Skip to main content
Open access
Articles
Published Online: 6 November 2015

Amygdala Activity During Autobiographical Memory Recall in Depressed and Vulnerable Individuals: Association With Symptom Severity and Autobiographical Overgenerality

Abstract

Objective:

In healthy individuals, autobiographical memory recall is biased toward positive and away from negative events, while the opposite is found in depressed individuals. This study examined amygdala activity during autobiographical memory recall as a putative mechanism underlying biased memory recall and depressive symptoms in currently depressed adults and two vulnerable populations: individuals remitted from depression and otherwise healthy individuals at high familial risk of developing depression. Identification of such vulnerability factors could enable interception strategies that prevent depression onset.

Method:

Sixty healthy control subjects, 45 unmedicated currently depressed individuals, 25 unmedicated remitted depressed individuals, and 30 individuals at high familial risk of developing depression underwent functional MRI while recalling autobiographical memories in response to emotionally valenced cue words. Amygdala reactivity and connectivity with anatomically defined amygdala regions were examined.

Results:

During positive recall, depressed participants exhibited significantly decreased left amygdala activity and decreased connectivity with regions of the salience network compared with the other groups. During negative recall, control subjects had significantly decreased left amygdala activity compared with the other groups, while depressed participants exhibited increased amygdala connectivity with the salience network. In depressed participants, left amygdala activity during positive recall correlated significantly with depression severity (r values >−0.38) and percent of positive specific memories recalled (r values >0.59).

Conclusions:

The results suggest that left amygdala hyperactivity during negative autobiographical recall is a trait-like marker of depression, as both vulnerable groups showed activity similar to the depressed group, while amygdala hypoactivity during positive autobiographical recall is a state marker of depression manifesting in active disease. Treatments targeting amygdala hypoactivity and blunted salience during positive autobiographical recall could exert antidepressant effects.
Identification of vulnerability factors for depression ultimately may enable researchers to formulate illness interception strategies for predicting and preempting the development of mood disorders. In this study, we investigated whether a putative but treatable biological mechanism of depression, namely, amygdala activity during positively and negatively valenced autobiographical memory recall, constitutes a vulnerability factor for depression. We assessed this neuroimaging biomarker in currently depressed individuals and in nondepressed groups that are vulnerable to depression, based on their past personal or family history.
Recalling specific, affectively positive autobiographical memories is central to adaptive functioning (1). This memory system has been hypothesized to operate in part to maintain a positive mood (2), with positive memory recall specifically important for regulating emotional well-being (3). In healthy individuals, autobiographical memory recall is biased toward positive events (4, 5). In contrast, individuals with depression manifest a bias away from positive memories, such that relative to healthy controls, they are less likely to recall a positive memory in response to a positive or neutral cue word (5), are more likely to recall positive memories that are overgeneral as opposed to specific (5, 6), and demonstrate longer latencies to recall memories in response to positive cues (7). This absence of a positive memory bias may contribute to the maintenance of the negative mood and impaired problem solving evident in depression (8), and it is therefore a potential therapeutic target.
However, no correlation between autobiographical memory performance and depressive symptoms has been found (e.g., 9), and research directly targeting autobiographical memory recall by having participants recall positive memories with the purpose of improving mood has proven it to be ineffective (10). Therefore, while autobiographical memory impairment is a key feature of depression (8), it may not be the impairment in autobiographical recall itself that directly relates to the maintenance of depressive symptoms. Rather, the presence of this biased recall in conjunction with an abnormality in regional brain function subserving salience and emotional processing may underlie this bias. Therefore, we aimed to test the hypothesis that a potential mechanism underlying emotionally biased autobiographical memory recall and the development of depression involves abnormal amygdala activity.
The amygdala influences the salience of stimuli or events (11), and amygdala engagement is critical for emotional processing and responding to both negative and positive emotional stimuli (12). This has been shown to be true for autobiographical stimuli (13), with healthy adults exhibiting greater amygdala activity during positive relative to negative autobiographical recall (14). With laboratory stimuli such as words and faces, the amygdala response in depressed individuals is “doubly dissociated” from that in control subjects, such that individuals with depression show exaggerated responses to negative stimuli and attenuated responses to positive stimuli (15). As the amygdala provides information about the salience of stimuli (11) and the salience of positive stimuli is reduced in depression (15), individuals with depression may not find positive memories as compelling and therefore expend less effort in recalling them, with the result that they are easily distracted by salient negative episodes. To test this hypothesis, we examined whether the doubly dissociated pattern of amygdala activity would be evident during emotionally valenced memory recall, with increased amygdala activity and connectivity with regions important for maintaining salience (i.e., the dorsal anterior cingulate) during positive memory recall, and decreased activity but increased connectivity with regions associated with effort (i.e., dorsomedial prefrontal cortex) during negative memory recall in controls relative to individuals with depression.
We previously identified regional brain differences during positive and negative memory recall in healthy subjects compared with depressed participants, healthy subjects at high risk for developing depression based on having a first-degree relative with depression (16), and individuals in full remission from depression (9). In whole-brain group analyses, we found differential recruitment of medial temporal structures but not the amygdala. The amygdala is a small structure located near the basotemporal surface, where the temporal signal-to-noise ratio is lower than in most other brain regions (17). Thus, significant activation may be missed by global voxel-wise analyses, which require conservative corrections for multiple comparisons (18). Therefore, to increase power to detect differences in amygdala activity, we increased our sample size and performed a small-volume-corrected region-of-interest analysis on the previously reported data. To identify other regions that interact with the amygdala during emotional autobiographical memory, we conducted a functional connectivity analysis.
In addition, we further investigated whether abnormal amygdala activity combined with biased memory recall might be associated with the development of depression. To test this hypothesis, we included a currently depressed group as well as two vulnerable groups: remitted and high-risk individuals. Including these vulnerable groups would assist us in distinguishing whether biased memory recall and amygdala activity reflect state markers or scars of depression on the one hand or trait-like persistent markers of illness on the other. While there are few abnormalities in information processing readily discerned in vulnerable groups without necessitating a mood induction (19), autobiographical memory recall is one such processing bias evident in vulnerable samples without prior mood induction (8, 9, 20), making it especially suitable for testing our hypotheses.
Given the amygdala’s role in positive memory recall, the difficulties individuals with depression have in recalling specific positive memories, and the role of positive memory recall in adaptive functioning, we predicted that amygdala responses to positive memories would be related to symptom severity in depression, to residual symptoms in remitted depression, and to deficits in recalling positive specific memories in both depression groups. However, we previously observed that autobiographical memory impairment extends to high-risk individuals without depressive symptoms (16), suggesting that this impairment interacts with some factor to lead to depression. Therefore, we did not expect any difference in amygdala activity between high-risk participants and controls. If this hypothesis were supported, it would suggest that amygdala hypoactivity to positive or hyperactivity to negative memories, in conjunction with the absence of a positive recall bias, may be causally related to the development and maintenance of depression, and that interventions targeting amygdala activity, such as real-time functional fMRI-based neurofeedback during emotional memory recall, may hold therapeutic potential (21).

