Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: 1 February 2000

Impaired Auditory Gating and P50 Nonsuppression Following Traumatic Brain Injury

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) can produce persistent attention and memory impairment that may in part be produced by impaired auditory sensory gating. The P50 evoked waveform response to paired auditory stimuli appears to be a useful measure of auditory gating. The first controlled measurement of the P50 ratio in TBI patients is described: when 20 patients with persistently symptomatic TBI were compared with 20 control subjects, the P50 ratio was significantly greater in the TBI group. The potential neurophysiologic and therapeutic implications of this finding in TBI patients who report symptoms consistent with impaired auditory gating are discussed.
Traumatic brain injury (TBI) affects approximately 200 per 100,000 persons annually in the United States1 and may produce significant cognitive morbidity among many TBI survivors.28 Attention and memory impairments are generally considered predictable and ubiquitous sequelae of significant TBI,912 and affected TBI patients often describe exacerbation of these impairments in settings of multiple sensory stimuli. Difficulty focusing and sustaining attention in the presence of competing stimuli is sometimes described by clinicians as distractibility. However, this term as it is often used may encompass at least two forms of attentional impairment. Distractibility is commonly used to describe a rapid shifting of robust selective attention between competing stimuli for brief periods of time (i.e., impaired concentration). Although distractible patients are not able to sustain their attention for long, their ability to select items from a noisy background on which to briefly focus their attention is relatively intact.13,14
By contrast, many patients with attentional impairment after TBI report difficulty in mounting robust selective attention to any stimulus, a problem compounded by the presence of multiple competing stimuli. The problem does not appear to be distractibility per se, but rather an inability to filter out, or “gate,” irrelevant sensory information,1517 a phenomenon we describe as impaired auditory sensory gating.
To make clear the distinction between distractibility and impaired sensory gating: patients with either problem often describe difficulty in conversing with the television on in the background, reading a list in a noisy supermarket, or responding to an interviewer's questions in a room with a noisy fan. When describing this experience in detail, distractible patients will offer a description of each of the distractors in some detail, and they are often able to remember and recount the content of these distractions. For example, while having a conversation in a noisy restaurant, distractible patients may say that they heard part of the conversation with their dinner partner, part of the conversation at the next table, and part of the music being played, and will often be able to relate the details of each part of these events. In other words, although their attention shifts among competing stimuli, they are able to attend to, encode, and later recall the content of the competing stimuli.
Patients with impaired auditory sensory gating also describe their experience in a noisy environment as overstimulating, but they have difficulty recounting the details of the stimuli—they are more apt to say there was “too much going on,” and they are unable to recount in any detail more than the mere presence of the stimuli. Their experience is one of both divided and incomplete attention to all of the stimuli. A metaphor we have found useful for describing the experience of impaired auditory gating is that of listening to a radio receiving two programs on one frequency, resulting in difficulty distinguishing and remembering the information presented by either.
It is plausible that a portion of the attention and memory impairments of persistently symptomatic TBI patients is due to impaired sensory gating,1821 since attention is at least in part dependent on such gating mechanisms to filter out irrelevant or excessive sensory inputs.22 Such sensory gating impairments may be relatively imperceptible to the usual types of testing performed in the practitioner's quiet examination room (e.g., the Mini-Mental State Examination23), but nonetheless may present the patient with significant difficulties with attention and memory in more usual, and often noisier, environments. However, sensory gating impairments can be identified via electrophysiologic means, including the P50 ratio of the evoked response to paired auditory stimuli (P50 ratio).
The P50 ratio, an electrophysiologic metric based on comparison of the middle latency auditory cortical evoked potential occurring 50 milliseconds after presentation of paired auditory stimuli presented 500 milliseconds apart, is believed to reflect auditory gating.22,24,25 Ordinarily when paired auditory stimuli are presented in this fashion, a cortical response is evoked after the first stimulus and an attenuated response is evoked by the second stimulus. A ratio of these responses may be calculated by comparing the amplitude of the P50 evoked by the second or test stimulus with that evoked by the first stimulus. The P50 ratio reflects the action of cholinergically mediated inhibitory neuronal pathways in the hippocampus.26,27 When these inhibitory pathways are impaired, patients demonstrate abnormally large P50 ratios—that is, the second evoked response is not attenuated (Figure 1).
Patients with schizophrenia and symptoms consistent with impaired auditory gating demonstrate a significantly increased P50 ratio compared to normal control subjects.22,25,28,29 These findings have been interpreted as electrophysiologic evidence of persistently disturbed hippocampal cholinergic functioning, an interpretation supported by similar P50 nonsuppression findings in animal models with cholinergic, particularly nicotinic, dysfunction in the hippocampus.30 In humans, observations of P50 suppression in a previously nonsuppressing subject following nicotine22,28,31or physostigmine22 administration also support a cholinergic hypothesis of auditory sensory gating.20,32,33
Auditory sensory gating appears to depend on cholinergic systems, and in particular those acting at low-affinity alpha-7 nicotinic receptors found in the hippocampus.25,28,32,34 Illness or injury that produces dysfunction of these systems would be expected to produce impaired auditory gating. Because TBI has been shown to damage hippocampal and other neocortical cholinergic systems3537 via diffuse axonal injury,10,3840 damage to cholinergic neurons,3537,4143 and/or interference with the intracellular mechanisms involved in the synthesis and release of acetylcholine,44,45 patients with symptoms consistent with impaired auditory gating following TBI would be expected to be nonsuppressors of the P50.
Other electrophysiologic studies have suggested that TBI may produce chronic abnormalities on quantitative electroencephalographic,5,4648 evoked potential,4952 and event-related potential5356 recordings. The P50 ratio may also reflect persistent neurophysiologic disturbance following TBI and might be particularly useful as a marker of both a specific symptom (i.e., impaired auditory sensory gating) and neurophysiological abnormality (i.e., disturbed cholinergic function).
We observed and reported this association between clinical complaints consistent with impaired auditory gating and P50 nonsuppression in three TBI patients seen by the first author.18 These first three patients varied in level of TBI severity (one each of mild, moderate, and severe) but shared the complaint of impaired auditory sensory gating, prompting speculation that this association might hold for any TBI patient with this symptom. The present study was therefore undertaken to determine whether the phenomenon of P50 nonsuppression could be observed in a larger sample of TBI patients with symptoms of impaired auditory gating. This report describes the first systematic and controlled application of the P50 paradigm to the study of symptomatic TBI patients.

