Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: 1 March 1999

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Identification in Disaster Workers

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Disaster workers who work with deceased victims are at increased risk of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Identification with the deceased has been proposed as one of the mechanisms in this stress-illness relationship. To examine this hypothesis, this study investigated three types of identification with the dead in a group of disaster workers: identification with the deceased as oneself, identification with the deceased as a friend, and identification with the deceased as a family member. METHOD: Fifty-four volunteer disaster workers who worked with the dead following an explosion on the USS Iowa naval ship were assessed 1, 4, and 13 months after the disaster. PTSD symptoms (measured with the DSMPTSD-IV scale), intrusive and avoidant disaster-related symptoms (measured with the Impact of Event Scale), somatization and general distress (measured with the SCL-90-R), and health care utilization were assessed. RESULTS: Disaster workers who reported identification with the deceased as a friend were more likely than those who did not to have PTSD, more intrusive and avoidant symptoms, and greater levels of other posttraumatic symptoms including somatization. Disaster workers who reported identification with the deceased as a family member had greater intrusive symptoms 1 month after the disaster than those who did not. There were no differences between those who did and did not identify with the deceased as self. Health care utilization was not associated with identification. CONCLUSIONS: Identification with the deceased is a risk factor for PTSD and posttraumatic symptoms in disaster workers exposed to the dead. Identification with the dead as a friend is specifically associated with higher risk for these workers.
Disaster workers are at risk of both acute and chronic posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (1). Perhaps the most difficult aspect of disaster work is exposure to violent death and dead bodies (2, 3) . Several early studies (4, 5) documented the highly stressful nature of working with the dead following a disaster. Two empirical investigations of emergency services personnel responding to the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake (6, 7) suggested that approximately 9% of the workers showed psychiatric symptoms at the level of those of an outpatient population. A study of body handlers (8) found that 11% had PTSD 3 months after their disaster work. Troops deployed in Operation Desert Storm who performed graves registration duties had increased posttraumatic stress symptoms (911) ; nearly one-half had current PTSD (11). Even professionals working with the dead have increased rates of posttraumatic stress symptoms (12) .
Disaster workers report that identification with the dead and their families is particularly stressful, including feelings of “It could have been me,” “It could have been my spouse,” or “It could have been my friend” (3, 13). Identification is a cognitive mechanism that has traditionally been seen as anxiety- and distress-reducing, as well as a normative aspect of cognitive development as children identify with their parents. However, identification may also be a risk factor for illness, serving as a cognitive mechanism by which symptoms are formed (14).
Identification has long been a focus of attention in the study of the psychiatric effects of disasters and traumatic events; however, it has not been empirically investigated. Identification with deceased victims is not unique to disaster workers. Identification with the deceased was described in bereaved victims of the Coconut Grove fire in Boston (15) and in community members after the crash of a commercial airliner in 1978 (16). Bereaved victims manifested traits and mannerisms of the deceased and imagined what their own last thoughts, feelings, and experiences would have been in the same situation. Such thoughts and feelings are different from previously described dissociative experiences in that they do not include any disturbance in time, person, or place (i.e., dissociation) and are not intrusive thoughts about the disaster, since the individuals were never involved in the disaster. Identification is a more complex cognition than intrusive thoughts and includes voluntary and involuntary active imagining.
Studies also suggest an increase in physical health problems and health care utilization associated with exposure to traumatic events and disaster (6, 17, 18). Higher rates of somatization and physical complaints have been found in disaster workers working with the dead (8) and Operation Desert Storm troops performing graves registration duties (11).
The present study extended our initial findings of exposure to traumatic death as a specific risk factor for PTSD by examining identification with the deceased as a mechanism for the development of acute and chronic PTSD, posttraumatic symptoms including somatization, and health care utilization. The subjects were volunteer disaster workers who participated in the body recovery and identification of sailors left dead after a gun turret explosion on the USS Iowa naval ship. These individuals were followed longitudinally during the year after the explosion and assessed 1, 4, and 13 months after the disaster.
On April 19, 1989, north of Puerto Rico, on the USS Iowa, gun turret 2 exploded. In the gun turret the crew had carried out their work surrounded by 17 inches of steel. The steel protection, designed to keep potential enemy fire out, entombed the men caught in the mishap. Of the 1,500 sailors on board, all 47 in the gun turret died. The bodies of the dead sailors were taken to Dover Air Force Base, Delaware, for identification, autopsy, and placement in caskets. Approximately 98 individuals from the Dover Air Force Base community quickly volunteered to assist the 28 professionals in the mortuary. These disaster workers were highly motivated and committed to their work, feeling that the victims were fellow members of the military. A total of 71 individuals actually assisted in the mortuary. Before the arrival of the bodies, all disaster workers received a briefing and orientation. The disaster workers moved the bodies through the identification process on gurneys and assisted in the autopsies, embalming, placement in caskets, and sorting of personal effects and in administrative duties. Most of the bodies were intact, with many still in their clothing; however, some were damaged beyond recognition. Nearly all of the disaster workers spent many hours surrounded by the dead, going home in the evening and returning to the mortuary the next day. The bodies were identified over a 3-day period, and the volunteers returned to their usual work after some time off. The research team was present throughout the 3-day period.

