Skip to main content
Full access
Perspectives
Published Online: 1 November 2014

A Translational Neuroscience Approach to Understanding the Development of Social Anxiety Disorder and Its Pathophysiology

Abstract

This review brings together recent research from molecular, neural circuit, animal model, and human studies to help understand the neurodevelopmental mechanisms underlying social anxiety disorder. Social anxiety disorder is common and debilitating, and it often leads to further psychopathology. Numerous studies have demonstrated that extremely behaviorally inhibited and temperamentally anxious young children are at marked risk of developing social anxiety disorder. Recent work in human and nonhuman primates has identified a distributed brain network that underlies early-life anxiety including the central nucleus of the amygdala, the anterior hippocampus, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Studies in nonhuman primates have demonstrated that alterations in this circuit are trait-like in that they are stable over time and across contexts. Notably, the components of this circuit are differentially influenced by heritable and environmental factors, and specific lesion studies have demonstrated a causal role for multiple components of the circuit. Molecular studies in rodents and primates point to disrupted neurodevelopmental and neuroplastic processes within critical components of the early-life dispositional anxiety neural circuit. The possibility of identifying an early-life at-risk phenotype, along with an understanding of its neurobiology, provides an unusual opportunity to conceptualize novel preventive intervention strategies aimed at reducing the suffering of anxious children and preventing them from developing further psychopathology.
Social anxiety disorder is highly prevalent and debilitating (1), with an estimated prevalence of about 18% (2, 3). The disorder is characterized by marked fearfulness and anxiety in social or performance situations, frequently resulting in avoidance and significant disability. In addition to suffering with social anxiety disorder, afflicted individuals often develop comorbid depressive and substance use disorders (1). Data suggest that social anxiety disorder is ∼20%−40% heritable, with environmental factors accounting for the remaining variability (4). Although social anxiety disorder is commonly diagnosed during adolescence, a period during which teenagers attempt to adjust to social change, it can start before adolescence, and its antecedents often manifest early in life (5, 6). Accumulating evidence suggests that a behaviorally inhibited or temperamentally anxious disposition during childhood can lead to the development of social anxiety disorder (5, 7). This at-risk phenotype is characterized by heightened, but nonpathological, levels of anxiety and may constitute a prodromal phenotype for social anxiety disorder. Although this phenotype is moderately stable over the course of development, it does not consistently predict the development of social anxiety disorder, nor is it invariant. This suggests that early-life interventions targeting highly anxious children have the potential to prevent the development of full-blown social anxiety disorder and its common comorbidities.
In this review, we take a cross-species approach to examine the behavior, neural circuits, and molecular systems that underlie the risk of developing social anxiety disorder. We discuss the biological basis of temperamental anxiety using data from humans and rodents, but we focus on insights gleaned from our studies of nonhuman primates. Rhesus monkeys are ideal for studying mechanisms underlying human development because, given their relatively recent evolutionary divergence from humans, the organization and function of the neural systems relevant to human anxiety, including the amygdala and prefrontal cortex, are conserved (8, 9). Rhesus monkeys and humans also share similar complex social environments that rely on parent-child bonding and peer relationships. These early relationships can both encourage and discourage the adaptive social and emotional learning that helps regulate anxiety and promote survival. Our aim is to understand how inborn and environmental influences converge on the specific biological systems that underlie extreme temperamental anxiety and the risks it confers. It is our hope that understanding the mechanisms modulating these biological substrates will guide the development of novel early-life behavioral and pharmacological interventions that will provide effective treatment for children at risk of suffering from social anxiety disorder and related disorders.
The earliest manifestations of adaptive anxiety-related responses to potential threat occur during infancy and childhood; during infancy, they are characterized by increased excitability, and later, by behavioral inhibition. Behavioral inhibition occurs in response to novelty and potential threat and is associated with autonomic and pituitary-adrenal activation. Childhood behavioral inhibition is thought to manifest during the second year of life, around the time that a child emerges from the normative stage of stranger anxiety and is developing the ability to behaviorally cope with threat (10, 11). Extreme behavioral inhibition, which has received considerable attention from the pioneering work of Jerome Kagan (12), often manifests as excessive shyness and extremely reserved and avoidant behavior in social situations.
Several prospective longitudinal studies have found that extreme behavioral inhibition is associated with increased odds of developing anxiety and depressive disorders (1318). A recent meta-analysis (7) suggested that early-life behavioral inhibition is the single greatest predictor of the development of social anxiety disorder, as nearly 50% of highly behaviorally inhibited children go on to develop social anxiety disorder (Figure 1A). Although behavioral inhibition fluctuates throughout childhood, studies suggest that individuals with stable levels of high behavioral inhibition have the greatest risk of developing social anxiety disorder (15, 1921). Several prospective and retrospective self-report studies have implicated childhood behavioral inhibition as a risk factor for depressive disorders (2224), and children of mothers with anxiety or depressive disorders tend to exhibit elevated levels of behavioral inhibition (2527). Estimates of the heritability of behavioral inhibition are consistent with the estimates of ∼20%−40% for the heritability of anxiety disorders and anxiety-related neuroticism (4, 28, 29). Many anxiety disorders, including those likely to develop later in highly behaviorally inhibited children, demonstrate partial shared heritability with each other (4, 30, 31). These studies suggest that extreme behavioral inhibition is an early phenotype that confers risk for the development of a broad range of stress-related psychopathology (3235) (Figure 1B).
FIGURE 1. Trajectories of Pathological Anxiety and Risk of Psychopathology for Children With Early-Life Anxious Temperament (AT)a
a Extreme AT exists on a continuum with social anxiety disorder (panel A), and many children with high levels of AT go on to experience disabling anxiety. Nearly 50% of children with extreme AT eventually develop social anxiety disorder, while in other children levels of AT remain stable or diminish with experience and maturation (7). During childhood and adolescence (panel B), children with extreme AT are most likely to develop social anxiety disorder, but throughout life individuals with extreme AT remain at a higher risk of developing other disorders that typically become manifest later in life, such as major depressive disorder and substance use disorders (1, 13, 22).
To further understand the biology of extreme early anxiety, our group has extensively validated a developmental rhesus monkey model. To assess behavioral inhibition, our initial studies developed the no-eye-contact (NEC) condition of the human intruder paradigm, in which the duration of freezing behavior, analogous to behavioral inhibition, was assessed in response to the uncertain threat of a human intruder looking away and presenting only his or her profile, being careful to make no eye contact with the monkey (Figure 2) (36). The NEC context can last for 30 minutes, during which the human intruder remains motionless, continuously presenting his or her profile throughout the entire test period. Our extensive studies examining various fear- and anxiety-related contexts have demonstrated that the NEC context specifically and reliably elicits freezing. In contrast, direct eye contact by the human intruder often elicits an overt aggressive response from the monkey. Freezing responses are evolutionarily conserved across diverse species, functioning to help the organism remain undetected in the presence of a potential predator (37). Freezing is often accompanied by reduced coo vocalizations. Although rhesus monkey coo vocalizations are affiliative and can be used to recruit support from conspecifics, they can also attract predators. Thus, the reduction in coo calling during the NEC context is adaptive, as in the presence of a potential predator the value of remaining undetected is a survival imperative. The NEC context also induces physiological changes, such as increased cortisol levels and right-frontal EEG asymmetry (38, 39). Moreover, similar to symptoms of anxiety in humans, behavioral inhibition in monkeys can be decreased with administration of the GABA-enhancing anxiolytic agent diazepam (36).
FIGURE 2. Measurement of Anxious Temperament (AT) in Human Children and Young Monkeys by Exposing Them to Potentially Threatening Contextsa
a In panel A, children confronted with a stranger or novel situation respond with varying degrees of behavioral inhibition and physiological activation. In panel B, during the no-eye-contact (NEC) context, an unfamiliar human intruder stands approximately 2.5 m from the monkey and remains still while looking away and presenting his or her profile to the monkey, making sure to make no eye contact. This potentially threatening NEC context specifically elicits robust behavioral inhibition and physiological activation. In contrast, other contexts, such as when a human intruder stares at the monkey, more robustly elicit fight-or-flight responses (36). Depending on the experiment, the NEC context can last 10–30 minutes. The human intruder remains motionless, continuing to present his or her profile throughout the entire test period.
To extend the assessment of behavioral inhibition, we developed the concept of anxious temperament (AT) to more completely reflect an individual’s dispositional physiological and behavioral responses to potential threat. In the monkey model, this composite AT measure is an average of standardized levels of NEC-context-induced freezing, coo vocalization reductions, and NEC-induced cortisol levels. More broadly, in relation to humans, we use the term AT to define the temperamental predisposition to display behavioral inhibition (increased freezing and decreased vocalizations) along with increased physiological reactivity (increased levels of the stress hormone cortisol) when exposed to novelty, unfamiliar individuals, or other potentially threatening situations. Compared with behavioral inhibition alone, we believe that the composite AT measure better estimates the at-risk human phenotype (40).
Although there are no diagnostic criteria for mental disorders in nonhuman primates, data suggest that monkeys with high AT are functionally impaired. Anecdotally, the veterinary records from one extremely high AT monkey in our colony revealed significant stress-related symptoms, including hair loss, chronic diarrhea, and extreme fearfulness (e.g., refusal to take treats, retreating to the back of the cage, and excessive “crying”). We also characterized AT during the NEC context in a large free-ranging colony of monkeys on the island of Cayo Santiago. Naturalistic observations of these animals revealed that high AT, tested in a field-improvised laboratory, was associated with elevated social inhibition (unpublished data). Specifically, high AT predicted fewer conspecific approaches (rs=−0.44, 0.007) when animals were free ranging, and high-AT animals maintained larger distances between themselves and their peers (rs=−0.31, p=0.03). At the most extreme, some of the highest AT animals were never observed to approach their peers, while during the same period their low-AT counterparts approached their peers upwards of 50 times. These data add to the relevance of the extreme-AT monkey model as it relates to the dysfunction experienced by extremely anxious children. Further supporting the homology between human and monkey AT is our demonstration that individual differences in AT are relatively stable across development and that AT is ∼20%−40% heritable (41, 42). Like human children, some high-AT monkeys exhibit a reduction in their AT levels as they mature, providing a unique opportunity for future studies to prospectively characterize brain mechanisms underlying recovery and resilience.

