Skip to main content
Full access
REGULAR
Published Online: 1 October 2008

Neuropsychological Deficits in Huntington’s Disease Gene Carriers and Correlates of Early “Conversion”

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
S ince the discovery of the genetic mutation responsible for Huntington’s disease, 1 there have been many attempts to determine whether people who carry the mutation, but who are not yet clinically ill, can be distinguished from those without the mutation. Brain imaging studies have often demonstrated structural and/or functional abnormalities in the striatum in presymptomatic individuals. 27 In addition, subtle oculomotor and other movement peculiarities and neuropsychiatric changes have been reported in some mutation-positive individuals prior to the onset of clinical signs and symptoms sufficient for diagnosis. 812 However, the search for presymptomatic changes in cognition among those with the huntingtin mutation has met with mixed results. Several investigations reported no significant differences in neuropsychological functioning between those with and those without the Huntington’s disease mutation. 1317 Others, however, have found such differences. 810, 18 Deficits in reaction time, psychomotor and processing speed, 10, 18, 2022 executive functioning, 2325 spatial analysis and constructional praxis, 26 verbal fluency, 23 verbal memory, 2628 and mental arithmetic 9, 10 have all been reported among mutation carriers, albeit inconsistently.
The discrepant findings among studies are due, in large part, to methodological differences. First, studies have varied in the degree of neurologic abnormality tolerated in the “asymptomatic” mutation-positive group. 29 While some researchers enrolled only persons with no, or very minimal, nonspecific neurological signs, 22, 27 others included subjects with “major signs consistent with Huntington’s disease.” 8, 9, 20, 28 In addition, some studies were of young mutation carriers who were almost certainly many years from disease onset, whereas others were of older subjects nearing the age of likely clinical onset. In those studies where proximity to clinical onset was explicitly addressed, time to disease onset (“conversion”) was most often estimated. 10, 1517, 28 The present study is one of the very few reporting data from those who have been followed prospectively from asymptomatic status to diagnosable disease onset. 18, 22 Other factors likely contributing to discrepant research findings include differences in the variety and sensitivity of the specific cognitive tests employed and differences in statistical power to detect group differences (due primarily to sample size). 29
We sought to address the limitations of previous work by studying a relatively large group of individuals with the Huntington’s disease mutation who were neurologically healthy at first examinations, who were followed prospectively, and who received the clinical diagnosis of Huntington’s disease at least 2 years after their initial evaluations. We compared the baseline cognitive test performance of these people to that of mutation-positive individuals of equivalent age and education who remained clinically asymptomatic throughout the study period and to at-risk persons who tested negative for the Huntington’s disease mutation. We hypothesized that those who became symptomatic would exhibit greater cognitive impairment at baseline, especially on tests of motor speed, dexterity, and executive control, than those in the other two groups.
METHODS

Research Participants and Procedure

All participants in the current study underwent presymptomatic genetic testing for Huntington’s disease at the Baltimore Huntington’s Disease Research Center at the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine and were enrolled in prospective longitudinal studies tracking motor, psychological (cognitive, behavioral and emotional), and brain morphometric changes. The parent study from which these data were drawn was fully reviewed and approved by the Johns Hopkins Medicine Institutional Review Board. All participants gave their informed consent.
At-risk asymptomatic subjects who were first evaluated between February 1986 and January 2007 were eligible for inclusion in these analyses. All were required to have definitive DNA tests for the huntingtin mutation, complete neuropsychological examinations, and scores ≤10 on the Quantified Neurologic Exam 30 at baseline. The Quantified Neurologic Exam is a standardized assessment of the motor system developed specifically for Huntington’s disease, with higher scores reflecting more movement abnormalities. Scores of barely symptomatic, just-diagnosed patients are typically in the high teens to low 20s. 16, 17 Thus, the single-digit Quantified Neurologic Exam scores of our subjects, and the fact that expert diagnosticians (including AR, RLM, CAR) rated them asymptomatic, ensure that all participants in this study were clinically healthy at entry.
The above criteria were met by 237 people at risk. One hundred thirty-four subjects tested negative for the Huntington’s disease mutation (negative group), while 103 tested positive. Mutation-positive subjects were followed with (in most cases) peri-annual visits. At each visit, the Quantified Neurologic Exam was readministered, as were the battery of neuropsychological tests and several psychiatric and functional rating scales. Of the 103 mutation-positive subjects, only the 70 who were followed for at least 3 years as of June 2007 are included in the analyses reported here; this ensures that they had at least some opportunity to “convert” (i.e., develop diagnosable signs of Huntington’s disease). Of these individuals, 21 in fact converted (converter group) and 49 remained unaffected (nonconverter group) as of June 2007.

