Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 August 2002

Effects of Methylphenidate on Functional Magnetic Resonance Relaxometry of the Cerebellar Vermis in Boys With ADHD

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The authors used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to test the effects of methylphenidate on steady-state blood volume in the midline vermis of the cerebellum in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). This region was selected as it has been observed to be significantly smaller in children with ADHD. Also, in preclinical studies, the vermis has been shown to modulate forebrain dopamine systems, to influence locomotor activity, and to contain a significant density of dopamine transporters. METHOD: T2 relaxometry was used to indirectly assess blood volume in the cerebellum (hemispheres and midline vermis) of 10 boys with ADHD who were administered placebo or one of three different doses of methylphenidate continuously for 1 week. T2 relaxation time values are inversely proportional to local cerebral blood volume. After each week of treatment, and within 1–3 hours of the boys’ afternoon dose, testing for drug efficacy was performed by using objective measures of activity. RESULTS: Moderate and high doses of methylphenidate increased T2 relaxation time in a rate-dependent manner—increasing T2 relaxation time in the most active children with ADHD and reducing T2 relaxation time in subjects with ADHD who were not objectively hyperactive. CONCLUSIONS: This preliminary study supports a role for the vermis in ADHD and suggests that further research is needed to clarify the relationship between vermal size, vermal blood flow, stimulant response, and the developmental pathophysiology of ADHD.
Neurobiological theories of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have predominantly focused on the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia. This is logical given the association of frontal lobe hypofunction with impulsive behavior, working memory, and deficits of executive function (1). The basal ganglia are well known to be involved in the regulation of locomotor activity, and both regions are primary targets of the ascending dopamine system (2), which is directly affected by stimulant medications (3). However, the largest morphometric studies of ADHD children have consistently found that both boys and girls with ADHD have smaller posterior-inferior lobules of the cerebellar vermis (47) than healthy children. This is particularly interesting as the cerebellum receives dopamine projections from the ventral tegmental area (8, 9) and the posterior-inferior lobules of the cerebellar vermis have the highest concentration of dopamine transporters in the primate vermis (10). Furthermore, there are reciprocal loops that interconnect a large and diverse set of cerebral cortical areas, including the prefrontal cortex, with the basal ganglia and cerebellum by way of the pons, dentate nucleus, and thalamus (11, 12). The vermis, through its fastigial projections to the ventral tegmental area and locus ceruleus, exerts strong effects on the turnover of dopamine and noradrenaline in the caudate and nucleus accumbens (1319). Stimulation of the hook bundle, a projection pathway in the white matter of the cerebellum, containing fibers from the caudal fastigial nucleus, which receives direct Purkinje cell efferents from the posterior-inferior vermis (20, 21), elicits locomotor activity in the decerebrate cat (22). In short, there are compelling reasons to hypothesize that the cerebellar vermis may be involved in both the pathophysiology of ADHD and therapeutic response to stimulant medications.
The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effects of methylphenidate on the cerebellar vermis of children with ADHD by using T2 relaxometry, a novel functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) procedure developed to assess changes in steady-state blood flow with long-term drug administration (23). Previously, we observed that daily treatment with high-dose methylphenidate changed T2 relaxation time in the putamen, supporting the link between dysfunction in cortical-striatal-thalamic circuits and the capacity to inhibit motor activity and sustain attention (23).
Although stimulants are known to exert robust effects on catecholamine systems, imaging studies have often failed to reveal reproducible medication effects. For example, Matochik et al. (24) found no consistent effect of the long-term administration of methylphenidate or dextroamphetamine on the regional brain metabolism of adult ADHD patients using PET. Similarly, Ernst et al. (25) found no consistent effect of intravenous dextroamphetamine on the global or regional metabolic rates of adult ADHD subjects, even though the drug produced robust effects on attention. Global metabolic rates changed substantially in most patients after treatment but in both directions. Volkow et al. (26) found that methylphenidate increased frontotemporal metabolism in subjects with a high availability of dopamine D2 receptors and decreased metabolism in subjects with a low availability of D2 receptors. These findings suggest that stimulants exert bidirectional effects on blood flow or metabolism, which may depend on individual differences in dopamine receptor density or other factors. We found that the magnitude and direction of methylphenidate effects on T2 relaxation time in the putamen were highly dependent on the subject’s hyperactivity (capacity to sit still when not taking medication) (23), which may be related to individual differences in dopamine receptor density or function as has been observed for mice (27) or dopamine transporter density or function as observed in primates (28). Hence, we coupled fMRI investigation with objective laboratory assessment of the child’s capacity to sit still and pay attention while receiving placebo and during treatment with low, intermediate, and high doses of methylphenidate.