Brain Regions Associated With AT

In an initial functional MRI (fMRI) study, Schwartz et al. examined young adults who during their second year of life had been characterized as inhibited or uninhibited (43). Strikingly, the results demonstrated that inhibited individuals had increased amygdala activation in response to novel neutral faces approximately 20 years after the original assessment. More recently, researchers demonstrated increased novelty-related amygdala activation in inhibited males approximately 18 years after being characterized as highly reactive at 4 months of age (44). Similarly, young adolescents (∼12.5 years old) who were inhibited during childhood had increased amygdala activation when instructed to rate their emotional responses to fearful faces (45). Extending these studies in prospectively characterized children, Blackford et al. (4651) studied young adults who self-reported current and past behavioral inhibition. These studies provide additional evidence that amygdala activation is associated with an inhibited temperament, and they extend previous findings by implicating specific processes within the amygdala. Specifically, Blackford et al. demonstrated that highly inhibited young adults show faster amygdala response (46), prolonged and exaggerated amygdala reactivity (47, 48), decreased amygdala habituation (49), and increased amygdala volume (50). In addition to elucidating the role of the amygdala, Blackford et al. have begun to extend the set of regions associated with behavioral inhibition to include the hippocampus (49), the lateral and medial orbitofrontal cortex (47, 48), and the insular cortex (48), as well as altered behavioral inhibition-related connectivity between these regions and the amygdala (51).
In neuroimaging studies of behavioral inhibition, the amygdala regions associated with behavioral inhibition are in the dorsal amygdala (Figure 3A). The dorsal amygdala is anatomically distinct from the more ventrally located quasi-cortical basal and lateral amygdalar nuclei. The central nucleus of the amygdala, which is located within the dorsal amygdala region, is primarily composed of striatal-like GABA-ergic neurons and is considered to be the primary output structure of the amygdala complex (53). In imaging studies, the precise localization of dorsal amygdala activations should be considered tentative, given the relatively large spatial confidence intervals associated with fMRI, particularly in studies examining relatively small groups of individuals (i.e., less than 50). Nevertheless, these studies suggest that the central nucleus may be important for instantiating the increased emotional reactivity characteristic of high-AT individuals.
FIGURE 3. Dorsal Amygdala Activation Predicts Variation in Anxious Temperament (AT) in Humans and Monkeysa
a From seven published reports examining the role of the amygdala in individuals with a history of childhood behavioral inhibition, we performed a two-dimensional activation likelihood meta-analysis of the location of activation peaks in the dorsal/ventral and medial/lateral dimensions. As shown in panel A, after dilating each peak with a 4-mm2 sphere, we found that six of the eight amygdala peaks overlapped (yellow) in the dorsal amygdala region (four of the peaks extended into the region shown in red) (AC-PC=anterior commissure-posterior commissure). In panel B, [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography of 238 rhesus monkeys (41) revealed that metabolism within the anterior temporal lobe predicted AT (yellow). Similar to the human studies, the peak of this region was located in the dorsal amygdala (the peak is shown in white and the 95% spatial confidence interval in red). In both humans (panel C) and monkeys (panel D), the peak activations correspond to the location of the central nucleus of the amygdala (52). Ce=central nucleus; AB=accessory basal nucleus; B=basal nucleus; L=lateral nucleus.
fMRI studies in healthy individuals experiencing fear and anxiety have consistently identified an underlying neural circuit that includes the amygdala, the hippocampus, the prefrontal cortex, and the insula. Moreover, adults with anxiety disorders (e.g., social and specific phobia) show increased activation in these same regions (54). Interestingly, novel social stimuli, such as emotional faces and eye whites, are sufficient to activate the amygdala (55). Consistent with the continuity between dispositional anxiety and anxiety disorders, children with generalized anxiety disorder also show increased amygdala activation (56). In addition to activation in the amygdala, children with generalized anxiety disorder demonstrate increased activation in prefrontal cortical area 47 near the anterior limb of the insula and increased amygdala-mid-insula functional coupling (56).
To further examine the temperamental nature of anxiety-related brain metabolism, we examined young rhesus monkeys (1–4 years old, corresponding approximately to ages 3–12 years in humans) by phenotyping them for AT and performing [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET). Because the FDG-PET human intruder paradigm allows for the simultaneous assessment of brain metabolism and AT, this model can provide insights into the neural substrates underlying AT. As previously described, AT was assessed by combining the NEC-context-induced decreased spontaneous “coo” vocalizations (thought to reflect “calls for help”), increased freezing (or behavioral inhibition), and increased stress-induced cortisol levels (36, 40). Our initial studies demonstrated that the components of AT are associated with metabolism in the extended amygdala, including the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Figure 3B), as well as the hippocampus, the anterior temporal lobe, and the brainstem periaqueductal gray matter (57, 58). The extended amygdala comprises the central nucleus of the amygdala, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and other forebrain structures that play an important role in the initiation of fear and maintenance of anxiety. Moreover, we found that our composite AT measure predicted significantly more variance in amygdala metabolism than any of the components that comprise AT (40, 59). Later studies examining FDG-PET in relation to AT during the NEC context in more than 200 young rhesus monkeys revealed that metabolism in anterior temporal lobe structures, including the central nucleus of the amygdala, the anterior hippocampus, and the anterior temporal pole, robustly predicted AT (41). These findings are consistent with human research in highly behaviorally inhibited individuals and in patients with anxiety disorders in that they provide evidence for the involvement of anterior temporal systems in the at-risk phenotype.
The fMRI and FDG-PET studies discussed above have been limited to studying brain activity in potentially stressful contexts—in the MRI scanner (60) and in the NEC context. To further elucidate the temperamental nature of brain metabolism, we extended these studies to examine brain activity during nonstressful conditions. Specifically, we performed FDG-PET scans on animals that were each exposed to two different stressful conditions (NEC and separation from cagemate into a test cage), and two different nonstressful conditions (alone in the home cage without cagemate and life as usual in the home cage with cagemate). Trait-like positive correlations between individual differences in AT and metabolism in the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior temporal pole, and periaqueductal gray matter were found in each condition regardless of the level of stress (40). Additionally, we examined the stability of AT’s neural substrates across time by assessing AT and FDG-PET in 24 animals that were exposed to the NEC context three times over 6–18 months. The results demonstrated interindividual stability over time in brain metabolism within AT-related regions (61). Additionally, the mean metabolism across the three observations predicted the individual’s mean AT (61). These data indicate that context-independent and temporally stable neural substrates underlie the trait-like nature of AT. These findings provide insight into AT, as they suggest that the neural substrates of AT are present even when no behavioral manifestations are apparent.
While our definition of AT is fairly circumscribed, there remains substantial variability in how AT presents. This variability is similar to the symptom heterogeneity observed within anxiety and affective diagnostic categories. For example, some monkeys display substantial freezing behavior while maintaining average levels of cortisol and emitting a normative number of coo calls. In contrast, other monkeys have high levels of cortisol relative to their behavioral responses. Studies in rodents by the Blanchards and others examining threat responses have suggested that while the activation of different physiological and behavioral responses can adaptively work together, they may also have different adaptive functions (37). This raises the intriguing possibility that animals expressing different anxiety response profiles have tendencies to activate common neural circuits that can, through their effects on specific neural substrates, bias physiology and behavior toward different adaptive responses. In examining the neural substrates underlying AT’s components, we found both common and specific brain regions that underlie the phenotype’s heterogeneity (59). The common brain regions included the central nucleus of the amygdala and anterior hippocampal regions, in which metabolism was independently associated with variation in freezing, cooing, and cortisol levels. This finding suggests that regardless of their “symptomatic” presentation, individuals with high levels of AT have increased metabolism in these brain regions. We also identified regional metabolism that was specific to each component of AT. For example, metabolism in the mid-hippocampus was uniquely associated with cortisol levels, as compared with freezing or coo vocalizations. Together these findings demonstrate that AT has both common and presentation-specific neural substrates and highlight the opportunity for understanding neural substrates that cut across phenotypic heterogeneity.
To understand how different brain systems relate to the heritability of AT, we used our large sample of brain imaging data from a multigenerational family pedigree to perform whole-brain heritability analyses. This was the first study to examine the heritability of brain metabolism across the entire brain. We were surprised to find differential heritability within AT’s neural substrates. Our results demonstrated significant heritability of anterior hippocampal metabolism, but no significant heritability of amygdalar central nucleus metabolism (41). These findings call into question the view that it is solely amygdala-altering genes that are responsible for the intergenerational transmission of anxiety. Rather, our findings suggest that AT-related genes are more likely to exert their influence by altering function in other components of the AT-related circuit. Moreover, the lack of heritability within the central nucleus of the amygdala implies that this region may be more likely to mediate the environmental influences known to modulate AT, such as parenting, behavior modeling, and exposure to stress.