Neuropsychological Tests

At each visit, subjects were administered the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), 31 Vocabulary and Block Design subtests of the WAIS-Revised (WAIS-R), 32 Grooved Pegboard Test, 33 Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT, written trial), 34 Standardized Road-Map Test of Directional Sense, 35 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, 36 Brief Test of Attention, 37 Stroop Color-Word Test, 38 and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). 39 This longitudinal study was initiated before the third edition of the WAIS (WAIS-III) and the revised version of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test were published.

Statistical Analyses

Baseline demographic, neurologic, and neuropsychological test data of the converter, nonconverter, and negative groups were compared with analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by post hoc analyses of pair-wise differences using the least significant difference test. Proximity to clinical onset was determined for the converter group to be the time between baseline examination and the first follow-up visit at which a confident diagnosis was made. The converter group was then divided into “early” and “late” subgroups based on median time to onset, and the neuropsychological test performance of these groups was compared to that of the nonconverter group.

RESULTS

The converter, nonconverter, and negative groups were equivalent in age, education, and sex distribution ( Table 1 ). As expected, the groups differed in CAG repeat length (p<0.001), with the converter and nonconverter groups having more CAG repeats than the negative group (p<0.001). The difference in CAG repeat length between converters and nonconverters did not reach statistical significance (p=0.06). Most importantly, all the Quantified Neurologic Exam scores were in the normal range (by design), and neither the difference among the three groups nor the difference between the converters and nonconverters was significant.
TABLE 1. Baseline Characteristics of Mutation-Positive Participants Who Developed Huntington’s Disease (Converted), Those Who Did Not, and Mutation-Negative Comparison Subjects
For the converter and nonconverter groups, proximity to disease onset was predicted from data available at baseline—namely, CAG repeat length and parental age of onset. 15 These two groups of mutation carriers did not differ significantly in predicted time to onset, nor did they differ in the length of time they had been followed at the time of these analyses. Subjects in the converter group had actual onset of Huntington’s disease an average of 7.94 years after their baseline visit (SD=4.89, range=1.82–18.28 years); their mean predicted time to onset was 7.12 years (SD=4.61, range=−2.57–15.06 years). These values are not statistically different from each other (p=0.44). The Pearson correlation ( r ) between actual and predicted time to onset was 0.68 (p=0.001). The mean Quantified Neurologic Exam score at the first “symptomatic” assessment for the converter subjects was 15.00 (SD=5.34).
There were no significant differences among the three groups in baseline performance of any of the 10 neuropsychological tests (many tests yielding multiple measures). The largest effect size (η 2 partial =0.029) was on time to complete the Standardized Road-Map Test (F 2,113 =2.40, p=0.094).
The converter group was divided into those who developed Huntington’s disease soon after their baseline visit (early subgroup) and those who converted later (late subgroup), using the median time to onset of 8.62 years. The early, late, and nonconverter groups did not differ in mean age or education ( Table 2 ). There was a small difference among the groups in mean baseline Quantified Neurologic Exam score, but difference between the early subgroup and late subgroup converters did not reach significance (p=0.07). Examination of the baseline neuropsychological performance of these three groups of participants, all of whom have the Huntington’s disease genetic mutation, revealed significant differences (p<0.01) only on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test ( Figure 1 ). The early converters differed from both the late converters and the nonconverter groups (who did not differ from each other) in number of sorts achieved (F 2,65 =9.17, η 2 partial =0.220, p<0.001) and in efficiency score (cards used divided by sorts achieved) (F 2,65 =6.53, η 2 partial =0.167, p<0.001). They also differed in the number of both perseverative errors (F 2,65 =6.50, η 2 partial =0.167, p=0.003) and nonperseverative errors (F 2,65 =4.96, η 2 partial =0.132, p=0.01).
TABLE 2. Baseline Characteristics of Mutation-Positive Subjects Who Converted Early, Those Who Converted Late, and Those Who Remained Asymptomatic
FIGURE 1. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Results for Huntington’s Disease Early Converters, Late Converters, and Nonconverters
Left: Inefficiency scores for patients who developed symptoms within 8.62 years (early), after 8.62 years (late), and those who remained asymptomatic. For subjects who achieved no sorts, the denominator is set at 0.5 to allow computation of this summary score. Higher scores indicate worse performance. Means+SE. Right: Number of perseverative and nonperseverative errors.