METHODS

Subjects

Subjects were recruited by a combination of direct referral from the first author's neuropsychiatry clinic at our university hospital, referrals from colleagues in the community, and responses to advertisement in a local newspaper. Recruitment efforts solicited the participation of patients who had experienced a TBI at least 1 year ago and subsequently developed symptoms of impaired attention and memory. Potential subjects were initially interviewed by telephone and were excluded if they had any history of psychiatric or neurologic illness prior to TBI, seizures, or alcohol or substance abuse/dependence, or if they were taking medications known to affect measures of auditory gating.57
TBI patients passing the initial telephone interview were subsequently interviewed by the first author. Subjects qualified for participation if they had experienced a definable TBI (nonpenetrating; not requiring neurosurgical intervention; posttraumatic amnesia [PTA] of at least 15 minutes, with or without loss of consciousness) at least 1 year prior to interview and described symptoms consistent with impaired auditory gating. Severity of injury was operationally defined by duration of PTA and was graded as mild (15 min–1 hr), moderate (1–24 hr), or severe (>24 hr)9 on the basis of patient/family interview and/or record review. This method of assigning TBI severity was chosen because estimation of duration of PTA was more consistently available than Glasgow Coma Scale58 scores. A Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)23,59 was performed, and subjects were included only if performance was at or above the 25th percentile. The performance of the MMSE was intended to disqualify patients with dementia due to TBI, thereby improving likelihood of historical accuracy generally and with regard to the specific symptoms in question and to better ensure meaningful informed consent and cooperative participation. Severity of persistent disability due to TBI was assessed with the Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS).60 Additionally, all subjects were interviewed using DSM-IV61 criteria to better characterize pre- and post-TBI psychiatric history. Subjects with any definable Axis I disorder prior to their TBI and any active Axis I mood or anxiety disorder at the time of P50 recording were disqualified from participation.
The first 20 subjects (15 women, 5 men) meeting these criteria were enrolled into the study after giving written informed consent, in accordance with the requirements of the Institutional Review Board. Twenty control subjects were selected from a previously acquired normal control population and were included only if they had no history of TBI, had no psychiatric illness known to affect P50 suppression, were on no medications known to affect P50 suppression, and were known to have P50 ratios in the normal range. TBI and control subjects were matched by gender and as closely as possible by age.
The mean age of the TBI subjects was 42.2 years (SD=11.3), and the mean age of the control subjects was 40.4 years (SD=11.0; difference not significant). Characteristics of the overall TBI group are summarized in Table 1. There were no differences between the TBI subgroups with respect to time elapsed since initial injury, GOS score, or MMSE score.