Causal Brain Regions and AT

Studies have been performed in patients with varying degrees of amygdala damage (6264). Consistent with functional imaging findings, a rare patient with bilateral damage to her entire amygdala was shown not to experience psychological discomfort in response to invasions of her personal space (65), did not have normative distrust of strangers (66, 67), and did not report normal fearfulness (68)—all features associated with decreased AT. This work supports the role of an amygdala-centered network in adaptive fear and anxiety as well as in anxiety disorders.
Targeted lesion studies in nonhuman primates reveal a causal role for dorsal amygdala regions in AT. Specific amygdala lesions decrease one’s reticence to act in potentially threatening situations and alter stress-induced cortisol release (6971). Amygdala lesions also decrease anxiety in novel social situations, consistent with its role in social anxiety (72, 73). Our studies employing specific neurotoxic lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala (74) demonstrated decreased freezing behavior and increased spontaneous coo vocalizations, two core components of AT. Although the central nucleus lesions did not directly affect cortisol, they reduced plasma concentrations of ACTH and CSF concentrations of corticotropin-releasing hormone, the two key upstream mediators of cortisol release.
Targeted lesion studies in primates have also assessed the causal influences of the hippocampus and orbitofrontal cortex on components of AT. Both of these regions have direct connections to the amygdala and are thought to play regulatory roles and provide contextual/regulatory information to the amygdala (Figure 4). Of particular interest is the finding that orbitofrontal cortex aspiration lesions decrease freezing behavior and cortisol levels (70, 71, 75). By combining the lesion strategy with FDG-PET imaging, we found that the effects of orbitofrontal cortex lesions on AT could be explained by orbitofrontal cortex-induced changes in the extended amygdala (i.e., the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis), a region we previously found to be associated with AT (76). Because orbitofrontal cortex aspiration lesions can also disrupt axons passing through this region, it is possible that the effects of these lesions are not due to orbitofrontal cortex damage per se, but rather result from damage to fibers originating in other prefrontal cortical regions. Consistent with this possibility, our recent fMRI study (77) found that increased metabolism of the central nucleus of the amygdala and AT are associated with decreased central nucleus-dorsolateral prefrontal cortical intrinsic connectivity. In contrast to the effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex and central nucleus, the evidence for hippocampal lesions affecting primate AT is mixed (69, 70). Together, these data suggest that the central nucleus of the amygdala, the orbitofrontal cortex, and possibly the hippocampus may each causally influence AT, emphasizing the contribution of multiple regions to dispositional anxiety.
FIGURE 4. Simplified Amygdala-Centric Model of the Brain Systems That Contribute to Monkey Anxious Temperament (AT)a
a Although the full extent of AT’s neural substrates remains unknown, neuroimaging work is beginning to identify regions that are more active in individuals with extreme AT, and lesion work suggests that at least some of these regions are causally involved in the genesis of AT. The most compelling evidence exists for the amygdala, which is a critical component of AT’s neural substrates and is the focus of extensive research that implicates it in fear- and anxiety-related processing. Here we present a simplified diagram of the monkey amygdala and how it fits into the larger set of brain systems that influence AT. The amygdala receives input from AT-related regulatory/evaluative (green), contextual (blue), and sensory (orange) neural systems, each of which is distributed throughout the brain. In general, amygdala information flows from the more ventral basal regions toward the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, which, via their projections to brainstem and hypothalamic structures (pink), initiate fear- and anxiety-related physiological and behavioral responses. All images are shown on slices adapted from reference 52. Ce=central nucleus; AB=accessory basal nucleus; B=basal nucleus; L=lateral nucleus; Me=medial nucleus; ICMs=intercalated masses; BST=bed nucleus of stria terminalis; ic=internal capsule; st=stria terminalis; ac=anterior commissure.
Lesion studies in rodents, although not assessing AT specifically, have demonstrated a causal role for many AT-related regions in unconditioned anxiety behaviors. In particular, rodent studies of unconditioned anxiety have causally implicated the amygdala (78), the ventral hippocampus (similar to the anterior hippocampus in primates) (79, 80), and the extended amygdala, including both the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (78, 81). In both rodents and primates, the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis project to the downstream structures necessary for initiating specific behavioral and physiological aspects of the fear response (Figure 4). Elegant rodent studies have demonstrated dissociable roles for the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, such that the central nucleus is required for processing immediate and imminent threats, whereas the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis is required for responding to prolonged and more distant threats (82). More recently, targeted optogenetic functional manipulations of specific projections and detailed anatomical studies have begun to elucidate projection- and cell-specific function within AT-related circuits. For example, some basolateral amygdala neurons provide excitatory input to the hippocampus and the central nucleus of the amygdala, which can initiate unconditioned fear- and anxiety-related behaviors (83, 84). Moreover, specific subregions and cell types within the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis have been demonstrated to mediate specific phenotypic expressions of anxiety (85, 86). This suggests that selective alterations in the extended amygdala could give rise to the phenotypic heterogeneity observed in high-AT primates and humans with anxiety disorders. The rodent studies complement the human and nonhuman primate studies, strengthening support for the central nucleus of the amygdala in unconditioned anxiety and drawing attention to other components of the extended amygdala.

Molecular Processes Underlying AT

To develop ideal interventions aimed at preventing the long-term negative consequences of early-life AT, it is important to understand the molecular alterations occurring within the AT neural circuit. Genetic studies indicate that either extremely rare critical polymorphisms or many polymorphisms with small additive effects influence anxiety. Moreover, the influence of parents can have effects on anxiety via alterations in DNA methylation and other epigenetic phenomena that, in the case of methylation, can be passed down as alterations in parental methylation profiles and modified by parental behavior (8789). Because mRNA levels reflect the confluence of genetic and environmental effects, we believe that examining gene expression within the neural substrates of AT can provide important clues. Because genetic methylation and expression profiles vary by tissue, region, and time, animal models are critical for developing a better understanding of the molecular alterations that underlie the altered brain function occurring in early-life AT.
Because the strongest evidence linking brain alterations to AT points to the central nucleus of the amygdala, we examined individual differences in central nucleus gene expression in relation to AT (61, 90). We performed prospective longitudinal brain imaging with behavioral and physiological assessments on 24 rhesus monkeys prior to measuring central nucleus gene expression. Altered gene expression occurred in some prominent anxiety-related neurochemical systems—neuropeptide Y and serotonin systems—such that individuals with high levels of NPY1R or 5HT2C gene expression in the central nucleus demonstrated lower levels of AT. AT was also negatively associated with alterations in neurodevelopmental systems within neurotrophic and cellular adhesion pathways (Figure 5). In particular, we observed a negative correlation between AT and the expression of neurotrophic receptor kinase 3 (NTRK3, also known as trkc) as well as its downstream partners, insulin receptor substrate 2 (IRS2) and ribosomal protein S6 kinase, 90kDa, polypeptide 3 (RPS6KA3, also known as RSK2). These genes are involved in growth factor membrane signaling (NTRK3), intracellular signaling (IRS2), and nuclear activation (RPS6KA3) (Figure 5B), all of which contribute to synaptic plasticity and development (Figure 5C). Importantly, individual differences in expression levels of NTRK3 in the central nucleus of the amygdala predicted trait-like central nucleus metabolism. NTRK3 is a growth-factor receptor that when activated can initiate widespread changes in cell growth and plasticity, similar to those seen after injection of BDNF, which binds to NTRK2 (also known as TrkB). In relation to potential epigenetic mechanisms associated with AT, we observed that high-AT individuals had decreased levels of GADD45B (growth arrest and DNA-damage-inducible, beta) in the central nucleus of the amygdala. GADD45B is known to be involved in plasticity and neurogenesis through activity-dependent methylation of growth factors and thus may be relevant to the observed decreases in levels of NTRK3 and its downstream partners (91). Other nonhuman primate studies of early-life stress have also implicated neurodevelopmental pathways (92, 93). Interestingly, recent research in squirrel monkeys demonstrated that coping in response to mild stress increases hippocampal neurogenesis and identified neurogenesis-related hippocampal gene expression in growth-related pathways that included NTRK3 (92).
FIGURE 5. Mechanisms of Decreased Neuroplasticity Mechanisms in the Maintenance of Early-Life Anxious Temperament (AT)a
a Research suggests that anxious individuals have decreased neurodevelopmental- and neuroplasticity-related gene activation in brain regions underlying AT, such as the central nucleus of the amygdala and the hippocampus (panel A). Within these regions (panel B), genes that encode adhesion molecules (e.g., EPHA4), trk receptors (e.g., NTRK3), intracellular kinase signaling molecules (e.g., IRS2), and intranuclear kinases (e.g., RPS6KA3) are inversely associated with individual differences in AT. These specific genes function to increase neuroplasticity (panel C) through their influences on synaptic plasticity, increasing spine size and creating new synapses, new spines, and new neurons.
Akil et al. have found similar results in relation to anxiety and depression (94). Studies of gene expression in the frontal cortex of patients with major depressive disorder identified decreased expression of fibroblast growth factor 2 (FGF2) (95). Much like activation of NTRK3, activation of FGF2 can increase neurogenesis and synaptic plasticity. FGF2 was also down-regulated in rodents bred to be behaviorally inhibited (96). Excitingly, a single injection of FGF2 into the behaviorally inhibited rodents during the first days of life, prior to formation of the blood-brain barrier, was sufficient to decrease behavioral inhibition (97). Follow-up study of these rodents revealed increased hippocampal neurogenesis and increased expression of neuroplasticity-related genes, including NTRK3, in relevant brain regions (97). These data implicate the FGF family as important for the development of anxiety during early life and further support plasticity-related interventions aimed at decreasing AT (94).
Researchers investigating the serotonin system in rodents have suggested that similar neuroplasticity-related mechanisms underlie anxiety and the efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). SSRIs are effective in treating anxiety disorders but often take weeks to fully work, suggesting an indirect mechanism. Stress impairs, whereas SSRIs increase hippocampal neurogenesis (98, 99), and preventing hippocampal neurogenesis via irradiation blocks the effects of SSRIs (99, 100). Recent studies have suggested that immature hippocampal neurons, in part, mediate the effects of SSRIs by enhancing the ability to discriminate complex threat-relevant information (101, 102). Interestingly, the effects of SSRIs on neurogenesis seem to be mediated by the BDNF receptor, NTRK2 (i.e., TrkB). These data further support the role of tyrosine kinase pathways, as well as neuroplasticity, in relation to anxiety.