Method

Participants

A total of 160 medically healthy right-handed individuals 18 to 55 years old were recruited from the community and enrolled in one of four participant groups: psychiatrically healthy individuals with no first-degree relatives with a history of a mood disorder (N=60; control group), psychiatrically healthy individuals with a first-degree relative with depression (N=30; high-risk group), unmedicated individuals in a current major depressive episode according to DSM-IV-TR criteria (N=45; depressed group), and unmedicated individuals remitted from depression as defined by DSM-IV-TR criteria and with a Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) score <7 (N=25; remitted group) (22). Participants underwent medical and psychiatric screening evaluations, which included the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (23). Data from subsets of these participants have been reported previously (9, 16, 24, 25), but this is the first report combining all samples.
Exclusion criteria included current pregnancy, general MRI exclusions, serious suicidal ideation, psychosis, major medical or neurological disorders, exposure to medication likely to influence cerebral function or blood flow within the past 3 weeks (3 months for the remitted group), and meeting DSM-IV-TR criteria for drug or alcohol abuse within the past year or for lifetime dependence (except nicotine). Additional exclusion criteria applied to the high-risk and control groups were current or past history of any major psychiatric disorder. After receiving a complete description of the study procedures, all participants provided written informed consent as approved by the Western Institutional Review Board. Participants received financial compensation for their participation.
Intelligence testing was conducted with the two-subtest (vocabulary and matrix reasoning) version of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (26). Anxiety and depressive symptoms were rated using the 21-item HAM-D (27), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (28), and the Profile of Mood States (POMS) (29).

