone J, Singh P, et al: Restrictive anorexia nervosa and set-shifting in adolescents: a biobehavioral interface. J Adolesc Health 2011; 49:99–101
37.
Liu X, Hairston J, Schrier M, et al: Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011; 35:1219–1236
38.
van den Heuvel OA, Remijnse PL, Mataix-Cols D, et al: The major symptom dimensions of obsessive-compulsive disorder are mediated by partially distinct neural systems. Brain 2009; 132:853–868
39.
Vanni-Mercier G, Mauguière F, Isnard J, et al: The hippocampus codes the uncertainty of cue-outcome associations: an intracranial electrophysiological study in humans. J Neurosci 2009; 29:5287–5294
40.
Carleton RN, Fetzner MG, Hackl JL, et al: Intolerance of uncertainty as a contributor to fear and avoidance symptoms of panic attacks. Cogn Behav Ther 2013; 42:328–341
41.
Carleton RN, Mulvogue MK, Thibodeau MA, et al: Increasingly certain about uncertainty: Intolerance of uncertainty across anxiety and depression. J Anxiety Disord 2012; 26:468–479

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 383 - 392
PubMed: 25526598

History

Received: 23 December 2013
Revision received: 22 July 2014
Revision received: 20 August 2014
Revision received: 3 September 2014
Accepted: 8 September 2014
Published online: 19 December 2014
Published in print: April 01, 2015

Authors

Details

Rachel Marsh, Ph.D.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
Gregory Z. Tau, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
Zhishun Wang, Ph.D.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
Yuankai Huo, M.S.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
Ge Liu, M.S.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
Xuejun Hao, Ph.D.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
Mark G. Packard, Ph.D.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
Bradley S. Peterson, M.D.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.
H. Blair Simpson, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Division of Translational Imaging, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Department of Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York; the Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Tex.; the Institute for the Developing Mind, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles; and the Division of Clinical Therapeutics in the Department of Psychiatry, the New York State Psychiatric Institute and the College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Marsh ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

Dr. Peterson has received grant support from NARSAD, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, NIMH, and the Simons Foundation, and has received investigator-initiated research support from Pfizer. Dr. Simpson has received research funds for clinical trials from Janssen Pharmaceuticals and Transcept Pharmaceuticals, has served as a consultant to Quintiles, Inc., and receives royalties from Cambridge University Press and UpToDate, Inc. All other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

National Institute of Mental Health 10.13039/100000025:
Supported in part by NIMH grants R21MH093889-09 (to Drs. Marsh and Simpson), K01-MH077652 (to Dr. Marsh), K24 MH091555 (to Dr. Simpson), and K02 MH74677 (to Dr. Peterson), as well as by the New York State Office of Mental Hygiene.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share