Research and Treatment Implications

Social anxiety disorder is common and debilitating. Lifelong social anxiety disorder often leads to further psychopathology, including mood and substance use disorders. Because it is possible to identify children at risk for social anxiety disorder early in life, the field has an unusual opportunity to conceptualize novel preventive intervention strategies. Treatments for social anxiety disorder are not completely effective, and no treatments exist for children with extreme AT, the forerunner of social anxiety disorder. While some children with extreme AT overcome their anxiety, early interventions promise to increase the number of children who grow up to be psychopathology free (19, 21).
Evolutionarily conserved anxiety-related phenotypes have facilitated cross-species translational research. Studies of the neural circuits of social anxiety disorder and nonhuman primate AT implicate the dorsal amygdala, the anterior hippocampus, brainstem regions, and the orbitofrontal cortex. The homology between rhesus AT, childhood dispositional anxiety, and social anxiety disorder provides a framework for the valid use of nonhuman primates in new treatment development.
Alterations in brain function associated with AT are stable over time and are context independent. In contrast, the symptoms associated with AT and social anxiety disorder are elicited by specific cues and contexts associated with potential threat. These findings provide a conceptual basis for new treatments directed at changing the stable altered neural tendencies of individuals affected by AT and social anxiety disorder. Attempting to modify one’s trait-like brain function has the potential advantage of targeting mechanisms that may result in relapse and failure to respond. Lesions to different components of AT’s neural circuit diminish but fail to completely normalize AT. Thus, treatments targeting multiple AT-related brain regions are likely to be most successful in treating social anxiety disorder and preventing its development. Our research provides insights into which brain regions should be targeted. We identified anxiety-general regions that underlie anxiety regardless of how it is expressed and phenotype-specific regions that are uniquely involved in a particular expression of anxiety, such as freezing. Therefore, fully effective treatments will need to target anxiety-general regions as well as response-specific regions as they relate to the diverse presentations of individuals with AT and social anxiety disorder. The data demonstrate that the intergenerational transmission of anxiety is mediated by a widely distributed set of brain regions with large variation in the extent to which altered metabolism in these regions is heritable. This raises the possibility that optimal neural treatment targets could vary depending on one’s family history of anxiety. As treatments become more neuroscientifically focused, it is likely that neural measures reflecting treatment-related changes in AT’s neural circuits will be useful predictors of long-term treatment outcomes.
Characterizing the molecular alterations in brain regions associated with anxiety and AT has begun to identify novel treatment targets. Numerous rodent studies have focused on the hippocampus, where plasticity and neurogenesis have been associated with lower anxiety. Our group initially focused on the central nucleus of the amygdala for molecular analyses because the central nucleus is a core and causal component of AT’s stable neural substrate. Finding a reduction in neuroplasticity-related genes that are associated with increased central nucleus metabolism and AT led us to speculate that extreme early-life AT may result from a diminished ability to modify intra-central nucleus circuits. Studies of amygdala development in nonhuman primates have demonstrated that the central nucleus undergoes protracted development (103), which seems to parallel the developmental course for children’s increased tendency to react by freezing to uncertainty and novelty (i.e., normative stranger anxiety). Thus, plasticity within this network is likely to be critical for the capacity to emerge from this period of heightened childhood fearfulness. We further hypothesize that the maturational ability to overcome or “unlearn” normative childhood fears relies on neuroplasticity mechanisms within AT’s neural substrates. Although the work described above specifically implicates NTRK3, FGF2, and other plasticity-related targets in relation to anxiety, we believe that the study results reflect involvement of broader neuroplasticity-related systems. Based on these results, it is likely that treatments that specifically increase neuroplasticity within the central nucleus and other components of AT’s neural substrates will be most effective in modulating early-life AT and preventing the development of social anxiety disorder. Neuroplasticity mechanisms in the hippocampus have been well studied and linked to antidepressant effects (99, 100). Because of the central role of the central nucleus of the amygdala in AT, studies elucidating central nucleus-specific neuroplasticity molecular pathways will be important in conceptualizing novel treatments.
We have made the case for developing early interventions that are aimed at preventing high-AT children from developing anxiety disorders. Because AT emerges during a period when the brain is rapidly changing, treatments aimed at altering mechanisms underlying aberrant brain development are likely to have the potential for long-term changes in anxiety trajectories. Animal studies provide evidence for many ways to influence neuroplasticity that are relevant to treatment, such as exercise, SSRIs, and brain electrical stimulation. The building of synapses, neurons, and the resulting refinement of brain networks is a complex process involving many diverse molecules. Future work should focus on behavioral, pharmacological, and neuromodulatory strategies aimed at modulating diverse neuroplasticity-related molecules within specific components of AT’s neural substrates, such as the central nucleus of the amygdala and the anterior hippocampus.
In summary, research relevant to AT and the development of social anxiety disorder has revealed a number of important insights that can be helpful in formulating neuroscientifically based early-life interventions: 1) children with extreme AT are at high risk of developing further psychopathology, especially social anxiety disorder; 2) the neural circuits that underlie social anxiety disorder and human temperamental anxiety are similar to those implicated in monkeys with extreme AT; 3) the neural circuits that underlie AT are trait-like; 4) heterogeneous presentations of AT are associated with activity in both shared and phenotype-specific neural substrates; 5) environment and heritability differentially influence components of AT’s neural circuit; and 6) preliminary evidence points to altered neuroplasticity-related gene expression in the genesis of AT. It is our hope that this review will help focus research efforts on early interventions that are designed to not only reduce the suffering of anxious children but also to prevent them from developing further psychopathology.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Jonathan A. Oler, Alexander J. Shackman, Do P.M. Tromp, Richard J. Davidson, Brad Postle, Wen Li, and Rick Jenison for their comments on early versions of the manuscript. They thank Steven E. Shelton, Helen VanValkenberg, Marissa Riedel, and the staff at the Harlow Center for Biological Psychology, the HealthEmotions Research Institute, the Waisman Center, the Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging and Behavior, and the Wisconsin National Primate Center.