fMRI Autobiographical Memory Task

Details of the functional MRI (fMRI) data acquisition parameters and autobiographical memory task have been reported previously (9, 16). Briefly, blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI was performed on a 3-T GE Discovery MR750 scanner and an eight-channel head coil. Participants were presented with an emotionally valenced or neutral cue word (20 positive, 20 negative, 20 neutral) and instructed to recall a past experience for 12 seconds and rate their retrieved memory on specificity and valence. Participants were instructed on the definitions of the standard memory categorizations of specific, categorical, extended, and semantic (8, 30), and were able to provide and classify examples of each type before scanning was started. Autobiographical memory recall was compared with a semantic example generation condition in which participants were presented with an emotionally valenced or neutral cue word and instructed to think of seven examples from the category, then rate the ease with which they generated examples and report the number generated. Following each cue presentation and set of ratings, participants engaged in a riser detection task involving nonword letter strings as a control for visual input/attention.

Data Analysis

fMRI analysis was performed using AFNI (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni), and consisted of slice timing correction, within-subject realignment, coregistration between anatomical and functional images, spatial normalization to a common stereotaxic array (31), and spatial smoothing (Gaussian kernel, 4 mm full width at half maximum). General linear model analysis was performed using AFNI’s 3dDeconvolve program. For each participant, the hemodynamic response to each event type was modeled as a boxcar function convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. The main effects of interest were cue word presentations that prompted positive specific memory recall, negative specific memory recall, example generation in response to positive cues, and example generation in response to negative cues. In addition to regressors modeling main effects and subject motion, each design matrix included regressors modeling rating selection and cue presentations in which other types of memories were recalled (categorical, extended, semantic). The nonword letter strings used as stimuli for the riser detection task were modeled as the baseline. A whole brain analysis of variance (ANOVA) was also performed to determine whether findings emerging from the present analysis replicate the patterns reported previously in other analyses of this sample (see the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article).
We defined regions of interest for the left and right amygdala using Talairach amygdala masks within AFNI. For each participant, the 3dDeconvolve output was resampled and the amygdala mask applied (AFNI’s 3dROIstats) to calculate the percent signal change within each region of interest for specific positive memories versus positive example generation and specific negative memories versus negative example generation.
The resulting percent signal change values were entered into a repeated-measures ANOVA with the repeated measure of valence (positive, negative) and amygdala region of interest (left, right), and the between-group factor of group (control, high-risk, depressed, remitted) using SYSTAT, version 13 (Systat Software, San Jose, Calif.). We decided a priori to perform Pearson correlations between amygdala activity, mood ratings, and memory performance to detect associations between amygdala activity, mood, and memory overgenerality. The threshold criterion for significance was set at Bonferroni-corrected p value of 0.05, applied for each post hoc group comparison and correlation (e.g., all control group versus high-risk group tests corrected for 36 comparisons [all variables in Table 1 plus four amygdala variables] and all control versus high-risk group correlations corrected for 50 multiple comparisons [25 variables in Table 1 excluding nonspecific memories, correlated with amygdala activity during positive and negative memory recall separately]). Potential group differences in age, IQ, memory performance, and depression and anxiety ratings were assessed using a one-way ANOVA.
TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics and Memory Performance for Each Group
MeasureControl Group (N=60)High-Risk Group (N=30)Remitted Group (N=25)Depressed Group (N=45)
Demographic characteristics and prescan ratings        
 N%N%N%N%
Female30501963.31664.02044.4
 MeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Age30.38.730.69.330.911.233.58.8
IQ11111.41097.21089.310911.9
Hamilton Depression Rating Scalea0.861.990.631.313.88b,c4.2218.90b,c,d6.15
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory        
 Statea25.25.527.35.929.56.845.9b,c,d12.1
 Traita26.24.427.14.134.0b,c7.754.5b,c,d9.3
Profile of Mood States        
 Depressiona0.361.130.481.122.00b,c3.0618.80b,c,d14.10
 Tensiona2.472.413.112.223.322.7811.50b,c,d8.33
 Angera0.451.310.330.732.004.967.07b,c,d6.88
 Vigora19.804.9419.046.5917.606.378.55b,c,d5.60
 Fatiguea2.612.742.002.304.88b,c4.4513.00b,c,d8.24
 Confusiona1.971.492.631.843.84b,c1.8411.00b,c,d5.87
 Friendlya20.404.4519.935.1219.105.0214.50b,c,d5.05
 Totala–12.009.17–10.5010.70–1.00b,c14.4052.50b,c,d40.30
Change from prescan to postscan ratings        
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory        
 State0.374.34–0.414.790.285.650.917.84
 Traita–0.201.72–0.301.88–2.32b,c3.05–2.80b,c3.91
Profile of Mood States        
 Depressiona0.030.87–0.220.85–1.04b,c2.53–2.89b,c7.96
 Tension0.022.74–0.782.06–0.444.46–0.576.52
 Anger–0.160.61–0.040.76–1.244.75–1.214.92
 Vigor–2.264.01–1.334.69–2.805.08–0.683.31
 Fatigue1.033.240.932.561.245.21–0.664.48
 Confusion0.902.420.302.280.962.090.934.60
 Friendly–0.732.76–1.483.42–1.403.42–1.253.10
 Total4.088.481.528.102.2813.90–3.7123.10
Autobiographical memory performance (% recalled)        
Specifica59.911.546.7b13.241.5b10.540.6b14.6
 Positivea58.713.057.211.751.2b,c13.446.0b,c12.6
 Negativea29.011.431.712.134.912.739.5b14.1
 Neutral12.410.111.28.114.014.114.511.1
Categoricala16.26.828.0b10.433.6b11.133.9b12.3
Extended3.53.25.53.83.92.83.02.7
Semantic6.75.28.16.56.55.16.55.4
No memory1.72.31.93.23.14.03.15.1
Can’t recall12.27.010.15.811.37.512.97.3
a
Significant difference between groups (p<0.05).
b
Significant difference from the control group (p<0.05).
c
Significant difference from the high-risk group (p<0.05).
d
Significant difference from the remitted group (p<0.05).
For the generalized linear model-based functional connectivity analysis, the data were low band pass filtered at 0.10 Hz and the left amygdala seed was anatomically defined. The time course of the mean BOLD signal from the seed during positive and negative memory recall were used, separately, as the stimulus regressor. The generalized linear model for each run also included six motion parameters. The generalized linear model based R2 statistics were converted to correlation coefficients and converted to z scores using the Fisher r-to-z transformation. AFNI’s 3dANOVA was used to determine how the functional connectivity pattern differed between groups. The significance threshold was set at corrected p value of 0.05 (3dClustSim cluster size >30 voxels, voxel p value <0.005).