Recording Technique

Auditory evoked potentials were assessed according to our previously described methods.28 Hearing threshold (in decibels) was determined by subjective recognition of the stimuli used in this paradigm at multiple sound pressure levels. Evoked potential recordings were performed with the subject alert, supine, and with eyes fixed on a distant target. EEG activity was recorded on a GRASS Model 78 electroencephalograph with model 7P511 preamplifiers, using a gold disk electrode at the vertex, referenced to one ear. The electro-oculogram (EOG) was simultaneously recorded between the right superior orbit and lateral canthus. The EEG was amplified 20,000 times with bandpass (–50%) between 1 and 300 Hz and was digitized at 100 Hz for acquisition and averaging by using proprietary software on an 80486 IBM-compatible computer. Individual trials were rejected if the EOG or EEG was greater than 50 μV, as such readings indicate movement artifact. The EEG was also monitored for signs of alpha wave activity or other slow waves, and trials demonstrating prominent alpha activity or slowing were rejected.
Auditory stimuli were presented in pairs in a conditioning-testing design with a 0.5-second intrapair interval and a 10-second interstimulus interval. A pulse stimulus approximately 1 ms in duration, amplified with a bandwidth between 20 and 12,000 Hz, was delivered through headphones. The intensity at the subject's ear was set at 30 to 45 dB above hearing threshold. Three sets of average responses were constructed, each based on responses to 16 pairs of stimuli, and were used to create an overall grand average of the conditioning and test P50 responses for each subject.
The P50 wave was identified and measured in each set of averages by using a previously described computer algorithm.25 The averages were digitally filtered with a 7-point low-pass smoothing routine and a recursive high-pass filter to narrow the bandpass to 10–250 Hz. Each filter was applied twice, in both forward and reverse directions, to increase roll-off and to preserve waveform latency. The algorithm identified the conditioning P50 wave as the most positive peak between 40 and 80 ms after the first stimulus. P50 amplitude was measured relative to the preceding negativity, and a conditioning wave amplitude of at least 0.5 μV was required. The test P50 wave was identified as the most positive peak with a latency from the test stimulus within 10 ms of the latency of the conditioning P50 response, as established in a previous study.25 The amplitude of the test P50 wave divided by the amplitude of the conditioning P50 wave, expressed as a percentage and referred to here as the P50 ratio, was used as a measure of auditory gating.

Statistical Analyses

Statistica software (Statsoft; Tulsa, OK) was used for all statistical analyses. One-tailed t-tests of independent samples were used to test for differences in demographic variables, P50 latency, and P50 conditioning and test amplitudes between TBI and control subjects. One-tailed methods were chosen because prior clinical observations of the P50 in patients with TBI suggested that P50 values were likely to vary only in the direction of decreased suppression.18 Bonferroni-corrected Pearson product-moment correlations were calculated between demographic and evoked potential variables.
To test for differences in P50 ratios between the TBI and control groups, we used the nonparametric Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. This very conservative method was chosen because it is possible that these groups may represent two different populations (with and without TBI, with and without impaired auditory gating symptoms) and because the general shape of the P50 ratio distributions differed (control subjects clustered between 0 and 20, roughly a truncated normal distribution). Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed to test for differences in demographic variables between each TBI-severity subgroup and control subjects, as well as to test for differences in P50 latency, conditioning and test amplitudes, and P50 ratio. MANOVA was determined to be an acceptable method for this analysis because although the P50 ratio is nonparametrically distributed, this difference derives from alteration most akin to a change in skewness and is therefore unlikely to have a problematic influence on F-value. Post hoc planned comparisons to test for differences in demographic and evoked potential variables between the TBI subgroups were performed by using Tukey's honest significant difference (HSD) test for unequal sample sizes.