References

1.
Stein MB, Stein DJ: Social anxiety disorder. Lancet 2008; 371:1115–1125
2.
Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE: Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:617–627
3.
Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE: Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:593–602
4.
Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS: A review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1568–1578
5.
Fox NA, Henderson HA, Marshall PJ, Nichols KE, Ghera MM: Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annu Rev Psychol 2005; 56:235–262
6.
Kessler RC, Ruscio AM, Shear K, Wittchen H-U: Epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 2010; 2:21–35
7.
Clauss JA, Blackford JU: Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: a meta-analytic study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012; 51:1066–1075.e1
8.
Rhesus Macaque Genome Sequencing and Analysis Consortium et al: Evolutionary and biomedical insights from the rhesus macaque genome. Science 2007; 316:222–234
9.
Wallis JD: Cross-species studies of orbitofrontal cortex and value-based decision-making. Nat Neurosci 2012; 15:13–19
10.
Scarr S, Salapatek P: Patterns of fear development during infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 1970; 16:53–90
11.
Schaffer HR: The onset of fear of strangers and the incongruity hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1966; 7:95–106
12.
Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N: Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science 1988; 240:167–171
13.
Biederman J, Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Rosenbaum JF, Hérot C, Friedman D, Snidman N, Kagan J, Faraone SV: Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1673–1679
14.
Chronis-Tuscano A, Degnan KA, Pine DS, Perez-Edgar K, Henderson HA, Diaz Y, Raggi VL, Fox NA: Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009; 48:928–935
15.
Essex MJ, Klein MH, Slattery MJ, Goldsmith HH, Kalin NH: Early risk factors and developmental pathways to chronic high inhibition and social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:40–46
16.
Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Biederman J, Henin A, Faraone SV, Davis S, Harrington K, Rosenbaum JF: Behavioral inhibition in preschool children at risk is a specific predictor of middle childhood social anxiety: a five-year follow-up. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2007; 28:225–233
17.
Prior M, Smart D, Sanson A, Oberklaid F: Does shy-inhibited temperament in childhood lead to anxiety problems in adolescence? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:461–468
18.
Schwartz CE, Snidman N, Kagan J: Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:1008–1015
19.
Fox NA, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Calkins SD, Schmidt LA: Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Dev 2001; 72:1–21
20.
Hirshfeld DR, Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Bolduc EA, Faraone SV, Snidman N, Reznick JS, Kagan J: Stable behavioral inhibition and its association with anxiety disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1992; 31:103–111
21.
Pfeifer M, Goldsmith HH, Davidson RJ, Rickman M: Continuity and change in inhibited and uninhibited children. Child Dev 2002; 73:1474–1485
22.
Beesdo K, Bittner A, Pine DS, Stein MB, Höfler M, Lieb R, Wittchen H-U: Incidence of social anxiety disorder and the consistent risk for secondary depression in the first three decades of life. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:903–912
23.
Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA: Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:1033–1039
24.
Gladstone GL, Parker GB: Is behavioral inhibition a risk factor for depression? J Affect Disord 2006; 95:85–94
25.
Battaglia M, Bajo S, Strambi LF, Brambilla F, Castronovo C, Vanni G, Bellodi L: Physiological and behavioral responses to minor stressors in offspring of patients with panic disorder. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31:365–376
26.
Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Gersten M, Hirshfeld DR, Meminger SR, Herman JB, Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N: Behavioral inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and agoraphobia: a controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:463–470
27.
Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Kagan J, Snidman N, Friedman D, Nineberg A, Gallery DJ, Faraone SV: A controlled study of behavioral inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and depression. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:2002–2010
28.
Jardine R, Martin NG, Henderson AS: Genetic covariation between neuroticism and the symptoms of anxiety and depression. Genet Epidemiol 1984; 1:89–107
29.
Martin N, Goodwin G, Fairburn C, Wilson R, Allison D, Cardon LR, Flint J: A population-based study of personality in 34,000 sib-pairs. Twin Res 2000; 3:310–315
30.
Olvera RL, Bearden CE, Velligan DI, Almasy L, Carless MA, Curran JE, Williamson DE, Duggirala R, Blangero J, Glahn DC: Common genetic influences on depression, alcohol, and substance use disorders in Mexican-American families. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2011; 156B:561–568
31.
Tambs K, Czajkowsky N, Røysamb E, Neale MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Aggen SH, Harris JR, Ørstavik RE, Kendler KS: Structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for dimensional representations of DSM-IV anxiety disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195:301–307
32.
Dilalla LF, Kagan J, Reznick JS: Genetic etiology of behavioral inhibition among 2-year-old children. Infant Behav Dev 1994; 17:405–412
33.
Goldsmith HH, Gottesman II: Origins of variation in behavioral style: a longitudinal study of temperament in young twins. Child Dev 1981; 52:91–103
34.
Plomin R, Dunn J (eds): The Study of Temperament: Changes, Continuities, and Challenges. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1986
35.
Robinson JL, Kagan J, Reznick JS, Corley R: The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: a twin study. Dev Psychol 1992; 28:1030–1037
36.
Kalin NH, Shelton SE: Defensive behaviors in infant rhesus monkeys: environmental cues and neurochemical regulation. Science 1989; 243:1718–1721
37.
Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ: Defensive behaviors, fear, and anxiety, in Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience, vol 17, Handbook of Anxiety and Fear. Edited by Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Griebel G, Nutt D. New York, Elsevier, 2008, pp 63–79 (http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1569733907000057)
38.
Davidson RJ, Kalin NH, Shelton SE: Lateralized response to diazepam predicts temperamental style in rhesus monkeys. Behav Neurosci 1993; 107:1106–1110
39.
Kalin NH, Shelton SE, Rickman M, Davidson RJ: Individual differences in freezing and cortisol in infant and mother rhesus monkeys. Behav Neurosci 1998; 112:251–254
40.
Fox AS, Shelton SE, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH: Trait-like brain activity during adolescence predicts anxious temperament in primates. PLoS ONE 2008; 3:e2570
41.
Oler JA, Fox AS, Shelton SE, Rogers J, Dyer TD, Davidson RJ, Shelledy W, Oakes TR, Blangero J, Kalin NH: Amygdalar and hippocampal substrates of anxious temperament differ in their heritability. Nature 2010; 466:864–868
42.
Rogers J, Shelton SE, Shelledy W, Garcia R, Kalin NH: Genetic influences on behavioral inhibition and anxiety in juvenile rhesus macaques. Genes Brain Behav 2008; 7:463–469
43.
Schwartz CE, Wright CI, Shin LM, Kagan J, Rauch SL: Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grown up”: adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science 2003; 300:1952–1953
44.
Schwartz CE, Kunwar PS, Greve DN, Kagan J, Snidman NC, Bloch RB: A phenotype of early infancy predicts reactivity of the amygdala in male adults. Mol Psychiatry 2012; 17:1042–1050
45.
Pérez-Edgar K, Roberson-Nay R, Hardin MG, Poeth K, Guyer AE, Nelson EE, McClure EB, Henderson HA, Fox NA, Pine DS, Ernst M: Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents. Neuroimage 2007; 35:1538–1546
46.
Blackford JU, Avery SN, Shelton RC, Zald DH: Amygdala temporal dynamics: temperamental differences in the timing of amygdala response to familiar and novel faces. BMC Neurosci 2009; 10:145
47.
Clauss JA, Cowan RL, Blackford JU: Expectation and temperament moderate amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex responses to fear faces. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2011; 11:13–21
48.
Blackford JU, Avery SN, Cowan RL, Shelton RC, Zald DH: Sustained amygdala response to both novel and newly familiar faces characterizes inhibited temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2011; 6:621–629
49.
Blackford JU, Allen AH, Cowan RL, Avery SN: Amygdala and hippocampus fail to habituate to faces in individuals with an inhibited temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 8:143–150
50.
Clauss JA, Seay AL, Vanderklok RM, Avery SN, Cao A, Cowan RL, Benningfield MM, Blackford JU: Structural and functional bases of inhibited temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (Epub ahead of print, March 11, 2014)
51.
Blackford JU, Clauss JA, Avery SN, Cowan RL, Benningfield MM, VanDerKlok RM: Amygdala-cingulate intrinsic connectivity is associated with degree of social inhibition. Biol Psychol 2014; 99:15–25
52.
Paxinos G: The Rhesus Monkey Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 2nd ed. Boston, Academic Press, 2009
53.
Swanson LW, Petrovich GD: What is the amygdala? Trends Neurosci 1998; 21:323–331
54.
Etkin A, Wager TD: Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1476–1488
55.
Whalen PJ, Kagan J, Cook RG, Davis FC, Kim H, Polis S, McLaren DG, Somerville LH, McLean AA, Maxwell JS, Johnstone T: Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science 2004; 306:2061
56.
McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Parrish JM, Adler A, Blair RJR, Fromm S, Charney DS, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, Pine DS: Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:97–106
57.
Fox AS, Oakes TR, Shelton SE, Converse AK, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH: Calling for help is independently modulated by brain systems underlying goal-directed behavior and threat perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005; 102:4176–4179
58.
Kalin NH, Shelton SE, Fox AS, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ: Brain regions associated with the expression and contextual regulation of anxiety in primates. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:796–804
59.
Shackman AJ, Fox AS, Oler JA, Shelton SE, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH: Neural mechanisms underlying heterogeneity in the presentation of anxious temperament. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110:6145–6150
60.
Eatough EM, Shirtcliff EA, Hanson JL, Pollak SD: Hormonal reactivity to MRI scanning in adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34:1242–1246
61.
Fox AS, Oler JA, Shelton SE, Nanda SA, Davidson RJ, Roseboom PH, Kalin NH: Central amygdala nucleus (Ce) gene expression linked to increased trait-like Ce metabolism and anxious temperament in young primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109:18108–18113
62.
Adolphs R: The biology of fear. Curr Biol 2013; 23:R79–R93
63.
Terburg D, Morgan BE, Montoya ER, Hooge IT, Thornton HB, Hariri AR, Panksepp J, Stein DJ, van Honk J: Hypervigilance for fear after basolateral amygdala damage in humans. Transl Psychiatr 2012; 2:e115
64.
van Honk J, Eisenegger C, Terburg D, Stein DJ, Morgan B: Generous economic investments after basolateral amygdala damage. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110:2506–2510
65.
Kennedy DP, Gläscher J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R: Personal space regulation by the human amygdala. Nat Neurosci 2009; 12:1226–1227
66.
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio AR: The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature 1998; 393:470–474
67.
Tranel D, Gullickson G, Koch M, Adolphs R: Altered experience of emotion following bilateral amygdala damage. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2006; 11:219–232
68.
Feinstein JS, Adolphs R, Damasio A, Tranel D: The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Curr Biol 2011; 21:34–38
69.
Chudasama Y, Izquierdo A, Murray EA: Distinct contributions of the amygdala and hippocampus to fear expression. Eur J Neurosci 2009; 30:2327–2337
70.
Machado CJ, Bachevalier J: Behavioral and hormonal reactivity to threat: effects of selective amygdala, hippocampal, or orbital frontal lesions in monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008; 33:926–941