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

The upper section of Table 1 presents means and standard deviations for demographic and mood ratings for each group (for clinical characteristics of the depression groups, see Table S1 in the online data supplement). The groups did not differ in age, IQ, or gender distribution but differed significantly in mood ratings, with depressed participants reporting significantly different ratings (indicating more negative overall mood) compared with the other groups (compared with the control group: corrected p values, <0.001; d values, >1.24; high-risk group: corrected p values, <0.001; d values >1.07; remitted group: corrected p values, <0.002; d values >0.85). While remitted participants had scores within the healthy range on all measures, their HAM-D scores, trait anxiety ratings, POMS depression, fatigue, and confusion subscale ratings, and total POMS scores were elevated relative to both healthy groups (compared with the control group: corrected p values, <0.02; d values >0.61; high-risk group: corrected p values, <0.02; d values >0.66). The high-risk and control groups did not differ significantly from each other on any clinical measure.
The POMS and STAI were administered immediately before and after completion of the fMRI task, and the associated omnibus tests are reported in the middle section of Table 1. No rating in the control or high-risk group changed significantly from pre- to postscan assessment, whereas POMS depression subscale and STAI trait anxiety ratings decreased significantly in both the depressed and remitted groups (corrected p values, <0.01 and <0.009, respectively). The depressed and remitted groups had significantly greater decreases in these measures compared with both the control group (compared with the depressed group: corrected p values, <0.005, d values, >0.52; remitted group: corrected p values, <0.004; d values >0.57) and the high-risk group (compared with the depressed group: corrected p values, <0.03; d values, >0.47; remitted group: corrected p values, <0.03; d values, >0.43). These change measures did not differ significantly between the remitted and depressed groups or between the control and high-risk groups.