Method

Subjects

Ten boys with ADHD (mean age=9.3 years, SD=1.6) and six healthy comparison boys (mean age=10.2 years, SD=1.5) were studied. McLean Hospital’s institutional review board approved this protocol, and written informed consent was obtained from parents or guardians. In addition, the fMRI procedures were carefully explained to the children, who also assented to participate. Children with ADHD were screened by use of a structured diagnostic interview (the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Epidemiologic Version [29]) and were included if they met criteria for ADHD and had at least six of nine symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity. Children then took part in a triple-blind (parent, child, and rater), randomized, placebo-controlled study of the effects of methylphenidate (0.0, 0.5, 0.8, or 1.5 mg/kg per day in two divided doses) on activity and fMRI. Children were treated continuously for 1 week with placebo or with a specific dose of methylphenidate. They were tested on day 7 for drug response by using objective measures of activity immediately after fMRI. On test days, parents were instructed to give the afternoon dose 1 hour before the fMRI appointment. Medication administration was assessed at each visit to ensure that time between ingestion and testing was held constant for each subject throughout the study. Within each subject there was an average of 28 minutes of variation in fMRI start times between the different dose conditions. Objective assessment of attention and activity followed fMRI by an average of 42 minutes (SD=39).

fMRI Procedures

Images were acquired by using a 1.5-T magnetic resonance scanner (Signa, General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee) equipped with a whole-body resonant gradient set capable of echo-planar imaging (Advanced NMR Systems, Inc., Wilmington, Mass.) and a standard quadrature head coil for image detection. During each examination, three categories of images were obtained: 1) scout images (typically T1-weighted sagittal images), 2) high-resolution T1-matched axial images through the 10 planes for which maps of T2 were generated, and 3) 32 spin-echo, echoplanar image sets, with TE incremented by 4 msec in each consecutive image set (e.g., TE 1=32 msec, TE 2=36 msec,… TE 32=156 msec) through the same 10 axial planes (TR=10 msec, slice thickness=7 mm with a 3-mm skip, in-plane resolution=3.125 mm × 3.125 mm, field of view=200 mm).
The 32 TE-stepped images were then transferred to an offline workstation and corrected for in-plane motion by using a modification of the DART image registration algorithm (30). The DART algorithm provided vertical, horizontal, and rotational values for subject movements during each functional scan. These values, which were within the range for which the registration algorithm has been validated, were significantly different between ADHD subjects and comparison subjects for horizontal and vertical movement on analysis of variance (ANOVA) (x axis: F=8.33, df=1, 14, p=0.01; y axis: F=9.07, df=1, 14, p=0.01). Of interest, there were no differences for ADHD subject movements as a function of methylphenidate dose (F=1.97, df=3, 21, p=0.15). For example, average head movement in the maximal vertical direction for boys with ADHD taking placebo was 1.16 mm, 1.66 mm after high-dose methylphenidate, and 0.12 mm for comparison subjects.
Values of T2 (x, y) and S (TE=0, x, y) were then calculated on a pixel-wise basis, assuming exponential decay, i.e., ln(S[x, y, n])=ln(S[TE=0], x, y)–TE (n)/T2(x, y), where (x, y) describes the location of the pixel, n characterizes the echo number, from 1 to 32, and S is the image signal intensity. Linear least-squares regression was used to calculate a single T2 relaxation time measure for each pixel (x, y). Calculations of regional T2 relaxation time were made for regions of interest drawn manually under the guidance of conventional T1-weighted matched MRI images of two to four slices through the cerebellum by using anatomic boundaries observed in T1-weighted, preliminary T2 relaxometry maps and an atlas of the cerebellum (31). Region-of-interest slice selections were based on an assessment of the presence of CSF in order to minimize partial volume artifacts, which result in abnormally elevated T2 relaxation times (values >100 msec were rejected). Sixteen voxel regions of interest in the vermis were conservatively limited to square regions (x=6 mm by y=6 mm) to avoid encroaching into ventricular space and to provide consistency between slices and subjects. Representative cerebellar regions of interest are depicted in Figure 1. Delineation of regions and analysis of imaging data were performed on coded images by an analyst who was familiar with cerebellar anatomy and was blind to the identity, diagnosis, and treatment conditions of the subject. The coefficient for intrarater reliability, alpha, was 0.987, based on four separate and independent T2 relaxation time measures from the vermis of seven subjects. Regional T2 relaxation times were calculated from the median value of all the designated pixels, as the median provides a regional estimate that is less susceptible to contamination by spurious values from CSF than does the mean. Regional T2 relaxation time values were averaged for all slices within subjects, and differences between placebo and drug conditions were then calculated.