METHOD

Approximately 2 weeks after the mortuary work, we contacted the 71 volunteers who had worked in the mortuary. After the study was described, the volunteers who agreed to participate (N=54; 76% response rate) gave written informed consent and completed assessments. Those who agreed to participate in the study did not differ from those who did not. The median date for completion of the time 1 assessment was 1 month after the disaster. Of the 54 time 1 participants, 41 participated at time 2 (76% response rate), for which the median completion date was 4 months after the disaster. At time 3, 44 (81%) of the 54 participated, and the median completion date was 13 months after the disaster.
The majority of the subjects (91%, N=49) were male, and the mean age was 29 years (range=19–48 years). The majority were white (83%), were married (83%), were enlisted members of the U.S. Air Force (96%), and had at least some college education (83%). Approximately 48% of the subjects had previous disaster work experience. Individuals participating at 1, 4, and 13 months did not differ significantly on any demographic variables.

Measures

Identification with the dead.

Identification with the dead was assessed at 1 month with five items: “It could have been me;” “One of the victims reminded me of a close friend or relative;” “It could have been my spouse;” “It could have been my son;” and “It could have been my father.” Subjects scored each identification item on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Items were then recoded into dichotomous responses: no (not at all) and yes. The items for spouse, son, and father showed high internal consistency (Kuder-Richardson coefficient=0.82), and in chi-square analyses the three items were significantly associated (all p values <0.0001). Therefore, these three items were combined into one variable, identification–family (scored yes if any one item was scored yes). Thus, we examined three types of identification: identification–self, identification–friend, and identification–family. At 1 month, each subject was coded as an identifier (scored yes) or nonidentifier (scored no) separately for each of the three identification types.

The Impact of Event Scale (19).

This widely used 15-item self-report scale was used at the three assessment times to measure intrusive and avoidant posttraumatic symptoms. Standard scoring was used, with items scored on a 4-point scale, rescaled, and summed to yield scores for intrusive and avoidant symptoms and total symptoms. Horowitz et al. (19) found a correlation of 0.42 between the intrusion and avoidance subscales; however, the subscales did not measure identical constructs. Several studies (20, 21) found that the subscales discriminated across populations and detected change over time. In our study, intrusive and avoidant symptoms were measured retrospectively; however, the time interval was relatively brief (approximately 1.5 to 2 months after the disaster).

The SCL-90-R (22).

This scale was used at all three assessment times to determine other symptoms. This self-report checklist inquires about symptoms during the preceding week. It is composed of 90 items that are scored on a 5-point scale (0=not at all, 1=a little bit, 2=moderately, 3=quite a bit, 4=extremely). The SCL-90-R provides a global index of symptoms reported and intensity-of-distress scores for nine subscales: somatization, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, phobic anxiety, hostility, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism. Reliability coefficients for the subscales range from 0.84 to 0.90, and the subscales correlate fairly highly with MMPI scales measuring similar constructs. The SCL-90-R has proven useful in other research on disasters and has been administered to a variety of community samples. We examined only scores on the SCL-90-R global severity index and the subscales that might be affected by trauma: anxiety, depression, hostility, and somatization.