Autobiographical Memory Recall

The lower section in Table 1 lists the percentage of each memory type recalled for each group and a breakdown of the percentages of specific memories recalled for each valence, as specific memory valence is the focus of the study. We replicated the overgenerality effect; control subjects recalled more specific and fewer categorical memories compared with all other groups (compared with the high-risk group: corrected p values, <0.001; d values >1.07; remitted group: corrected p values, <0.02; d values >1.67; depressed group: corrected p values, <0.001; d values >1.47), and these other groups did not differ from each other. No other memory categorization differed between groups. When examining the percentages of specific memories rated as positive, negative, and neutral, we found that the control and high-risk group had more positive specific memories than either the remitted group (compared with the control group: corrected p=0.01; d=0.57; high-risk group: corrected p=0.04; d=0.48) or the depressed group (compared with the control group: corrected p<0.001; d=0.99; high-risk group: corrected p=0.003; d=0.92), and the depressed group had significantly more negative specific memories compared with the control group (corrected p=0.001; d=0.82).

Amygdala BOLD Activity

Figure 1 shows the percent signal change within the left and right amygdalae during positive memory recall relative to positive example generation and negative memory recall relative to negative example generation. There was a significant region of interest-by-valence-by-group interaction (F=7.20, df=3, 156, p<0.001) in the left amygdala, with depressed participants exhibiting decreased activity in response to positive specific memory recall relative to the other groups (compared with the control group: corrected p=0.001; d=0.92; high-risk group: corrected p=0.004; d=0.71; remitted group: corrected p=0.001; d=0.89) and the other groups not differing from each other. When left amygdala activity was examined during negative specific memory recall, the control group had decreased activity relative to all of the other groups (compared with the high-risk group: corrected p=0.002; d=0.23; remitted group: corrected p=0.02; d=0.11; depressed group: corrected p<0.001; d=0.33), which did not differ from each other. In the right amygdala, the same pattern of activity was observed, but the only difference that reached statistical significance was for greater activity during positive memory recall versus positive example generation in the control group compared with the depressed group (corrected p=0.001; d=0.58). An additional analysis confirmed that this was the case for both the new participants and those included in previous studies (see the online data supplement).
FIGURE 1. Amygdala Activity During Emotional Valenced Memory Recalla
a The graphs show percent signal change in the left and right amygdala during positive specific autobiographical memory recall relative to positive example generation, and negative specific autobiographical memory recall relative to negative example generation for each participant group. Error bars indicate standard error.
b Significant difference from the depressed group (corrected p<0.05).
c Significant difference from the control group (corrected p<0.05).

Correlation Between Amygdala Response, Mood, and Memory Performance

Because of small variance in scores and memory performance in the healthy groups, we examined correlations separately for each group (Figure 2). There was a significant inverse correlation in depressed participants, with higher left amygdala activity during positive memory recall associated with lower HAM-D score (r=−0.45, corrected p=0.002) and POMS depression subscale score (r=−0.43, corrected p=0.004). No significant correlation was found in the other groups. During negative memory recall, one correlation remained significant after applying corrections for multiple testing: an inverse relationship in the depressed group between left amygdala activity during negative memory recall and POMS ratings of friendliness (r=−0.34, corrected p=0.03).
FIGURE 2. Correlation Between Amygdala Activity and Prescan Mood Ratings in the Depressed Groupa
a Panels A and B show the correlation between the percent signal changes in the left amygdala during positive memory recall relative to positive example generation and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) scores and Profile of Mood States (POMS) depression subscores. Panel C shows the correlation between the percent signal changes in the left amygdala during negative memory recall relative to negative example generation and POMS friendliness subscores.
We also examined the relationship between amygdala activity during memory recall and changes in mood ratings between pre- and postscan assessments (Figure 3). We found no significant correlation in the healthy groups. However, the depression groups exhibited a significant inverse correlation between left amygdala activity during positive memory recall and changes in STAI trait anxiety ratings (depressed group: r=−0.37, corrected p=0.02; remitted group: r=−0.42, corrected p=0.001) and POMS depression scores (depressed group: r=−0.39, corrected p=0.03; remitted group: r=−0.28, corrected p=0.05).
FIGURE 3. Correlation Between Amygdala Activity, Pre- to Postscan Mood Rating Changes, and Autobiographical Memory Performance in the Depressed and Remitted Groupsa
a The correlation between percent signal change in the left amygdala during positive memory recall relative to positive example generation and, in panel A, the change in trait measures of anxiety on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI); in panel B, the change in Profile of Mood States (POMS) depression subscores; in panel C, the percent of positive specific memories recalled; and in panel D, the percent of negative memories recalled.
Finally, we examined the relationship between amygdala activity during memory recall and the percent of specific memories recalled for each valence and overall percent of specific memories recalled (Figure 3). There was no significant correlation in the healthy groups. However, in the depression groups, there was a significant correlation between left amygdala activity during positive memory recall and the percent of positive specific memories recalled (depressed group: r=0.61, corrected p=0.001; remitted group: r=0.59, corrected p=0.002) and an inverse correlation between left amygdala activity during positive memory recall and the percent of negative memories recalled (depressed group: r=−0.51, corrected p=0.004; remitted group: r=−0.56, corrected p=0.03).