Assessment of Activity

To accurately gauge the effects of methylphenidate on cerebellar blood flow, it was important before scanning to precisely assess individual differences in the ability to sit still and pay attention. A newly developed procedure was used in which subjects were challenged to attend to a monotonous but demanding computerized continuous performance task, during which head movements were recorded by using infrared motion analysis (23, 32). The optical tracking system recorded fidget-induced vertical and horizontal displacements of a reflective head marker to a resolution of 0.04 mm. Measures of immobility were derived from marker movement by calculating the average time between movements during the 15-minute test. A movement was defined to occur whenever the marker moved >1 mm from the position of the previous movement.

Results

Conventional analysis of cerebellar or vermal T2 relaxation time failed to reveal any significant effect of dose with repeated measures ANOVA (F=1.17, df=3, 27, p=0.34). Some subjects had an increase in T2 relaxation time, and others had a decrease. However, the magnitude and direction of change was not random but varied directly with each subject’s degree of hyperactivity when unmedicated. Analysis of covariance with motor activity as a covariant revealed a robust effect of methylphenidate treatment on vermal T2 relaxation time (F=4.85, df=3, 24, p=0.009), along with a significant dose-by-activity interaction (F=3.90, df=3, 24, p=0.02). Further, the effect of dose was linear (F=15.14, df=1, 8, p=0.005). No effect of methylphenidate treatment was observed on T2 relaxation time in the adjacent cerebellar hemispheres (dose: F=0.88, df=3, 21, p>0.40; dose by activity: F=0.47, df=3, 21, p>0.70).
Boys who spent the least time sitting still (e.g., who were the most hyperactive) demonstrated a positive dose-dependent change in T2 relaxation time (red circles in Figure 2). A negative dose-dependent change in T2 relaxation time was observed in boys who sat still longer (green circles in Figure 2). Vermal T2 relaxation time increased 1.2%, 2.1%, and 5.1% at low, intermediate, and high doses of methylphenidate, respectively, in seven boys who were at least 20% more active than the most active comparison subject (blue circles in Figure 2). In contrast, T2 relaxation time decreased an average of 1.3%, 3.5%, and 3.9% in the three least-active boys with ADHD. Percent changes in T2 relaxation time for hyperactive and nonhyperactive boys were significantly different by repeated measures ANOVA for intermediate and high doses but not low doses of methylphenidate (Figure 2). There was no significant association between percent change in T2 relaxation time and percent change in activity with methylphenidate. However, at the highest dose tested, there was a modest correlation (r=0.45, df=8, p=0.19), which would have been significant if an apparent outlying point had been eliminated (r=0.72, df=7, p=0.02).
There was a significant association between the objective measures of time spent immobile taking placebo and number of DSM-IV symptoms of hyperactivity reported during the clinical interview (r=0.72, df=10, p=0.01). However, clinical rating of activity was a weak covariate that failed to reveal a significant effect of methylphenidate on T2 relaxation time (F=0.66, df=3, 23, p>0.50).