PTSD

We identified PTSD by means of the DSMPTSD-IV scale (8, 23 26). This scale uses the SCL-90-R, 12 supplemental items scored similar to those on the SCL-90-R, and the Impact of Event Scale to identify subjects who meet the DSM-IV criteria for PTSD symptom distribution. The DSMPTSD-IV scale has good sensitivity and specificity when compared with diagnoses from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV in community samples (26, 27).

Health care utilization

Health care utilization was assessed separately for three time periods: up to 1 month after the trauma, 1–4 months afterward, and 4–13 months afterward. A yes/no response was given for the following three questions: Have you obtained any medical care during this time for 1) an annual physical? 2) physical problems? 3) emotional or family problems?

Data Analyses

Initial t tests and chi-square tests determined between-group differences (identifiers versus nonidentifiers) in demographic variables for each of the three types of identification (identification–self, identification–friend, and identification–family). We calculated mean Impact of Event Scale and SCL-90-R scores for each of the three types of identification at each time point (1, 4, and 13 months after the disaster). For each identification type we performed repeated measures multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) to determine any differences due to time, differences between groups, or group-by-time interactions. MANOVA was also used on multiple outcomes simultaneously, i.e., on the Impact of Event Scale intrusion and avoidance subscales and on the SCL-90-R subscales (anxiety, depression, hostility, and somatization). When appropriate, we then used MANOVA and t tests to examine the differences between the two groups on the various subscales at the various times. We did not explore time effects when there was no group effect or group-by-time interaction, since they were not relevant to this study. To control for experimentwise error, when we examined PTSD and health care utilization for each of the identification types, we first examined the association of identifiers and nonidentifiers across all three times—using only the data of the subjects who were present for all three assessments—with chi-square analysis. If there was a significant finding, we then proceeded to examine each assessment point. Significance level was set at p=0.05. SAS software(28) was used for all statistical analyses.

RESULTS

Of the 54 disaster workers, 74% (N=40) were classified as identifier–self and 26% (N=14) as nonidentifier–self; 30% (N=16) as identifier–friend and 70% (N=38) as nonidentifier–friend; and 41% (N=22) as identifier–family and 59% (N=32) as nonidentifier–family.

Identification–Self

Subjects who identified with the deceased as self did not differ demographically (age, race, marital status, military rank, and education) from those who did not identify with the deceased as self (nonidentifiers).
We examined differences in Impact of Event Scale total score between the subjects who did and did not identify with the deceased as self (identifier–self versus nonidentifier–self) with the use of repeated measures MANOVA (table 1). No significant group or group-by-time effects were found, but there was a significant change over time (Wilks’s lambda=0.559; F=11.43, df=2, 29, p=0.0002). Using a repeated measures MANOVA to examine the intrusion and avoidance subscale scores, we found no significant group or group-by-time effects, but again there was a significant time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.540; F=5.74, df=2, 27, p=0.002). Further examination using a repeated measures MANOVA showed significant change over time for both intrusion (Wilks’s lambda=0.627; F=8.64, df=2, 29, p=0.001) and avoidance (Wilks’s lambda=0.616; F=9.05, df=2, 29, p=0.0009) and again no group or group-by-time effects.
There was no significant association between PTSD and subjects who did and did not identify with the deceased as self.
Repeated measures MANOVA showed no significant group, time, or group-by-time effects for the global severity index of the SCL-90-R (table 2). Similarly, repeated measures MANOVA showed no significant group, time, or group-by-time effects for the SCL-90-R subscales (anxiety, depression, hostility, and somatization).
There was no significant association between health care utilization and subjects who did and did not identify with the deceased as self.

