Skip to main content
Full access
Brief Report
Published Online: 1 December 1997

CSF 5-HIAA and Family History of Antisocial Personality Disorder in Newborns

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The relationship between genetic liability for antisocial behavior and CSF 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) in newborns was explored. METHOD: The authors assayed 5-HIAA in “leftover” CSF from 193 neurologically normal newborns and obtained family psychiatric histories of the newborns' first- and second-degree relatives. RESULTS: Levels of 5-HIAA were significantly lower in the infants with family histories of antisocial personality disorder than in the newborns without such family histories. CONCLUSIONS: These findings support the possibility that serotonin mediates one component of genetic liability to antisocial outcome, but the magnitude of that component may be less than what has been inferred from previously published reports. (Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1771–1773)
Astrong predictor of the development of antisocial behavior is antisocial personality disorder in one or both parents; this seems to be due to both environmental and genetic factors (1). Numerous studies of humans (clinical groups, aged 6 years to adult) (2) and nonhuman primates (3) have demonstrated associations between hostile-aggressive behavior and low CSF levels of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA). One way to help clarify the direction of causal influences in the relation between serotonin and aggression is to determine whether low levels of CSF 5-HIAA existing before the development of antisocial behavior might be associated with genetic risk for antisocial development. To our knowledge, this has never previously been studied in humans.
Heritability and stability of interindividual differences in CSF 5-HIAA have been demonstrated in nonhuman primates (3), but studies of humans (reviewed recently [4]) have been inconclusive. We previously demonstrated (4) that 5-HIAA could be reliably assayed from samples of “leftover” CSF obtained from febrile newborns (N=119) for whom meningitis was ruled out and that interindividual differences in 5-HIAA were stable over the first year of life (correlation between time 1 and time 2: r=0.64, N=31, p<0.01). For this study, we hypothesized that infants with family histories positive for antisocial personality disorder would have lower CSF 5-HIAA levels than those without such family histories. Since there are no known predisposing demographic factors for fever in babies, studies of cohorts of such infants provide an opportunity to measure CSF 5-HIAA within an epidemiologic sampling frame.

METHOD

Leftover portions of the second aliquot of CSF drawn from 646 febrile infants (age, birth to 3 months) in the emergency department at St. Louis Children's Hospital during the calendar year 1995 were assayed for 5-HIAA by using high-performance liquid chromatography (4). For four infants, leftover samples were also available from the first and fourth 0.75-ml aliquots drawn at lumbar puncture, and the samples showed significant gradient effects: the first aliquots had lower mean 5-HIAA levels than the fourth aliquots (t=–4.50, df=3, p=0.02). The medical records of all 646 infants were reviewed, and 350 infants were excluded from the study on the basis of neurologic abnormalities (4).
For the 296 remaining children, attempts were made to interview at least one parent during 1996. We reached 199 (67.2%) of the families, of whom 195 agreed to participate. The characteristics of these families were representative of the population of the St. Louis metropolitan area. The group of children whose families could not be reached differed from the group studied in terms of race (35% Caucasian versus 53% in the group that was reached) but not in terms of mean 5-HIAA level, gender, age at the time of lumbar puncture, or birth weight. After complete description of the study to the subjects' parents, written informed consent was obtained. We conducted the Family History Assessment Module (5) and the Denver Developmental Screening Test II (6) on the basis of the parents' reports. The validity of the Family History Assessment Module in screening for DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders was established in a study comparing family histories with direct diagnostic interviews of 2,654 individuals (5). Two children were excluded from the study because of developmental delays.