RESULTS

Means and standard deviations for all P50 variables are reported in Table 2. The P50 ratio was significantly greater in the overall TBI group (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, P<0.001; Figure 2) than in the control group. The conditioning amplitude was significantly smaller (t=–2.66, P=0.012) and the test amplitude was significantly larger (t=5.97, P<0.001) in the TBI group compared with the control group. Mean latency did not differ significantly between the overall TBI and control groups (t=0.13, P=0.89). Bonferroni-corrected correlations revealed no significant relationships between any of the demographic variables and evoked potential variables in the TBI subjects.
MANOVA demonstrated a significant effect on P50 ratio by group (F=23.8, df=3,36, P<0.001; Figure 3). Post hoc comparisons revealed significant P50 ratio differences between each TBI subgroup and the control group (P<0.001 for each comparison) but did not reveal significant P50 ratio differences between the TBI subgroups.
MANOVA demonstrated a significant effect on test amplitude by group (F=12.3, df=3,36, P<0.001); post hoc comparisons revealed a trend toward a difference between the mild TBI and control groups (P<0.09) and significant differences between the moderate TBI and control groups (P<0.03) and the severe TBI and control groups (P<0.001), but no differences between the TBI subgroups (Figure 4).
MANOVA suggested a trend toward an effect on conditioning amplitude by group (F=2.48, df=3,36, P=0.08), with a smaller conditioning amplitude among the TBI groups. Post hoc comparison of conditioning amplitude by group revealed no significant differences between the TBI subgroups and the control group or between the TBI subgroups.
MANOVA revealed no significant effects on demographic variables by group.