DISCUSSION

Most previous studies that have reported cognitive impairment in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease have found it in mutation-positive persons who were judged to be close to onset. 17 While informative, studies using estimated years to onset provide less definitive evidence of preclinical cognitive changes than investigations of persons who actually convert to Huntington’s disease within a given period. To our knowledge, only two previous studies have followed asymptomatic mutation carriers prospectively until they have become clinically ill. Paulsen et al. 22 analyzed data from the 36-center Huntington’s Study Group and reported that 70 of 260 mutation-positive subjects converted to Huntington’s disease within 2 years of baseline evaluation. These participants performed more poorly on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test and all three trials of the Stroop Color-Word Test (but not on a verbal fluency test) than those who did not convert within 2 years. Notable limitations of this study are the very minimal neurocognitive data available (i.e., three timed tests) and the brief follow-up period. In particular, the fact that all participants converted within 2 years raises the possibility that some persons who were minimally symptomatic were inadvertently enrolled. Snowden et al. 18 compared 51 mutation-positive and 85 mutation-negative subjects on 10 tests, most with multiple outcome measures. Differences emerged only on simple tracing time on the Standardized Road-Map Test (i.e., not making left-right decisions), the color naming trial of the Stroop test, and an alternating hand movement test. These tasks are all described as psychomotor performance tasks that make minimal demands on higher-level cognition, thereby making them particularly sensitive to striatal dysfunction. Twenty-four of the 51 mutation-positives had follow-up assessments, and 15 of them converted to symptomatic at least 5 years after baseline. There were statistical trends for these 15 subjects to differ from the nine who remained unaffected on two variables from the Road-Map Test, time to complete the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Nelson modification), and time to complete a picture sequencing task. No mention is made of whether these close-to-onset and far-from-onset groups had equivalent Quantified Neurologic Exam scores at baseline and whether they were followed for the same length of time.
Our study extends the work of Paulsen and Snowden by studying demographically similar samples of asymptomatic mutation-positive and mutation-negative subjects, matched for Quantified Neurologic Exam score, and by comparing presymptomatic mutation-carriers who developed Huntington’s disease at least 2 years later (mean=7.9 years) to well-matched mutation carriers followed the same length of time but who remained unaffected. Under these conditions, we found no evidence of cognitive impairment associated with having the mutation or with converting during the study period. Although we administered many of the same tests as Snowden et al., 18 we relied on standardized administration. We did not employ the speed modifications Snowden and colleagues made to many tasks, which may have made them more sensitive. On the other hand, Snowden’s mutation-positive and mutation-negative groups differed significantly in baseline Quantified Neurologic Exam score, which may have contributed to the observed group differences in psychomotor performance.
When our converters were stratified into those who developed clinical signs within 8.62 years (the early group, with an average time to conversion of 3.72 years) and those who developed the illness after 8.62 years (the late group, with an average time to conversion of 11.78 years), significant differences were seen on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The early converters achieved fewer sorts, were less efficient (i.e., they required more cards to achieve the sorts they completed), and made more perseverative and nonperseverative errors.
Previous studies from our research group have demonstrated no significant cognitive impairments associated with the huntingtin mutation when subjects are more than 8 years from predicted disease onset. 15, 16 While no overall group differences were identified between the mutation-positive and mutation-negative groups we described in 2002, 17 a median split of the mutation-positive group on estimated time to onset (8.1 years) yielded significant group differences. Mutation-carriers who were closer to predicted illness onset (mean=4.0 years) performed significantly worse than those further from onset (mean=13.1 years) on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test, WAIS-R Block Design subtest, and all trials of the Stroop test, as well as the Standardized Road-Map Test of Directional Sense and nondominant hand performance on the Grooved Pegboard Test.