Amygdala Functional Connectivity

Figures 4 and 5 show regions where the groups differed in functional connectivity with the amygdala during positive and negative memory recall. During positive memory recall (Figure 4), the depressed group had less amygdala connectivity with the right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the left and right posterior cingulate cortex than all of the other groups (whose amygdala connectivity was similar to each other). Conversely, the depressed group had higher amygdala connectivity with the left and right medial frontal polar cortex and the left superior temporal gyrus than the other groups. Amygdala connectivity with the left precuneus and the left middle and inferior temporal gyrus was greater in both healthy groups relative to each of the depression groups.
FIGURE 4. Group Differences in Amygdala Functional Connectivity During Positive Autobiographical Memory Recalla
a The graphs show correlation coefficients between the amygdala and regions recruited during positive autobiographical memory recall for each participant group. Coordinates correspond to the stereotaxic array by Talairach and Tournoux. ACC=anterior cingulate cortex; BA=Brodmann’s area; L=left; R=right; k=cluster size, which refers to the number of contiguous voxels for which the voxel F statistic corresponds to a corrected p<0.05.
FIGURE 5. Group Differences in Amygdala Functional Connectivity During Negative Autobiographical Memory Recalla
a The graphs show correlation coefficients between the amygdala and regions recruited during negative autobiographical memory recall for each participant group. Coordinates correspond to the stereotaxic array by Talairach and Tournoux. ACC=anterior cingulate cortex; BA=Brodmann’s area; L=left; R=right; PFC=prefrontal cortex; k=cluster size, which refers to the number of contiguous voxels for which the voxel F statistic corresponds to a corrected p<0.05.
During negative memory recall (Figure 5), amygdala connectivity was increased in the depressed group relative to the other three groups with all regions in Figure 5 (left dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; superior temporal gyrus and insula; left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, thalamus, and middle temporal gyrus; and right precuneus, inferior temporal gyrus, and amygdala) except the left medial frontal polar cortex, which showed lower amygdala connectivity in the depressed group. For the regions that showed the greatest amygdala connectivity in the depressed group, the connectivity was lowest in either the control group or the high-risk group, with the remitted group showing lower amygdala connectivity than the depressed group but higher than at least one of the healthy groups.