DISCUSSION

The present study observed significantly increased P50 ratios among patients who presented with symptoms consistent with impaired auditory gating following TBI. The current subject group was studied because they offered complaints consistent with impaired auditory gating, among many other cognitive complaints, and it was suspected that these complaints might reflect abnormal P50 suppression. This difference in P50 ratio was observed both between the entire TBI group and the control group, as well as between each TBI subgroup (mild, moderate, and severe) and the control group. Given the results of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test of P50 ratio differences between the overall TBI group and the control group, and the MANOVA and post hoc comparisons of effects on P50 ratio by group, the likelihood of a Type 1 error (false positive) in these results is less than 1 in 1,000. Consequently, P50 nonsuppression among TBI patients with symptoms consistent with impaired auditory gating appears to be a robust finding among patients with mild, moderate, and severe TBI, and it distinguished TBI patients with this symptom from normal control subjects regardless of the level of TBI severity.
The difference in P50 ratio between the TBI and control groups appears to be the result of both a decrease in the conditioning amplitude and an increase in the test amplitude in the TBI groups. The MANOVA analysis of conditioning amplitude by group suggests that the decrease in conditioning amplitude may be a somewhat less robust effect than the increase in test amplitude.
Planned post hoc comparisons of the effects of group on P50 conditioning amplitude, test amplitude, and P50 ratio, using Tukey's HSD test for unequal sample sizes, found no significant differences among the three TBI subgroups based on level of TBI severity. However, although the statistical analysis we used was designed to accommodate the unequal and relatively small sample sizes of the individual TBI subgroups, it is possible that these samples provide inadequate power to find a difference if one exists. Increasing the sample size of each TBI subgroup in subsequent studies is necessary to evaluate more definitively the relationship between level of initial TBI severity and P50 electrophysiology. However, the present data preliminarily suggest that similar levels of P50 nonsuppression may be observed in TBI patients with symptoms of impaired auditory gating despite disparate levels of injury severity and that this phenomenon does not appear to be associated only with severe injuries.
The hippocampus is particularly vulnerable to injury from acceleration/deceleration and rotational forces to which the brain is subjected during TBI.9,36,38,41,42,62 Because the hippocampus appears to be the structure most strongly related to auditory gating and P50 suppression,22 and because reports describing P50 nonsuppression in schizophrenia22,25,63,64 have suggested that hippocampal cholinergic abnormalities substantially contribute to this finding, P50 nonsuppression among TBI patients with symptoms of impaired auditory gating may be consistent with a hypothesis of impaired hippocampal cholinergic function.18 Although reticulothalamic51,6569 and/or reticulofrontal49,69,70 cholinergic abnormalities may also be contributory, the relative contribution of each system to P50 electrophysiology and to impaired auditory sensory gating has not yet been fully clarified. Nonetheless, the present finding supports the general hypothesis of cholinergic dysfunction in the development of P50 nonsuppression and impaired auditory gating following TBI.
Transient normalization of P50 suppression among patients with schizophrenia has been observed in response to cholinergic augmentation.22,28,31 Because this deficit is mediated by low-affinity alpha-7 nicotinic receptors, which rapidly depolarize in response to administration of nicotine, the treatment of impaired auditory gating by using cholinergic augmentation strategies with most currently available agents has been problematic.26,28,31 However, the recent development of centrally selective long-acting acetylcholinesterase inhibitors71 may make cholinergic augmentation strategies more feasible and potentially beneficial for TBI patients similar to those included in this study. Further, if speculations regarding the potentially deleterious impact of impaired sensory gating on attention, memory, and other aspects of cognition are correct, treatment of impaired sensory gating might be of significant benefit to affected TBI patients.
The present study is limited in its ability to clarify whether P50 nonsuppression specifically indexes impaired auditory gating following TBI or instead indexes significant TBI more generally. Not all TBI patients report symptoms of impaired auditory gating, and therefore not all could be expected to be P50 nonsuppressors. This study examined only patients with symptoms of impaired auditory gating, and thus the prevalence of P50 nonsuppression among TBI patients more generally remains uncertain. It also is not clear whether P50 nonsuppression followed or preceded TBI and what the effect of TBI may be with respect to producing impaired auditory sensory gating if the latter is the case. Additionally, this study cannot establish the strength of the relationship between neuropsychological impairments and these electrophysiologic measures. Because this study was intended only to investigate whether P50 nonsuppression might be observed in TBI patients with complaints consistent with impaired auditory gating, formal neuropsychological testing has not been performed on all of these subjects and is therefore not available to further assist interpretation of these findings. Finally, although the speculation about hippocampal cholinergic dysfunction as the foundation for these symptoms is appealing, at present there are neither structural imaging nor pharmacologic manipulation data to more clearly support such speculations.
Studies designed to address each of these issues are clearly needed to more fully establish the validity of the current finding, the relationship of this finding to neuropsychological and structural neuroimaging variables, and the impact of treatment with cholinergic augmentation on symptoms, neuropsychological performance, and cerebral electrophysiology in TBI patients.
FIGURE 1. Examples of P50 evoked response to paired auditory stimuli: P50 suppression (top) and nonsuppression (bottom)To the left of each set of tracings is information on the channel recorded (Channel 1=vertex), amplitude and latency of conditioning and test responses, and the P50 ratio (test/conditioning×100). Amplitude (vertical axis) is expressed in μV, and latency (horizontal axis) is expressed in milliseconds. In each pair of tracings, the left window illustrates the average conditioning response and the right window illustrates the average test response. The P50 evoked response is demarcated in each window by tick marks.
FIGURE 2. Box-whisker plot of P50 ratio differences between the traumatic brain injury (TBI) and control groups, using the nonparametric Kolmogorov-Smirnov testTBI group in this plot includes subjects with mild, moderate, and severe TBI and demonstrates a significant difference (P<0.001) in P50 ratio between the groups.
FIGURE 3. Box-whisker plot of the effect of group on P50 ratio using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), demonstrating a significant effectPlanned post hoc comparisons using Tukey's honest significant difference test for unequal sample sizes demonstrates significant differences between each TBI subgroup and the control group, and no statistically significant differences between the TBI subgroups.
FIGURE 4. Box-whisker plot of effect of group on P50 test amplitude using MANOVA, demonstrating a significant effectPlanned post hoc comparisons using Tukey's honest significant difference test for unequal sample sizes demonstrates significant differences between the moderate and severe TBI subgroups and the control group, and no statistically significant differences between the TBI subgroups.
TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics for the overall traumatic brain injury group and each TBI subgroup
TABLE 2. Means and standard deviations of P50 findings for the control group, overall traumatic brain injury (TBI) group, and TBI subgroups

Footnote

This work was supported by a Postdoctoral Fellowship in Neuroscience and Traumatic Brain Injury, Office of Academic Affairs, Department of Veterans Affairs (D.A.); the National Institute of Mental Health (Grants MH47476, MH15442, MH56601, MH35468 to M.R., MH01121 and MH50787 to L.E.A.); and Pfizer, Inc. (J.T.).