Identification–Friend

Subjects who identified with the deceased as a friend were younger (mean age=25.1 years, SD=4.38) than those who did not identify with the deceased as a friend (mean=30.5 years, SD=6.92) (t=2.87, df=53, p=0.006). Chi-square analysis indicated a marginal difference in rank between identifiers and nonidentifiers (χ2=5.37, df=2, p=0.07): 37.5% (N=6) of the identifiers ranked E1–E3, 50.0% (N=8) ranked E4–E5, and 12.5% (N=2) ranked E6 or above, as compared with 36 of the nonidentifiers: 11.1% (N=4); 61.1% (N=22); and 27.8% (N=10), respectively. There were no other demographic differences between the two groups.
We examined differences in Impact of Event Scale total score between subjects who did and did not identify with the deceased as friend (identifier–friend versus nonidentifier–friend) by means of repeated measures MANOVA and found a significant group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.696; F=6.34, df=2, 29, p=0.005), group-by-time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.966; F=6.39, df=2, 29, p=0.005), and time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.330; F=9.37, df=1, 30, p=0.005) (figure 1). Those who identified with the deceased as a friend had higher total Impact of Event Scale scores 1 month after the disaster and 4 months after the disaster than did the nonidentifiers (table 1).
A repeated measures MANOVA for Impact of Event Scale intrusion and avoidance scores revealed a significant group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.762; F=5.40, df=2, 29, p=0.01), group-by-time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.684; F=3.12, df=4, 27, p=0.03), and time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.317; F=14.57, df=4, 27, p=0.0001) (figure 1). For intrusion, repeated measures MANOVA revealed a significant group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.945; F=11.13, df=1, 30, p=0.02), group-by-time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.972; F=5.81, df=2, 29, p=0.008), and time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.386; F=23.03, df=2, 29, p=0.0001). Identifiers had higher intrusion scores than did nonidentifiers 1 month after the disaster and 4 months afterward (table 1). For avoidance, repeated measures MANOVA showed a significant group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.981; F=6.43, df=1, 30, p=0.005), group-by-time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.977; F=3.66, df=2, 29, p=0.04), and time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.429; F=19.26, df=2, 29, p=0.0001). Identifiers had higher avoidance scores than did nonidentifiers 1 month after the disaster and 4 months afterward (table 1).
Of the subjects who identified with the deceased as a friend, 33% (N=3) had PTSD at sometime during the follow-up, compared with only 4.5% (N=1) of the nonidentifiers (χ 2=4.71, df=1, p=0.03). Given this finding, we examined the relationship of PTSD and identification–friend for each time separately. At 4 months, 27% (N=3) of the identifiers had PTSD, compared with 4% (N=1) of the nonidentifiers (χ 2=4.61, df=1, p=0.03). There were no differences in the proportions of subjects with PTSD at 1 month and 13 months.
A repeated measures MANOVA of the global severity index scores on the SCL-90-R showed a significant group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.672; F=15.10, df=1, 31, p=0.0005) (table 1). At 4 months, identifiers had significantly higher scores than nonidentifiers (table 2). No effects of time or group by time were present. Repeated measures MANOVA of the four SCL-90-R subscale scores (anxiety, depression, hostility, somatization) revealed a significant group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.533; F=5.90, df=4, 27, p=0.002). No group-by-time effect was present; however, there was a borderline effect of time (Wilks’s lambda=0.550; F=2.30, df=8, 23, p=0.06). A repeated measures MANOVA of each of the four subscale scores showed significant group effects for all four: anxiety (Wilks’s lambda=0.644; F=17.10, df=1, 31, p=0.0003), depression (Wilks’s lambda=0.779; F=8.79, df=1, 31, p=0.005), hostility (Wilks’s lambda=0.685; F=14.23, df=1, 31, p=0.0007), and somatization (Wilks’s lambda=0.712; F=12.12, df=1, 31, p=0.0012) (figure 2.). Examination of the means for anxiety shows that at 4 months, identifiers scored significantly higher than nonidentifiers (table 2). Similarly, the mean score on depression at 4 months was significantly higher for the identifiers than the nonidentifiers. On hostility the identifiers scored significantly higher than the nonidentifiers at 1 month and at 13 months. On somatization the identifiers’ scores were significantly higher than those of the nonidentifiers at 13 months. With respect to time effects, only the effect on somatization was significant (Wilks’s lambda=0.752; F=4.78, df=2, 30, p=0.02).
There was no significant association between health care utilization and subjects who did and did not identify with the deceased as a friend.