Discussion

The highest dose of methylphenidate produced on average a 5.1% increase in T2 relaxation time in the children with ADHD who were objectively hyperactive and a 3.9% reduction in T2 relaxation time in the three ADHD children who were not objectively hyperactive. Are these percent changes biologically important? First, while these appear to be small percentage differences, they are quite strong and correspond to an effect size (Cohen’s d) of 2.13 (95% confidence interval=0.82–3.44) in the hyperactive subjects. A linear regression relationship between vermal T2 relaxation time and relative cerebral blood volume has been established by using dynamic susceptibility contrast MRI (33) in nine healthy 18–22-year-old adults (34) under resting conditions. Vermal relative cerebral blood volume values ranged from 0.7 to 2.0 (arbitrary units), while T2 relaxation times ranged from 90.0 to 95.5 msec. Hence, a 4-msec change in T2 relaxation time may be associated with a very marked change in relative cerebral blood volume. Precise estimation of relative cerebral blood volume changes should not be inferred, as this correspondence was observed in unmedicated adults and not medicated children, but it does suggest that this degree of change in T2 relaxation time should be associated with a biologically important change in relative cerebral blood volume.
These findings indicate that methylphenidate exerts a robust dose-dependent effect on the paramagnetic properties of the cerebellar vermis, which presumably is the result of alterations in regional blood flow and volume. However, both the magnitude and direction of the effects were entirely dependent on the subject’s basal level of hyperactivity or capacity to sit still. Failure to incorporate this intervening variable in the analysis completely obscured the effect. This may help to explain why previous efforts have failed to find consistent effects of methylphenidate or dextroamphetamine on regional brain metabolism (24, 25). Conceivably, precise quantitative measures of basal (unmedicated) activity or rate would have provided a key to understanding why regional brain metabolism increased in some subjects and decreased in others.
Preclinical research has frequently suggested that stimulants work in a rate-dependent manner, exerting behavioral effects that are inversely correlated to the basal rate of the behavior (35). That is, stimulants tend to decrease behaviors that normally occur at high rates and to increase behaviors that occur at low rates. Rate dependency contrasts with clinical reports that suggest that stimulants exert the same qualitative effect on all individuals (36, 37). However, those reports also acknowledge prominent quantitative differences in degree of response. The hypothesis that stimulants exert rate-dependent effects is supportive of observations by Vaidya et al. (38), found when using blood-oxygen-level-dependent fMRI, that stimulants exert qualitatively different effects on activation of the striatal system of normal comparison subjects and children with ADHD. Our primary finding—that methylphenidate effects on T2 relaxation time in the vermis depend strongly on basal activity levels—is wholly consistent with a rate-dependency hypothesis and suggest a possible neurobiological substrate for these behavioral effects.
Although methylphenidate acts primarily as a dopamine transporter antagonist, it also has affinity for the noradrenaline transporter (39, 40), and it is possible that noradrenaline neurons play a role in the response observed. Further binding studies in human tissue are essential to explore these issues.
The effects of methylphenidate on T2 relaxation time in the cerebellar vermis are consistent with a large number of studies implicating the cerebellum as a modulator of forebrain dopamine outflow (18, 19, 41, 42). Dopamine receptors (43, 44) and transporter immunoreactivity (10) have recently been observed in the primate cerebellum, and these observations are consistent with direct effects of stimulants on the cerebellum. Although early studies implicated the cerebellum as a modulator of dopaminergic outflow, only a few imaging studies have provided information on the interaction of stimulant drugs with the cerebellum (26, 45–48). In two studies, Volkow et al. (26, 46) reported consistently increased relative metabolic activity in the cerebellum after administration of intravenous methylphenidate (0.5 mg/kg) in normal adults. In the first study, the change in methylphenidate-stimulated activity from placebo activity correlated strongly with cerebellar dopamine receptor availability (26).
Other preclinical and clinical studies provide additional support for an association between the cerebellar vermis, hyperactivity, and stimulant action. For example, developmental lesions or mutations that have been associated with the transient development of hyperactivity in rats (49) and mice (50) also result in pathology of the midline cerebellum. When the cerebellum of neonatal rats is sequentially exposed to X-rays during production of granule cells targeted for the posterior-inferior vermis, these animals express greater spontaneous wheel running as young adults than do nonexposed rats (49). Mice homozygous for the “pallid” recessive mutation (phenotypically lack vestibular otoliths and display behavioral hyperactivity) are calmed by administration of amphetamine (50). In a clinical positron emission tomography imaging study of adults with ADHD who were retrospectively diagnosed with childhood hyperactivity, Schweitzer et al. (personal communication) observed blood flow changes in lobule VIII that were responsive to methylphenidate administration. In summary, clinical and preclinical behavioral, correlational, and imaging studies support an association among the cerebellar vermis, hyperactivity, and stimulant action.
Although the findings reported here are preliminary in nature and require further replication, taken together, they implicate the cerebellar vermis as an important brain region that may be directly involved in the pathophysiology of ADHD and in rate-dependent stimulant effects on behavior. Further research will be needed to clarify the relationship between vermal size, vermal blood flow, stimulant response, and the developmental pathophysiology of ADHD.
Figure 1. Brain Regions of Interest Sampled With Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in the Cerebellum of Boys With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder for T2 Relaxation Timea
aOutlines indicate approximate sampling regions for cerebellar hemispheres and cerebellar vermis on T2 relaxometry maps and matched T1-weighted axial images.
Figure 2. Relation of Change in Cerebellar Vermis T2 Relaxation Time Between Placebo and Three Doses of Methylphenidate to Basal Level of Physical Activity in Boys With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Assessed With Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaginga
aPhysical activity was defined as the average time the subject sat still while receiving placebo. The low, intermediate, and high doses of methylphenidate were 0.5, 0.8, and 1.5 mg/kg per day in two divided doses.
bHyperactive boys spent an average of 20% less time sitting still than the most active healthy comparison subject.
cFor both axes, the center of the ellipse indicates the mean, and the half-width indicates the standard deviation.
dNonmedicated; values on the vertical axis are arbitrary.
eNonsignificant group effect (F=0.63, df=1, 8, p=0.63).
fSignificant group effect (F=6.75, df=1, 8, p=0.04).
gSignificant group effect (F=15.98, df=1, 8, p=0.004).