References

1.
Kraus JF, Sorenson SB: Epidemiology, in Neuropsychiatry of Traumatic Brain Injury, edited by Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Hales RE. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 3–41
2.
Kraus JF, Nourjah P: The epidemiology of mild, uncomplicated brain injury. J Trauma 1988; 28:1637–1643
3.
Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Hales RE (ed): Neuropsychiatry of Traumatic Brain Injury. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994
4.
Rimel RW, Giordani B, Barth JT, et al: Disability caused by minor head injury. Neurosurgery 1981; 9:221–228
5.
Watson MR, Fenton GW, McClelland RJ, et al: The post-concussional state: neurophysiologic aspects. Br J Psychiatry 1995; 167:514–521
6.
Levin HS, Mattis SRRM, et al: Neurobehavioral outcome following minor head injury: a three-center study. J Neurosurg 1987; 66:234–243
7.
Rutherford WH, Merret JD, McDonald JR: Sequelae of concussion caused by minor head injuries. Lancet 1977; i:1–4
8.
Rutherford WH, Merret JD, McDonald JR: Symptoms at one year following concussion from minor head injuries. Injury 1979; 10:225–230
9.
Strub RL, Black FW (ed): Neurobehavioral Disorders: A Clinical Approach. Philadelphia, PA, FA Davis, 1988
10.
Filley CM (ed): Neurobehavioral Anatomy. Niwot, CO, University Press of Colorado, 1995
11.
McAllister TW: Mild traumatic brain injury and the postconcussive syndrome, in Neuropsychiatry of Traumatic Brain Injury, edited by Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Hales RE. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 357–392
12.
Lovell MR, Franzen MD: Neuropsychological assessment, in Neuropsychiatry of Traumatic Brain Injury, edited by Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Hales RE. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 133–160
13.
Rund BR, Zeiner P, Sundet K, et al: No vigilance deficit found in either young schizophrenic or ADHD subjects. Scand J Psychol 1998; 39:101–107
14.
Seidman LJ, Biederman J, Weber W, et al: Neuropsychological function in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:260–268
15.
Gualtieri CT: Pharmacotherapy and the neurobehavioral sequelae of traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 1988; 2:101–129
16.
Gualtieri CT, Evans RW: Stimulant treatment for the neurobehavioral sequelae of traumatic brain injury. Clin Neuropharmacol 1988; 12:273–290
17.
Lezak MD: Subtle sequelae of brain damage: perplexity, distractibility and fatigue. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 1978; 57:9–15
18.
Arciniegas D, Adler L, Topkoff J, et al: Attention and memory dysfunction after traumatic brain injury: cholinergic mechanisms, sensory gating, and a hypothesis for further investigation. Brain Inj 1999; 13:1–13
19.
O'Shanick GJ, O'Shanick AM: Personality and intellectual changes, in Neuropsychiatry of Traumatic Brain Injury, edited by Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Hales RE. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 163–188
20.
Stevens KE, Nagamoto H, Johnson RG, et al: Kainic acid lesions in adult rats as a model of schizophrenia: changes in auditory information processing. Neuroscience 1998; 82:701–708
21.
Venables P: Input dysfunction in schizophrenia, in Progress in Experimental Personality Research, edited by Maher BA. Orlando, FL, Academic Press, 1964, pp 1–47
22.
Freedman R, Adler LE, Bickford P, et al: Schizophrenia and nicotinic receptors. Harv Rev Psychiatry 1994; 2:179–192
23.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198
24.
Harris JG, Adler LE, Young DA, et al: Neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1996; 20:253–260
25.
Nagamoto HT, Adler LE, Waldo MC, et al: Sensory gating in schizophrenics and normal controls: effects of changing stimulation intervals. Biol Psychiatry 1989; 25:549–561
26.
Adler LE, Olincy A, Waldo M, et al: Schizophrenia, sensory gating, and nicotinic receptors. Schizophr Bull 1998; 24:189–202
27.
Adler LE, Freedman R, Ross RG, et al: Elementary phenotypes in the neurobiological and genetic study of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (in press)
28.