Identification–Family

The majority (95.4%, N=21) of the subjects who identified with the deceased as a family member were currently married. Of the nonidentifiers, 75.0% (N=24) were married (χ2=3.93, df=1, p=0.05). No other demographic variables were significantly different between the two groups.
A repeated measures MANOVA for Impact of Event Scale total score showed a borderline group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.885; F=3.90, df=1, 30, p=0.06) but no significant group-by-time effect (table 1); there was a significant effect of time (Wilks’s lambda=0.421; F=19.98, df=2, 29, p=0.0001). Repeated measures MANOVA showed no significant group or group-by-time effect for intrusion and avoidance modeled together, but as expected, there was a significant time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.411; F=9.68, df=4, 27, p=0.0001). Repeated measures MANOVA for avoidance showed no group or group-by-time effect, but there was a significant time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.493; F=14.89, df=2, 29, p=0.0001). For intrusion the group-by-time effect was not significant; however, there was a significant group effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.842; F=5.62, df=1, 30, p=0.02) and time effect (Wilks’s lambda=0.493; F=14.91, df=2, 29, p=0.0001). One month after the disaster, the scores on intrusion were higher for the subjects who reported identifying with the deceased as a family member than for those who did not (table 1).
Repeated measures MANOVA showed no significant group, time, or group-by-time effects on scores on the global severity index as well as the SCL-90-R subscales together (table 2).
There was no significant association between health care utilization and subjects who did and did not identify with the deceased as a family member.

DISCUSSION

Several researchers have observed an association between PTSD and identification with the deceased in disaster workers exposed to dead bodies (8, 13, 14, 29). This study is the first empirical examination of this observation. Importantly, subjects were exposed to only one aspect of disaster trauma, working with the dead. This provided the opportunity to examine exposure to traumatic death without the confounding effects of other disaster stressors. In addition, the subjects were followed longitudinally over the year after the disaster. The results of our study indicate that identification is both common and frequent, occurring in nearly 75% of disaster workers exposed to deceased victims. In addition, identification with the dead, particularly as a friend, is associated with higher rates of PTSD and greater intrusion, avoidance, and somatization, both acutely and over the long term.
Our findings are consistent with the notion that identification is not only a risk factor for negative outcomes but also a mechanism by which exposure to the dead leads to disease and symptoms in disaster workers. The longitudinal nature of this study decreases the possibility that the association of identification with negative outcome is a measurement effect, since identification was measured only at 1 month after the disaster yet predicted later outcomes. It is not clear, however, whether 1) exposure to deceased victims activates identification and subsequently the development of symptoms, 2) some individuals who are characteristically identifiers are a high-risk group for this particular type of exposure, or 3) identifiers are generally at risk of more psychiatric illness and symptoms regardless of exposure. The fact that all types of identification were not equally predictive of PTSD and other symptoms (i.e., identification with the victim as a friend was specifically associated with negative outcome) supports some specificity in the identification process rather than a global identifier trait. The fact that younger individuals were more likely to report identification with the deceased as a friend might also provide an explanation for the often reported finding that younger individuals exposed to death and trauma are at greater risk of negative outcomes (5, 30). Perhaps the specific recall of friends (i.e., “chumships”) is related to developmental experiences of friend formation in childhood. Certainly it highlights the often neglected role of friendships in adult development.
The study of the mechanisms by which somatic symptoms and health care utilization are related to PTSD is complicated by the known association of increased rates of PTSD among persons who are injured in a disaster or traumatic event (31 33) and the increased smoking (17) and accidental injury (34) associated with chronic PTSD. Since participants in our study had no injuries, were not exposed to other disaster stressors, and suffered primarily from acute PTSD, the finding of increased levels of somatization in the identification–friend group (but no increase in health care utilization) is of particular interest. The identifier group may have had an increased awareness of physical frailty and bodily health issues, either prior to or as a result of their exposure to deceased victims. Better measures of health care utilization may be helpful in identifying potential costs of somatization or clarifying the relationship between somatic complaints and health care utilization (18) in PTSD.
Several important limitations should be considered in interpreting the results of our study. A larger number of subjects would have been preferable in order to increase the level of confidence in our results. To generalize our findings to other disaster populations, a more diverse study group, allowing for examination of possible contributions of demographic variables, would also have been preferable. Although we used self-reports to assess both predictor and outcome variables, we used previously validated scales or scale items. Future studies should address the measurement of identification with the dead—both frequency and intensity—to further understand the relationship of identification with the deceased to PTSD and somatization.
Beyond these methodological considerations, this study suggests that the association of PTSD, disaster-related intrusive and avoidant symptoms, and general symptoms (including anxiety, depression, hostility, and somatization) with identification with the dead in disaster workers is prominent and mediated by the time since the disaster work and, importantly, by the type of identification (i.e., friend versus self and family). Further study of the causes of identification—psychological, contextual, and neurobiological—particularly identification–friend, are important to understanding this stress-illness process. If identification is a causative mechanism, interventions directed to decreasing identification should also decrease negative outcomes.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
FIGURE 1.Scores on the Impact of Event Scale 1, 4, and 13 Months After a Disaster for Disaster Workers Who Did and Did Not Identify With the Dead as a Friend
FIGURE 2.Scores on the SCL-90-R 1, 4, and 13 Months After a Disaster for Disaster Workers Who Did and Did Not Identify With the Dead as a Friend