Footnote

Presented in part at the 63rd annual scientific meeting of the College on Problems of Drug Dependence, Scottsdale, Ariz., June 16–21, 2001. Received June 15, 2001; revision received April 2, 2002; accepted April 4, 2002. From the Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston; and the Developmental Biopsychiatry Research Program and the Brain Imaging Center, McLean Hospital. Address reprint requests to Dr. Anderson, Developmental Biopsychiatry Research Program, McLean Hospital, 115 Mill St., Belmont, MA 02478; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported in part by NIMH grant MH-48343 (to Dr. Teicher) and National Institute on Drug Abuse grants DA-09448 and DA-14178 (Dr. Renshaw). Dr. Anderson is supported by a special supplement to grant MH-53636 (Dr. Teicher). The authors thank Anne Smith, R.T., and Eileen Connolly, R.T., for collecting fMRI data and F. Xavier Castellanos, M.D., Marc J. Kaufman, Ph.D., Jay N. Giedd, M.D., and Julie B. Schweitzer, Ph.D., for their discussions.

References

1.
Barkley RA: Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997; 121:65-94
2.
Graybiel AM: The basal ganglia. Curr Biol 2000; 10:R509-R511
3.
Swanson J, Volkow N: Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic properties of methylphenidate in humans, in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Edited by Solanto MV, Arnsten AFT, Castellanos FX. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 2001, pp 259-282
4.
Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Marsh WL, Hamburger SD, Vaituzis AC, Dickstein DP, Sarfatti SE, Vauss YC, Snell JW, Lange N, Kaysen D, Krain AL, Ritchie GF, Rajapakse JC, Rapoport JL: Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:607-616
5.
Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Berquin PC, Walter JM, Sharp W, Tran T, Vaituzis AC, Bastain T, Blumenthal J, Nelson J, Zijdenbos A, Evans AC, Rapoport JL: Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:289-295
6.
Mostofsky SH, Reiss AL, Lockhart P, Denckla MB: Evaluation of cerebellar size in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Neurol 1998; 13:434-439
7.
Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, Rapoport JL, Castellanos FX: Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder—a morphometric MRI study. Neurology 1998; 50:1087-1093
8.
Ikai Y, Takada M, Mizuno N: Single neurons in the ventral tegmental area that project to both the cerebral and cerebellar cortical areas by way of axon collaterals. Neuroscience 1994; 61:925-934
9.
Ikai Y, Takada M, Shinonaga Y, Mizuno N: Dopaminergic and non-dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area of the rat project, respectively, to the cerebellar cortex and deep cerebellar nuclei. Neuroscience 1992; 51:719-728
10.
Melchitzsky DS, Lewis DA: Tyrosine hydrolase- and dopamine transporter-immunoreactive axons in the primate cerebellum: evidence for a lobular- and laminar-specific dopamine innervation. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 22:466-472
11.
Middleton FA, Strick PL: Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2000; 31:236-250
12.
Schwarz C, Thier P: Binding of signals relevant for action: towards a hypothesis of the functional role of the pontine nuclei. Trends Neurosci 1999; 22:443-451
13.
Albert TJ, Dempesy CW, Sorenson CA: Anterior cerebellar vermal stimulation: effect on behavior and basal forebrain neurochemistry in rat. Biol Psychiatry 1985; 20:1267-1276
14.
Dempesy CW, Tootle DM, Fontana CJ, Fitzjarrell AT, Garey RE, Heath RG: Stimulation of the paleocerebellar cortex of the cat: increased rate of synthesis and release of catecholamines at limbic sites. Biol Psychiatry 1983; 18:127-132
15.
Nieoullon A, Cheramy A, Glowinski J: Release of dopamine in both caudate nuclei and both substantia nigrae in response to unilateral stimulation of cerebellar nuclei in the cat. Brain Res 1978; 148:143-152