71.
Murray EA, Izquierdo A: Orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala contributions to affect and action in primates. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1121:273–296
72.
Emery NJ, Capitanio JP, Mason WA, Machado CJ, Mendoza SP, Amaral DG: The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav Neurosci 2001; 115:515–544
73.
Machado CJ, Emery NJ, Capitanio JP, Mason WA, Mendoza SP, Amaral DG: Bilateral neurotoxic amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): consistent pattern of behavior across different social contexts. Behav Neurosci 2008; 122:251–266
74.
Kalin NH, Shelton SE, Davidson RJ: The role of the central nucleus of the amygdala in mediating fear and anxiety in the primate. J Neurosci 2004; 24:5506–5515
75.
Kalin NH, Shelton SE, Davidson RJ: Role of the primate orbitofrontal cortex in mediating anxious temperament. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:1134–1139
76.
Fox AS, Shelton SE, Oakes TR, Converse AK, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH: Orbitofrontal cortex lesions alter anxiety-related activity in the primate bed nucleus of stria terminalis. J Neurosci 2010; 30:7023–7027
77.
Birn RM, Shackman AJ, Oler JA, Williams LE, McFarlin DR, Rogers GM, Shelton SE, Alexander AL, Pine DS, Slattery MJ, Davidson RJ, Fox AS, Kalin NH: Evolutionarily conserved prefrontal-amygdalar dysfunction in early-life anxiety. Mol Psychiatry (Epub ahead of print, May 27, 2014)
78.
Davis M, Whalen PJ: The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry 2001; 6:13–34
79.
Fanselow MS, Dong H-W: Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 2010; 65:7–19
80.
Kjelstrup KG, Tuvnes FA, Steffenach H-A, Murison R, Moser EI, Moser M-B: Reduced fear expression after lesions of the ventral hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:10825–10830
81.
Walker DL, Toufexis DJ, Davis M: Role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis versus the amygdala in fear, stress, and anxiety. Eur J Pharmacol 2003; 463:199–216
82.
Davis M, Walker DL, Miles L, Grillon C: Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35:105–135
83.
Tye KM, Prakash R, Kim S-Y, Fenno LE, Grosenick L, Zarabi H, Thompson KR, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K: Amygdala circuitry mediating reversible and bidirectional control of anxiety. Nature 2011; 471:358–362
84.
Felix-Ortiz AC, Beyeler A, Seo C, Leppla CA, Wildes CP, Tye KM: BLA to vHPC inputs modulate anxiety-related behaviors. Neuron 2013; 79:658–664
85.
Kim S-Y, Adhikari A, Lee SY, Marshel JH, Kim CK, Mallory CS, Lo M, Pak S, Mattis J, Lim BK, Malenka RC, Warden MR, Neve R, Tye KM, Deisseroth K: Diverging neural pathways assemble a behavioural state from separable features in anxiety. Nature 2013; 496:219–223
86.
Viviani D, Charlet A, van den Burg E, Robinet C, Hurni N, Abatis M, Magara F, Stoop R: Oxytocin selectively gates fear responses through distinct outputs from the central amygdala. Science 2011; 333:104–107
87.
Dias BG, Ressler KJ: Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nat Neurosci 2014; 17:89–96
88.
Zhang T-Y, Parent C, Weaver I, Meaney MJ: Maternal programming of individual differences in defensive responses in the rat. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2004; 1032:85–103
89.
Zhang TY, Labonté B, Wen XL, Turecki G, Meaney MJ: Epigenetic mechanisms for the early environmental regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression in rodents and humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013; 38:111–123
90.
Roseboom PH, Nanda SA, Fox AS, Oler JA, Shackman AJ, Shelton SE, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH: Neuropeptide Y receptor gene expression in the primate amygdala predicts anxious temperament and brain metabolism. Biol Psychiatry (Epub ahead of print, Nov 13, 2013)
91.
Ma DK, Jang M-H, Guo JU, Kitabatake Y, Chang M-L, Pow-Anpongkul N, Flavell RA, Lu B, Ming G-L, Song H: Neuronal activity-induced Gadd45b promotes epigenetic DNA demethylation and adult neurogenesis. Science 2009; 323:1074–1077
92.
Lyons DM, Buckmaster PS, Lee AG, Wu C, Mitra R, Duffey LM, Buckmaster CL, Her S, Patel PD, Schatzberg AF: Stress coping stimulates hippocampal neurogenesis in adult monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107:14823–14827
93.
Sabatini MJ, Ebert P, Lewis DA, Levitt P, Cameron JL, Mirnics K: Amygdala gene expression correlates of social behavior in monkeys experiencing maternal separation. J Neurosci 2007; 27:3295–3304
94.
Turner CA, Watson SJ, Akil H: The fibroblast growth factor family: neuromodulation of affective behavior. Neuron 2012; 76:160–174
95.
Evans SJ, Choudary PV, Neal CR, Li JZ, Vawter MP, Tomita H, Lopez JF, Thompson RC, Meng F, Stead JD, Walsh DM, Myers RM, Bunney WE, Watson SJ, Jones EG, Akil H: Dysregulation of the fibroblast growth factor system in major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004; 101:15506–15511
96.
Perez JA, Clinton SM, Turner CA, Watson SJ, Akil H: A new role for FGF2 as an endogenous inhibitor of anxiety. J Neurosci 2009; 29:6379–6387
97.
Turner CA, Clinton SM, Thompson RC, Watson SJ Jr, Akil H: Fibroblast growth factor-2 (FGF2) augmentation early in life alters hippocampal development and rescues the anxiety phenotype in vulnerable animals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108:8021–8025
98.
Perera TD, Coplan JD, Lisanby SH, Lipira CM, Arif M, Carpio C, Spitzer G, Santarelli L, Scharf B, Hen R, Rosoklija G, Sackeim HA, Dwork AJ: Antidepressant-induced neurogenesis in the hippocampus of adult nonhuman primates. J Neurosci 2007; 27:4894–4901
99.
Sahay A, Hen R: Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nat Neurosci 2007; 10:1110–1115
100.
Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, Weisstaub N, Lee J, Duman R, Arancio O, Belzung C, Hen R: Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 2003; 301:805–809
101.
Aimone JB, Deng W, Gage FH: Resolving new memories: a critical look at the dentate gyrus, adult neurogenesis, and pattern separation. Neuron 2011; 70:589–596
102.
Sahay A, Wilson DA, Hen R: Pattern separation: a common function for new neurons in hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron 2011; 70:582–588
103.
Chareyron LJ, Banta Lavenex P, Amaral DG, Lavenex P: Stereological analysis of the rat and monkey amygdala. J Comp Neurol 2011; 519:3218–3239