Footnote

Received March 12, 1998; revision received July 28, 1998; accepted Aug. 25, 1998. From the Department of Psychiatry, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences. Address reprint requests to Dr. Ursano, Department of Psychiatry, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences, 4301 Jones Bridge Road, Bethesda, MD 20814

References

1.
McFarlane AC: Long-term psychiatric morbidity after a natural disaster. Med J Aust 1986; 145:561–563
2.
Raphael B: When Disaster Strikes: How Individuals and Communities Cope With Catastrophe. New York, Basic Books, 1986
3.
Ursano RJ, McCarroll JE: The nature of the traumatic stressor: handling dead bodies. J Nerv Ment Dis 1990; 178:396–398
4.
Frazer AG, Taylor AJW: The stress of post-disaster body handling and victim identification work. J Human Stress 1982; 8(4):5–12
5.
Jones DR: Secondary disaster victims: the emotional effects of recovering and identifying human remains. Am J Psychiatry 1985; 142:303–307
6.
Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Metzler TJ, Ronfeldt HM, Foreman C: Stress responses of emergency services personnel to the Loma Prieta earthquake Interstate 880 freeway collapse and control traumatic incidents. J Trauma Stress 1996; 9:63–85
7.
Weiss DS, Marmar CR, Metzler TJ, Ronfeldt HM: Predicting symptomatic distress in emergency services personnel. J Consult Clin Psychol 1995; 63:361–368
8.
Ursano RJ, Fullerton CS, Kao T: Longitudinal assessment of post-traumatic stress disorder and depression following exposure to traumatic death. J Nerv Ment Dis 1995; 183:36–43
9.
McCarroll JE, Ursano RJ, Fullerton CS: Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder following recovery of war dead. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1875–1877
10.
Sutker PB, Uddo M, Brailey K, Allain AN, Errera P: Psychological symptoms and psychiatric diagnoses in Operation Desert Storm troops serving graves registration duty. J Trauma Stress 1994; 7:159–171
11.
Sutker PB, Uddo M, Brailey K, Vasterling JJ, Errera P: Psychopathology in war-zone deployed and nondeployed Operation Desert Storm troops assigned graves registration duties. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:383–390
12.
McCarroll JE, Fullerton CS, Ursano RJ, Hermsen JM: Posttraumatic stress symptoms following forensic dental identification: Mt Carmel, Waco, Texas. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:778–782
13.
Fullerton CS, McCarroll JE, Ursano RJ, Wright KM: Psychological responses of rescue workers: firefighters and trauma. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1992; 62:371–378
14.
Ursano RJ, Fullerton CS: Cognitive and behavioral responses to trauma. J Applied Social Psychol 1990; 20:1766–1775
15.
Lindemann E: Symptomatology and management of acute grief. Am J Psychiatry 1944; 101:141–148
16.
Shuchter SR, Zisook S: Psychological reactions to the PSA crash. Int J Psychiatry Med 1984; 14:293–301
17.
Friedman MJ, Schnurr PP: The relationship between trauma, post-traumatic stress disorder, and physical health, in Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Stress: From Normal Adaptation to Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Edited by Friedman MJ, Charney DS, Deutch AY. Philadelphia, Lippincott-Raven, 1995, pp 507–524
18.
Zatzick DF, Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Browner WS, Metzler TJ, Golding JM, Stewart A, Schlenger WE, Wells KB: Posttraumatic stress disorder and functioning and quality of life outcomes in a nationally representative sample of male Vietnam veterans. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1690–1695