RESULTS

The prevalence rates for lifetime psychiatric disorders among the 2,338 adult first- and second-degree relatives of the 193 infants, as ascertained by the Family History Assessment Module, were as follows: antisocial personality disorder—2% alcoholism—4% drug abuse—2% major depression—5% lifetime history of suicide attempt—1%.
Linear regression analysis revealed that age, time of lumbar puncture, and having a parent with antisocial personality disorder independently predicted 5-HIAA level, as shown in table 1. Infants who had parents with antisocial personality disorder (N=16) had lower age-adjusted 5-HIAA levels (mean=735 pmol/ml, SD=167) than did infants with negative family histories of antisocial personality disorder (mean=827 pmol/ml, SD=210) (t=1.68, df=173, p=0.05, one-tailed). The mean level of the infants for whom antisocial personality disorder occurred exclusively among second-degree relatives (N=20) was intermediate between those of the other two groups (mean=805 pmol/ml, SD=250) (single-factor analysis of variance for all three groups: F=1.39, df=2,190, p=0.25). There was a low but statistically significant correlation (r=0.12, N=193, p<0.05) between CSF 5-HIAA level and degree of relatedness to the closest family member positive for antisocial personality disorder (i.e., distance, in number of meiotic divisions, to the closest relative with antisocial personality disorder). Seven infants in the lowest 10% of the distribution of age-adjusted 5-HIAA levels had first- or second-degree relatives with antisocial personality disorder, compared to two such relatives for the infants in the upper 10% of the distribution (Fisher's exact p=0.06). There was no significant difference in 5-HIAA level between the infants whose relatives with antisocial personality disorder were violent and the infants whose relatives with antisocial personality were not violent.
Separate linear regression analyses were subsequently performed for each disorder assessed on the Family History Assessment Module; there was no statistically significant association of 5-HIAA with family history of major depression, suicide, mania, schizophrenia, drug abuse, or alcoholism.

DISCUSSION

These data demonstrate a modest but statistically significant inverse association between family history of antisocial personality disorder and CSF 5-HIAA level in newborns. Although the data suggest that family history of antisocial personality disorder accounts for less than 5% of the variance in 5-HIAA levels in newborns, there are potential sources of error, including ascertainment of antisocial personality disorder by family history, reliability of the CSF sampling method, unassessed factors for which infants seen in emergency rooms for fever might not be representative of the population, and the possibility of dominant rather than additive genetic influences on 5-HIAA level. Any of these factors could have led to underestimation of the magnitude of genetic influences on the relation between 5-HIAA and antisocial personality disorder. One premise of the study is that heritable interindividual differences in CSF 5-HIAA are relatively preserved from birth to adulthood. It is possible that genetic factors are involved in individual-specific changes in 5-HIAA over the life course.
If one assumes relative stability of interindividual differences in 5-HIAA over the life course and if it is true that family history of antisocial personality disorder accounts for less than 5% of the variance in newborns' CSF 5-HIAA levels, these findings raise questions about causal relationships that have been inferred from associations between 5-HIAA and antisocial behavior in one-time samplings of violent individuals. Alternative explanations for the association would include 1) the possibility that environmental factors influence both 5-HIAA and social behavior in humans and 2) the likelihood that some of the genetic liability for antisocial behavior is mediated by factors other than the serotonin system.
TABLE 1

Footnote

Received Feb. 10, 1997; revision received July 15, 1997; accepted Aug. 25, 1997. From the Departments of Psychiatry and Pediatrics, Washington University School of Medicine, and the Laboratory of Clinical Science, NIMH, Bethesda, Md. Address reprint requests to Dr. Constantino, Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, 4940 Children's Place, St. Louis, MO 63110; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by NIMH grant MH-56317. The authors thank Ms. Donna Walck and Ms. Lisa Arce for assistance in securing CSF samples and Ms. Teresa Tolliver and Ms. Su Jan Huang for assistance with analysis of the CSF 5-HIAA.

References

1.
Cadoret RJ, Yatew WR, Troughton E, Woodworth G, Stewart MA: Genetic-environmental interaction in the genesis of aggressivity and conduct disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:916–924
2.
Virkkunen M, Goldman D, Nielsen DA, Linnoila M: Low brain serotonin turnover rate (low CSF 5-HIAA) and impulsive violence. J Psychiatry Neurosci 1995; 20:271–275
3.
Higley JD, King ST Jr, Hasert MF, Champoux M, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Stability of interindividual differences in serotonin function and its relationship to severe aggression and competent social behavior in rhesus macaque monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology 1996; 14:67–76
4.
Constantino JN, Murphy DL: Monoamine metabolites in “leftover” newborn human cerebrospinal fluid—a potential resource for biobehavioral research. Psychiatry Res 1996; 65:129–142
5.
Rice JP, Reich T, Bucholz KK, Neuman RJ, Fishman R, Rochberg N, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI, Schuckit MA, Begleiter H: Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995; 19:1018–1023
6.
Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B: The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics 1992; 89:91–97

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1771 - 1773
PubMed: 9396964

History

Published online: 1 December 1997
Published in print: December 1997

Authors

Details

John N. Constantino, M.D.
Jennifer A. Morris, B.A.
Dennis L. Murphy, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share