16.
Snider SR, Snider RS: Alterations in forebrain catecholamine metabolism produced by cerebellar lesions in the rat. J Neural Transm 1977; 40:115-128
17.
Snider RS, Maiti A, Snider SR: Cerebellar pathways to ventral midbrain and nigra. Exp Neurol 1976; 53:714-728
18.
Snider SR, Snider RS: Structural and functional relationships between cerebellum and catecholamine systems: an overview. Exp Brain Res Suppl 1982; 6:557-606
19.
Tellerman K, Astrow A, Fahn S, Snider SR, Snider RS, Glassgold JM: Cerebellar control of catecholaminergic activities: implications for drug therapy of movement disorders. Int J Neurol 1979; 13:135-155
20.
Jansen J, Brodal A: Experimental studies on the intrinsic fibers of the cerebellum, II: the cortico-nuclear projection. J Comp Neurol 1940; 73:267-321
21.
Mori S, Matsui T, Mori F, Nakajima K, Matsuyama K: Instigation and control of treadmill locomotion in high decerebrate cats by stimulation of the hook bundle of Russell in the cerebellum. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 2000; 78:945-957
22.
Mori S, Matsui T, Kuze B, Asanome M, Nakajima K, Matsuyama K: Stimulation of a restricted region in the midline cerebellar white matter evokes coordinated quadrupedal locomotion in the decerebrate cat. J Neurophysiol 1999; 82:290-300
23.
Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A, Glod CA, Maas LC, Renshaw PF: Functional deficits in basal ganglia of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder shown with functional magnetic resonance imaging relaxometry. Nat Med 2000; 6:470-473
24.
Matochik JA, Liebenauer LL, King AC, Szymanski HV, Cohen RM, Zametkin AJ: Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder after chronic stimulant treatment. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:658-664
25.
Ernst M, Zametkin AJ, Matochik JA, Jons PH, Cohen RM: DOPA decarboxylase activity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults: a [fluorine-18]fluorodopa positron emission tomographic study. J Neurosci 1998; 18:5901-5907
26.
Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Logan J, Angrist B, Hitzemann R, Lieberman J, Pappas N: Effects of methylphenidate on regional brain glucose metabolism in humans: relationship to dopamine D2 receptors. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:50-55
27.
Helmeste JM, Seeman P: Amphetamine-induced hypolocomotion in mice with more brain D2 dopamine receptors. Psychiatry Res 1982; 7:351-359
28.
Miller GM, De La Garza RD II, Novak MA, Madras BK: Single nucleotide polymorphisms distinguish multiple dopamine transporter alleles in primates: implications for association with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry 2001; 6:50-58
29.
Orvaschel H: Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Epidemiologic Version (K-SADS-E), 5th ed. Fort Lauderdale, Fla, Nova Southeastern University, Center for Psychological Studies, 1994
30.
Maas LC, Frederick BD, Renshaw PF: Decoupled automated rotational and translational registration for functional MRI time series data: the DART registration algorithm. Magn Reson Med 1997; 37:131-139
31.
Duvernoy HM: The Human Brain Stem and Cerebellum: Surface, Structure, Vascularization, and Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy With MRI. New York, Springer-Verlag Wien, 1995
32.
Teicher MH, Ito Y, Glod CA, Barber NI: Objective measurement of hyperactivity and attentional problems in ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35:334-342
33.
Levin J, Ross M, Harris G, Renshaw P: Applications of dynamic susceptibility contrast magnetic resonance imaging in neuropsychiatry. Neuroimage 1996; 4:S147-S162
34.
Anderson CM, Maas LC, Renshaw PF, Teicher MH: Non-invasive T2 relaxation time measures correlate with gadolinium-based cerebral blood volume estimates in the putamen and cerebellar vermis of young adults. Abstracts of the Society for Neuroscience 2000, 30:1730
35.