In the present study, the converter group was tested an average of 7.94 years before conversion, and the nonconverter group had a predicted time to onset of 9.25 years. Thus, both groups were relatively far from onset at baseline testing. The early converters were tested a mean of 3.72 years before disease onset, and only they displayed selective cognitive deficits on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Other research groups have identified executive dysfunction in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Lawrence and colleagues 23 reported findings similar to those described here in their study of persons with manifest Huntington’s disease, asymptomatic gene carriers, and mutation-negative comparison subjects. On a modified Tower of London task assessing planning and problem-solving, the gene carriers in this study achieved fewer perfect solutions and required a greater number of attempts to reach solutions than the mutation-negative comparison subjects. In another study, Lawrence et al. 24 reported more errors in attentional set-shifting (an aspect of executive functioning also measured by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) in asymptomatic gene carriers compared to mutation-negative subjects. Finally, difficulties with response inhibition have also been documented in preclinical huntingtin mutation carriers. 25
As we see them, the strengths of this study include objectively, operationally defined asymptomatic status of all participants; reliance on actual rather than estimated time to disease onset; extensive follow-up (e.g., some participants followed prospectively for more than 15 years); and relatively comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. Several limitations of the study are acknowledged. First, the number of converters (n=21) remains relatively small. Continued follow-up of our mutation-positive sample should yield additional cases over time. Second, the diagnosis of Huntington’s disease, and the determination of “conversion,” remains a clinical judgment and is subject to error. This is a limitation shared by all studies of the clinical emergence of Huntington’s disease. Finally, we relied heavily on standardized neuropsychological tests. The results of Snowden et al. 18 suggest that research paradigms that stress low-level, automatic, cognitive and motor routines may be more sensitive than clinical tests on the earliest brain changes in Huntington’s disease.
Our finding of a highly selective presymptomatic cognitive change among those “incubating” Huntington’s disease parallels recent reports of changes in brain morphology and functioning prior to clinical disease onset. Research from our group has revealed progressive decreases in the volume of the caudate nucleus on structural MRI starting 9–11 years prior to clinical onset. 2 Other structural changes have been reported in cerebral white matter 4041 and selected cortical regions. 3 Studies using PET imaging have revealed decreased activation at rest in the frontal and temporal cortex 40 as well as decreased D 2 receptor binding in the striatum. 42 Functional imaging studies with cognitive activation have found reduced activation in subcortical regions (caudate and thalamus), 4 as well as increased activation in medial prefrontal cortex 4 or decreased activation in the anterior cingulate cortex, 5 probably depending on the task. Such changes in the structure and function of fronto-striatal circuitry are consistent with the executive dysfunction demonstrated in our asymptomatic gene carriers.
In conclusion, this long-term prospective study of neurologically healthy persons with the Huntington’s disease mutation found that those who develop symptoms an average of 7.9 years after baseline assessment displayed no cognitive impairments. However, those who converted soon after baseline (within 8.6 years), as a group, performed more poorly on a problem-solving test requiring mental flexibility and concept formation than those who developed the disease later. This impairment is by no means sufficiently robust to be used in counseling individual cases, but it does support prior observations that the huntingtin mutation has phenotypic expression prior to frank disease onset.