Adler LE, Hoffer LD, Wiser A, et al: Normalization of auditory physiology by cigarette smoking in schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1856–1861
29.
Clementz BA, Geyer MA, Braff DL: Poor P50 suppression among schizophrenia patients and their first-degree biological relatives. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:1691–1694
30.
Luntz-Leybman V, Bickford P, Freedman R: Cholinergic gating of response to auditory stimuli in rat hippocampus. Brain Res 1992; 587:130–136
31.
Adler LE, Hoffer LJ, Griffith J, et al: Normalization by nicotine of deficient auditory sensory gating in the relatives of schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry 1992; 32:607–616
32.
Bickford P, Nagamoto HT, Johnson R, et al: Auditory sensory gating in hippocampal neurons: a model system in the rat. Biol Psychiatry 1990; 27:183–192
33.
Stevens KE, Wear KD: Normalizing effects of nicotine and a novel nicotinic agonist on hippocampal auditory gating in two animal models. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1997; 57:869–874
34.
Johnson MR, Adler LE: Transient impairment in P50 auditory sensory gating induced by a cold-pressor test. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 33:380–387
35.
DeAngelis MM, Hayes RL, Lyeth BG: Traumatic brain injury causes a decrease in M2 muscarinic cholinergic receptor binding in the rat brain. Brain Res 1994; 653:39–44
36.
Dixon CE, Bao J, Bergmann JS, et al: Traumatic brain injury reduces hippocampal high-affinity [3H]choline uptake but not extracellular choline levels in rats. Neurosci Lett 1994; 180:127–130
37.
Dixon CE, Hamm RJ, Taft WC, et al: Increased anticholinergic sensitivity following closed skull impact and controlled cortical impact traumatic brain injury in the rat. J Neurotrauma 1994; 11:275–287
38.
Cassidy JW: Neuropathology, in Neuropsychiatry of Traumatic Brain Injury, edited by Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Hales RE. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 43–79
39.
Alexander MP: Traumatic brain injury, in Psychiatric Aspects of Neurologic Disease, 2nd edition, edited by Benson DF, Blumer E. New York, Grune and Stratton, 1982, pp 219–248
40.
Teasdale G, Mendelow D: Pathophysiology of head injuries, in Closed Head Injury: Psychological, Social, and Family Consequences, edited by Brooks N. New York, Oxford University Press, 1984, pp 4–36
41.
Dixon CE, Bao J, Johnson KM, et al: Basal and scopolamine-evoked release of hippocampal acetylcholine following traumatic brain injury in rats. Neurosci Lett 1995; 198:111–114
42.
Dixon CE, Ma X, Marion DW: Reduced evoked release of acetylcholine in the rodent neocortex following traumatic brain injury. Brain Res 1997; 749:127–130
43.
Saija A, Robinson SE, Lyeth BG, et al: The effects of scopolamine and traumatic brain injury on central cholinergic neurons. J Neurotrauma 1988; 5:161–170
44.
Murdoch I, Perry EK, Court JA, et al: Cortical cholinergic dysfunction after human head injury. J Neurotrauma 1998; 15:295–305
45.
Ciallella JR, Yan HQ, Ma X, et al: Chronic effects of traumatic brain injury on hippocampal vesicular acetylcholine transporter and M2 muscarinic receptor protein in rats. Exp Neurol 1998; 152:11–19
46.
Thatcher RW, Walker RA, Gerson I, et al: EEG discriminant analyses of mild head trauma. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1989; 73:94–106
47.
Thatcher RW, Cantor DS, McAllister R, et al: Comprehensive predictions of outcome in closed head-injured patients: the development of prognostic equations. Ann NY Acad Sci 1991; 620:82–101
48.
Randolph C, Miller MH: EEG and cognitive performance following closed head injury. Neuropsychobiology 1988; 20:43–50
49.
Gutling E, Gonser A, Imhof HG, et al: Prognostic value of frontal and parietal somatosensory evoked potentials in severe head injury: a long-term follow up study. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994; 92:568–570
50.
Gupta NK, Verma NP, Guidice MA, et al: Visual evoked response in head trauma: pattern-shift stimulus. Neurology 1986; 36:578–581
51.
Jabbari B, Vance SC, Harper MG, et al: Clinical and radiological correlates of somatosensory evoked potentials in the late phase of head injury: a study of 500 Vietnam veterans. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987; 67:289–297