Discussion

This study demonstrates abnormal patterns of amygdala activity in depressed compared with control, high-risk, and remitted participants as they engage in autobiographical memory recall. While left amygdala activity was elevated in the depressed, remitted, and high-risk groups relative to the control group during negative specific memory recall, activity was lower in the depressed group compared with the other groups during positive memory recall.
The results suggest that amygdala hyperactivity during negative memory may be a trait-like marker of depression, as both vulnerable groups showed activity similar to that of the depressed group, and higher than that of the control group. In contrast, amygdala hypoactivity and reduced functional connectivity with regions of the salience and self-referential networks during positive specific memory recall may constitute a state marker of depression that manifests during active (symptomatic) illness and returns to normal with symptom remission. However, the present study employed a cross-sectional design, so the imaging differences may have been the result of unidentified dissimilarities between groups. Definitive conclusions regarding state/trait markers thus require longitudinal studies in which the same participants undergo scanning in different mood states.
The finding that depressed participants with lower self-reported and clinician-rated depressive symptoms had higher left amygdala activity during positive memory recall further suggests that amygdala hypoactivity to positive stimuli is clinically relevant. To the extent that the amygdala is important in maintaining the salience of an emotional cue (11), we hypothesize that low amygdala reactivity to positive memories reduces their salience and thus decreases the likelihood or frequency of their being recalled. Notably, amygdala connectivity with regions that form the salience network (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex) (32) and those implicated in self-referential processing (precuneus, posterior cingulate) (33) was reduced in the depressed group relative to all other groups during positive memory recall, while amygdala connectivity with regions involved in effortful reasoning within the domain of social cognition (frontal polar cortex) (34) was increased in the depressed group relative to the other groups, further supporting our hypothesis that positive memories are not rendered salient or personally relevant during positive memory recall and therefore patients expend less effort recalling them (and that abnormalities in positive processing are state markers of depression). The inverse correlation between amygdala activity during positive memory recall and the percent of negative memories recalled further suggests that patients who are able to recruit the amygdala during positive memory recall manifest an adaptive emotional processing bias shifted toward the positive (and away from the negative) valence.
Furthermore, we replicated the overgenerality effect, with the depressed (8) and vulnerable groups (9, 16) recalling fewer specific and more categorical memories relative to the control group. The greater statistical power of the present study revealed that depressed participants had more negative specific memories than control subjects in addition to fewer positive memories. There was a nominal trend in this direction in our previous studies (16, 35). These data suggest that depression is characterized by a bias toward specific negative memories. Clinically, if individuals spontaneously remember specific negative events but only have vague recollections of their positive experiences, they may spend more time contemplating negative than positive events (36), or sustain a cognitive style involving rumination on negative events (37). Including measures of rumination would enable more direct tests of this hypothesis. However, the hypothesis that negative events are more salient and easier to recall for depressed relative to nondepressed individuals is further supported by the findings that amygdala connectivity during negative memory recall was abnormally increased with regions of the salience network (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula) (32) and those involved in self-referential processing (precuneus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) (33, 38), but decreased with regions involved in effortful processing (frontal polar cortex) (34) in the depressed and remitted groups.
In both currently and formerly depressed participants, ratings of depression and anxiety decreased after completion of the autobiographical memory task and were associated with higher amygdala activity during positive memory recall. No such relationship was found between ratings and amygdala responses during negative specific memory recall. The significant decrease in anxiety ratings seen in both depression groups was evident only for the trait measure of anxiety on the STAI; no differences were found on the STAI state anxiety measure. Test-retest reliability on the STAI trait measure is 0.89 over a 2-month interval (28), which suggests that this indeed reflects a stable measure of anxiety. That both depression groups showed a significant decrease on this “stable” measure suggests that completing the task resulted in these individuals reevaluating their life in a less negative manner, consistent with anecdotal reports from depressed participants that they enjoyed performing the task. That this change was correlated with amygdala activity during positive memory recall suggests that those individuals with depression best able to recruit their amygdala during positive memory recall experience the most emotional benefit, and that it is the amygdala response to positive stimuli that is clinically relevant (as no correlation was found between mood change and amygdala responses to negative memories).
We have begun to further explore the hypothesis that amygdala hypoactivity is causally related to depression by using real-time fMRI neurofeedback to train depressed participants to up-regulate their amygdala response during positive memory recall. The results (21) supported our hypothesis: successfully learning to up-regulate the amygdala response to positive memories via real-time fMRI neurofeedback decreased STAI ratings of anxiety and POMS depression ratings and increased ratings of happiness in depressed participants. These results were observed after a single session of amygdala neurofeedback, suggesting beneficial effects for individuals in an acute depressive episode. Further testing of the clinical efficacy of this procedure as an intervention for depression is under way (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT02079610).

Conclusions

We have presented evidence that amygdala responses to emotional stimuli in depression are doubly dissociated from those in nondepressed individuals by virtue of an increased response to negative memories along with increased functional connectivity to regions implicated in salience and self-referential processing, and a decreased response to positive memories along with decreased functional connectivity in salience and self-referential processing regions. Amygdala hyperactivity to negative memory recall may thus constitute a trait-like marker of depression, as it is evident in at-risk groups. In contrast, amygdala hypoactivity to positive stimuli is only observed during the active illness state and is related to depressive symptoms and the lack of a normative positive processing bias during memory recall, potentially due to positive stimuli not recruiting the amygdala to a sufficient extent to render them sufficiently salient to generate a specific memory. These data suggest that treatments directly targeting amygdala hypoactivity to positive stimuli hold therapeutic potential for individuals with depression.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2015.15010119.ds001.pdf)
File (jan2016_amygdala.mp3)