Solanto MV, Arnsten AFT, Castellanos FX: The neuroscience of stimulant drug action in ADHD, in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Edited by Solanto MV, Arnsten AFT, Castellanos FX. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 2001, pp 355-379
36.
Rapoport JL, Buchsbaum MS, Zahn TP, Weingartner H, Ludlow C, Mikkelsen EJ: Dextroamphetamine: cognitive and behavioral effects in normal prepubertal boys. Science 1978; 199:560-563
37.
Rapoport JL, Buchsbaum MS, Weingartner H, Zahn TP, Ludlow C, Mikkelsen EJ: Dextroamphetamine: its cognitive and behavioral effects in normal and hyperactive boys and normal men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980; 37:933-943
38.
Vaidya CJ, Austin G, Kirkorian G, Ridlehuber HW, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD: Selective effects of methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998; 95:14494-14499
39.
Patrick KS, Caldwell RW, Ferris RM, Breese GR: Pharmacology of the enantiomers of threo-methylphenidate. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1987; 241:152-158
40.
Gatley SJ, Pan D, Chen R, Chaturvedi G, Ding YS: Affinities of methylphenidate derivatives for dopamine, norepinephrine and serotonin transporters. Life Sci 1996; 58:231-239
41.
Glowinski J, Nieoullon A, Cheramy A: Regulations of the activity of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic pathways by cortical, cerebellar, and sensory neuronal afferences. Adv Biochem Psychopharmacol 1978; 19:75-87
42.
Nieoullon A, Cheramy A, Glowinski J: Control of the activity of the two dopaminergic systems by cortical, cerebellar and sensory afferents in the cat. Appl Neurophysiol 1979; 42:51-53
43.
Ciliax BJ, Nash N, Heilman C, Sunahara R, Hartney A, Tiberi M, Rye DB, Caron MG, Niznik HB, Levey AI: Dopamine D(5) receptor immunolocalization in rat and monkey brain. Synapse 2000; 37:125-145
44.
Barili P, Bronzetti E, Ricci A, Zaccheo D, Amenta F: Microanatomical localization of dopamine receptor protein immunoreactivity in the rat cerebellar cortex. Brain Res 2000; 854:130-138
45.
Schweitzer JB, Anderson C, Ernst M: ADHD: neuroimaging and behavioral/cognitive probes, in Functional Neuroimaging in Child Psychiatry. Edited by Ernst M, Rumsey JM. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 2000, pp 278-297
46.
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Hitzemann R, Gatley J, Ding YS, Wong C, Pappas N: Differences in regional brain metabolic responses between single and repeated doses of methylphenidate. Psychiatry Res 1998; 83:29-36
47.
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Fischman M, Foltin R, Abumrad NN, Gatley SJ, Logan J, Wong C, Gifford A, Ding YS, Hitzemann R, Pappas N: Methylphenidate and cocaine have a similar in vivo potency to block dopamine transporters in the human brain. Life Sci 1999; 65:L7-L12
48.
Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Hitzemann R, Angrist B, Gatley SJ, Logan J, Ding Y-S, Pappas N: Association of methylphenidate-induced craving with changes in right striato-orbitofrontal metabolism in cocaine abusers: implications in addiction. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:19-26
49.
Altman J, Bayer SA: Development of the Cerebellar System in Relation to Its Evolution, Structure, and Functions. Boca Raton, Fla, CRC Press, 1997
50.
Douglas RJ, Clark GM, Erway LC, Hubbard DG, Wright CG: Effects of genetic vestibular defects on behavior related to spatial orientation and emotionality. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1979; 93:467-480

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1322 - 1328
PubMed: 12153824

History

Published online: 1 August 2002
Published in print: August 2002

Authors

Details

Carl M. Anderson, Ph.D.
Ann Polcari, R.N., Ph.D.
Steven B. Lowen, Ph.D.
Perry F. Renshaw, M.D., Ph.D.
Martin H. Teicher, M.D., Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share