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1162 - 1173
PubMed: 25157566

History

Received: 4 April 2014
Revision received: 6 June 2014
Accepted: 16 June 2014
Published online: 1 November 2014
Published in print: November 01, 2014

Authors

Details

Andrew S. Fox, Ph.D.
From the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, the HealthEmotions Research Institute, and the Waisman Center for Brain Imaging and Behavior, University of Wisconsin, Madison.
Ned H. Kalin, M.D.
From the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, the HealthEmotions Research Institute, and the Waisman Center for Brain Imaging and Behavior, University of Wisconsin, Madison.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Kalin ([email protected]).

Funding Information

National Institute of Mental Health10.13039/100000025: R21MH91550, R01MH81884, R01MH46729, P50MH84051, MH100031, R21MH09258
Dr. Fox reports no financial relationships with commercial interests. Dr. Kalin has served on scientific advisory boards for Corcept Therapeutics, Neuronetics, CeNeRx BioPharma, and Skyland Trail; is a stockholder with equity options in Corcept Therapeutics and CeNeRx BioPharma; owned Promoter Neurosciences; and holds patents for promoter sequences for corticotropin-releasing factor CRF2alpha and a method of identifying agents that alter the activity of the promoter sequences, promoter sequences for urocortin II and the use thereof, and promoter sequences for corticotropin-releasing factor binding protein and the use thereof.Supported by NIH Intramural Research Program and extramural grants R21MH91550, R01MH81884, R01MH46729, P50MH84051, MH100031, R21MH09258, the HealthEmotions Research Institute, and Meriter Hospital.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share