Acknowledgments

J. Brandt receives royalty income from Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., on sales of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, the current version of one of the tests used in this study. Supported by grant #NS16375 from the National Institute on Neurological Disorders and Stroke and a Center of Excellence Award from the Huntington’s Disease Society of America.

Footnote

Received September 26, 2007; revised January 15, 2008; accepted January 31, 2008. The authors are affiliated with the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine in Baltimore; Drs. Brandt, Margolis, and Ross are also affiliated with the Department of Neurology and Dr. Ross is affiliated with the Department of Neurosciences at The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine. Address correspondence to Jason Brandt, Ph.D., The Johns Hopkins Hospital, 600 N. Wolfe St., Meyer 218, Baltimore, MD 21287-7218; [email protected] (e-mail).
Copyright © 2008 American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

References

1.
The Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Research Group: A novel gene containing a trinucleotide repeat that is expanded and unstable on Huntington’s disease chromosomes. Cell 1993; 72:971–983
2.
Aylward EH, Sparks BF, Field KM, et al: Onset and rate of striatal atrophy in preclinical Huntington disease. Neurology 2004; 63:66–72
3.
Rosas HD, Hevelone ND, Zaleta AK, et al: Regional cortical thinning in preclinical Huntington disease and its relationship to cognition. Neurology 2005; 65:745–747
4.
Paulsen JS, Zimbelman JL, Hinton SC, et al: fMRI biomarker of early neuronal dysfunction in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Am J Neuroradiol 2004; 25:1715–1721
5.
Reading S, Dziorny AC, Peroutka LA, et al: Functional brain changes in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Ann Neurol 2004; 55:879–883
6.
Reynolds NC Jr, Hellman RS, Tikofsky RS, et al: Single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT) in detecting neurodegeneration in Huntington’s disease. Nucl Med Commun 2002; 23:13–18
7.
Feigin A, Leenders KL, Moeller JR, et al: Metabolic network abnormalities in early Huntington’s disease: an 18 F fluorodeoxyglucose PET study. J Nucl Med 2001; 42:1591–1595
8.
Foroud T, Siemers E, Kleindorfer BS, et al: Cognitive scores in carriers of Huntington’s disease gene compared to noncarriers. Ann Neurol 1995; 37:657–664
9.
Kirkwood SC, Siemers E, Hodes ME, et al: Subtle changes among presymptomatic carriers of the Huntington’s disease gene. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000; 69:773–779
10.
Kirkwood SC, Siemers E, Stout JC, et al: Longitudinal cognitive and motor changes among presymptomatic Huntington disease gene carriers. Arch Neurol 1999; 56:563–568
11.
Blekher T, Johnson SA, Marshall J, et al: Saccades in presymptomatic and early stages of Huntington disease. Neurology 2006; 67:394–399
12.
Golding CV, Danchaivijitr C, Hodgson TL, et al: Identification of an oculomotor biomarker of preclinical Huntington disease. Neurology 2006; 67:485–487
13.
de Boo GM, Tibben A, Lanser JB, et al: Early cognitive and motor symptoms in identified carriers of the gene for Huntington’s disease. Arch Neurol 1997; 54:1353–1357
14.
de Boo GM, Tibben A, Hermans J, et al: Memory and learning are not impaired in presymptomatic individuals with an increased risk for Huntington’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1999; 21:831–836
15.
Campodonico JR, Codori AM, Brandt J: Neuropsychological stability over two years in asymptomatic carriers of the Huntington’s disease mutation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1996; 61:621–624
16.
Campodonico JR, Aylward E, Codori AM, et al: When does Huntington’s disease begin? J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1998; 4:467–473
17.
Brandt J, Shpritz B, Codori AM, et al: Neuropsychological manifestations of the genetic mutation for Huntington’s disease in presymptomatic individuals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002; 8:918–924
18.
Snowden JS, Craufurd D, Thompson J, et al: Psychomotor, executive, and memory function in preclinical Huntington’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002; 24:133–145