52.
Fenton GW: The postconcussional syndrome reappraised. Clin Electroencephalogr 1996; 27:174–182
53.
Papanicolaou AC, Levin HS, Eisenberg HM, et al: Evoked potential correlates of posttraumatic amnesia after closed head injury. Neurosurgery 1984; 14:676–678
54.
Olbrich HM, Nau HE, Zerbin D, et al: Clinical application of event related potentials in patients with brain tumors and traumatic brain injuries. Acta Neurochirurgica 1986; 80:116–122
55.
Pratap-Chand R, Sinniah M, Salem FA: Cognitive evoked potential (P300): a metric for cerebral concussion. Acta Neurol Scand 1988; 78:185–189
56.
Curry SH: Event-related potentials as indicants of structural and functional damage in closed head injury. Prog Brain Res 1980; 54:507–515
57.
Nagamoto HT, Adler LE, Hea RA, et al: Gating of auditory P50 in schizophrenics: unique effects of clozapine. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:181–188
58.
Teasdale G, Jennett B: Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: a practical scale. Lancet 1974; 2: 81–84
59.
Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, et al: Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and education level. JAMA 1993; 269:2386–2391
60.
Jennett B, Bond M: Assessment of outcome after severe brain damage: a practical scale. Lancet 1975; i:480–484
61.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1994
62.
Dixon CE, Ma X, Marion DW: Effects of CDP-choline treatment on neurobehavioral deficits after TBI and on hippocampal and neocortical acetylcholine release. J Neurotrauma 1997; 14:161–169
63.
Boutros NN, Zouridakis G, Overall J: Replication and extension of P50 findings in schizophrenia. Clin Electroencephalogr 1991; 22:40–45
64.
Boutros N, Torello MW, Burns EM, et al: Evoked potentials in subjects at risk for Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatry Res 1995; 57:57–63
65.
Buchwald JS, Erwin RJ, Read S, et al: Midlatency auditory evoked responses: differential abnormality of P1 in Alzheimer's disease. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1989; 74:378–384
66.
Garcia-Rill E, Biedermann JA, Chambers T, et al: Mesopontine neurons in schizophrenia. Neuroscience 1995; 66:321–335
67.
Frith CD, Friston KJ: The role of the thalamus in “top down” modulation of attention to sound. Neuroimage 1996; 4:210–215
68.
Guillery RW, Feig SL, Lozsadi DA: Paying attention to the thalamic reticular nucleus. Trends Neurosci 1998; 21:28–32
69.
Breese CR, Adams C, Logel J, et al: Comparison of the regional expression of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor alpha-7 mRNA and 125 alpha-bungarotoxin binding in human postmortem brain. J Comp Neurol 1997; 387:385–398
70.
Rizzo PA, Pierelli F, Pozzessere G, et al: Subjective posttraumatic syndrome: a comparison of visual and brain stem auditory evoked responses. Neuropsychobiology 1983; 9:78–82
71.
Rogers SL, Farlow MR, Doody RS, et al: A 24-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1997; 50:136–145

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 77 - 85
PubMed: 10678517

History

Published online: 1 February 2000
Published in print: February 2000

Authors

Details

David Arciniegas, M.D.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.
Ann Olincy, M.D.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.
Jeannie Topkoff, B.S.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.
Kara McRae, B.A.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.
Ellen Cawthra, R.N.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.
Christopher M. Filley, M.D.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.
Martin Reite, M.D.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.
Lawrence E. Adler, M.D.
Received January 11, 1999; revised May 6, 1999; accepted May 26, 1999. From the Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver; and the Research Service, Psychiatry Service, and Neurology Service, Denver Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado. Address correspondence to Dr. Arciniegas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268–28, 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Denver, CO 80262.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share