19.
Horowitz MJ, Wilner N, Alvarez W: Impact of Event Scale: a measure of subjective stress. Psychosom Med 1979; 41:209–218
20.
Schwarzwald J, Solomon Z, Weisenberg M, Mikulincer M: Validation of the Impact of Event Scale for psychological sequelae of combat. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:251–256
21.
Zilberg N, Weiss DS, Horowitz MJ: Impact of Event Scale: a cross-validation study and some empirical evidence. J Consult Clin Psychol 1982; 50:407–414
22.
Derogatis LR: SCL-90-R: Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual, II. Towson, Md, Clinical Psychometric Research, 1983
23.
Norris FH, Riad JK: Standardized self-report measures of civilian trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder, in Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD. Edited by Wilson JP, Keane TM. New York, Guilford Press, 1997, pp 7–42
24.
Staab JP, Grieger TA, Fullerton CS, Ursano RJ: Acute stress disorder, subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder and depression after a series of typhoons. Anxiety 1996; 2:219–225
25.
Ursano RJ, Fullerton CS, Kao T, Bhartiya VR, Dinneen MP: PTSD in community samples: development of a self-report instrument, in Proceedings of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, World Conference, Amsterdam. Chicago, International Traumatic Stress Society, 1992, p 35
26.
Ursano RJ, Fullerton CS: Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual for DSMPTSD-III-R and DSMPTSD-IV. Alexandria, Va, Defense Technical Information Center, 1997
27.
Fullerton CS, Ursano RJ, Crowley B, Epstein RS, Craig KJ, Baum AS: PTSD in community samples: a measurement problem, in 1997 Annual Meeting New Research Program and Abstracts. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1997, p 202
28.
SAS/STAT User’s Guide, version 6.1. Cary, NC, SAS Institute, 1990
29.
McCarroll JE, Ursano RJ, Fullerton CS, Oates GL, Ventis WL, Friedman H, Shean GL, Wright KM: Gruesomeness, emotional attachment, and personal threat: dimensions of the anticipated stress of body recovery. J Trauma Stress 1995; 8:343–349
30.
Norris F: Epidemiology of trauma: frequency and impact of different potentially traumatic events on different demographic groups. J Consult Clin Psychol 1992; 60:409–418
31.
Kulka RA, Schlenger WE, Fairbank JA, Hough RL, Jordan BK, Marmar CR, Weiss DS: Trauma and the Vietnam War Generation: Report of Findings From the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study. New York, Brunner/Mazel, 1990
32.
McFarlane AC, Atchison M, Rafalowicz E, Papay P: Physical symptoms in post-traumatic stress disorder. J Psychosom Res 1994; 38:715–726
33.
Brandt GT, Norwood AE, Ursano RJ, Wain H, Jaccard JT, Fullerton CS, Wright K: Psychiatric morbidity in medical and surgical patients evacuated from the Persian Gulf War. Psychiatr Serv 1997; 48:102–104
34.
Bullman TA, Kang HK: Posttraumatic stress disorder and the risk of traumatic deaths among Vietnam veterans. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994; 182:604–610

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 353 - 359
PubMed: 10080548

History

Published online: 1 March 1999
Published in print: March 1999

Authors

Details

Robert J. Ursano, M.D.
Carol S. Fullerton, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share