19.
Lemiere J, Decruyenaere M, Evers-Kiebooms G, et al: Cognitive changes in patients with Huntington’s disease (HD) and asymptomatic carriers of the HD mutation: a longitudinal follow-up study. J Neurol 2004; 251:935–942
20.
Witjes-Ané MN, Vegter-van der Vlis M, van Vugt JP, et al: Cognitive and motor functioning in gene carriers for Huntington’s disease: a baseline study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003; 15:7–16
21.
Lemiere J, Decruyenaere M, Evers-Kiebooms G, et al: Longitudinal study evaluating neuropsychological changes in so-called asymptomatic carriers of the Huntington’s disease mutation after 1 year. Acta Neurol Scand 2002; 106:131–141
22.
Paulsen JS, Zhao H, Stout JC, et al: Clinical markers of early disease in persons near onset of Huntington’s disease. Neurology 2001; 57:658–662
23.
Lawrence AD, Weeks RA, Brooks DJ, et al: The relationship between striatal dopamine receptor binding and cognitive performance in Huntington’s disease. Brain 1998; 121:1343–1355
24.
Lawrence AD, Hodges JR, Rosser AE, et al: Evidence for specific cognitive deficits in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Brain 1998; 121:1329–1341
25.
Farrow M, Churchyard A, Chua P, et al: Attention, inhibition, and proximity to clinical onset in preclinical mutation carriers for Huntington’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2007; 29:235–246
26.
Rosenberg NK, Sorensen SA, Christensen AL: Neuropsychological characteristics of Huntington’s disease carriers: a double blind study. J Med Genet 1995; 32:600–604
27.
Hahn-Barma V, Deweer B, Durr A, et al: Are cognitive changes the first symptoms of Huntington’s disease? A study of gene carriers. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998; 64:172–177
28.
Solomon AC, Stout JC, Johnson SA, et al: Verbal episodic memory declines prior to diagnosis in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:1767–1776
29.
Georgiou-Karistianis N, Smith E, Bradshaw JL, et al: Future directions in research with presymptomatic individuals carrying the gene for Huntington’s disease. Brain Res Bull 2003; 59:331–338
30.
Folstein SE, Jensen B, Leigh RJ, et al: The measurement of abnormal movement: methods developed for Huntington’s disease. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 1983; 5:605–609
31.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198
32.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York, Psychological Corporation, 1981
33.
Matthews C, Kløve H: Instruction Manual for the Adult Neuropsychology Test Battery. Madison, Wis, University of Wisconsin Medical School, 1964
34.
Smith A: Symbol Digit Modalities Test Manual. Los Angeles, Western Psychological Services, 1973
35.
Money J: Standardized Road-Map Test of Directional Sense Manual. San Rafael, Calif, Academic Therapy Publications, 1976
36.
Brandt J, Benedict RHB: The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised Professional Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 2001
37.
Schretlen D: Brief Test of Attention Professional Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1997
38.
Golden C: Stroop Color and Word Test Manual. Chicago, Stoelting, 1978
39.
Heaton RK: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1981
40.
Ciarmiello A, Cannella M, Lastoria S, et al: Brain white-matter volume loss and glucose hypometabolism precede the clinical symptoms of Huntington’s disease. J Nucl Med 2006; 47:215–222
41.
Reading SAJ, Yassa MA, Bakker A, et al: Regional white matter change in pre-symptomatic Huntington’s disease: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatr Res Neuroimaging 2005; 140:55–62
42.
van Oostrom JCH, Maguire RP, Verschuuren-Bemelmans CC, et al: Striatal dopamine D2 receptors, metabolism, and volume in preclinical Huntington disease. Neurology 2005; 65:941–943

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 466 - 472
PubMed: 19196932

History

Published online: 1 October 2008
Published in print: Fall, 2008

Authors

Details

Anjeli B. Inscore, Psy.D.
Barnett Shpritz, O.D., M.A.
Russell L. Margolis, M.D.
Christopher A. Ross, M.D., Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share