References

1.
Rasmussen AS, Berntsen D: Emotional valence and the functions of autobiographical memories: positive and negative memories serve different functions. Mem Cognit 2009; 37:477–492
2.
Bluck S, Alea N: Exploring the functions of autobiographical memory: why do I remember the autumn? in Critical Advances in Reminiscence: From Theory to Application. Edited by Webster JD, Haight BK. New York, Springer, 2002
3.
Rusting CL, DeHart T: Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: consequences for mood-congruent memory. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000; 78:737–752
4.
Rubin DC, Berntsen D: Life scripts help to maintain autobiographical memories of highly positive, but not highly negative, events. Mem Cognit 2003; 31:1–14
5.
Young KD, Erickson K, Drevets WC: Match between cue and memory valence during autobiographical memory recall in depression. Psychol Rep 2012; 111:129–148
6.
Lemogne C, Piolino P, Friszer S, et al: Episodic autobiographical memory in depression: specificity, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective. Conscious Cogn 2006; 15:258–268
7.
Van Vreeswijk MF, De Wilde EJ: Autobiographical memory specificity, psychopathology, depressed mood and the use of the Autobiographical Memory Test: a meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther 2004; 42:731–743
8.
Williams JM, Barnhofer T, Crane C, et al: Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol Bull 2007; 133:122–148
9.
Young KD, Bellgowan PS, Bodurka J, et al: Neurophysiological correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in currently and formerly depressed subjects. Psychol Med 2014; 44:2951–2963
10.
Joormann J, Siemer M, Gotlib IH: Mood regulation in depression: differential effects of distraction and recall of happy memories on sad mood. J Abnorm Psychol 2007; 116:484–490
11.
Davis M, Whalen PJ: The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry 2001; 6:13–34
12.
Everitt BJ, Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, et al: Appetitive behavior: impact of amygdala-dependent mechanisms of emotional learning. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003; 985:233–250
13.
Svoboda E, McKinnon MC, Levine B: The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44:2189–2208
14.
Markowitsch HJ, Vandekerckhove MM, Lanfermann H, et al: Engagement of lateral and medial prefrontal areas in the ecphory of sad and happy autobiographical memories. Cortex 2003; 39:643–665
15.
Victor TA, Furey ML, Fromm SJ, et al: Relationship between amygdala responses to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:1128–1138
16.
Young KD, Bellgowan PS, Bodurka J, et al: Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in patients with depression and individuals at high risk for depression. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:698–708
17.
Merboldt KD, Fransson P, Bruhn H, et al: Functional MRI of the human amygdala? Neuroimage 2001; 14:253–257
18.
Poldrack RA: Region of interest analysis for fMRI. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2007; 2:67–70
19.
Ramel W, Goldin PR, Eyler LT, et al: Amygdala reactivity and mood-congruent memory in individuals at risk for depressive relapse. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:231–239
20.
Mackinger HF, Pachinger MM, Leibetseder MM, et al: Autobiographical memories in women remitted from major depression. J Abnorm Psychol 2000; 109:331–334
21.
Young KD, Zotev V, Phillips R, et al: Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of amygdala activity in patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS One 2014; 9:e88785
22.
Frank E, Prien RF, Jarrett RB, et al: Conceptualization and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder: remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:851–855
23.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, et al: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 2002
24.
Young KD, Bellgowan PS, Bodurka J, et al: Autobiographical deficits correlate with gray matter volume in depressed and high risk participants. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (Epub ahead of print, April 28, 2015)
25.
Young KD, Bellgowan PS, Bodurka J, et al: Functional neuroimaging correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in subjects at risk for depression. Brain Sci 2015; 5:144–164
26.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, Tex, Harcourt Assessment, 1999
27.
Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62
28.
Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RD: STAI Manual. Palo Alto, Calif, Consulting Psychologists Press, 1970
29.
McNair D, Lorr M, Dropplemen L: Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, Educational and Industrial Testing Service, 1971
30.
Williams JM, Dritschel BH: Emotional disturbance and the specificity of autobiographical memory. Cogn Emotion 1988; 2:221–234
31.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. New York, Thieme Medical Publishers, 1988
32.
Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, et al: Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 2007; 27:2349–2356
33.
Cavanna AE, Trimble MR: The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 2006; 129:564–583
34.
Moll J, de Oliveira-Souza R, Eslinger PJ: Morals and the human brain: a working model. Neuroreport 2003; 14:299–305
35.
Young KD, Erickson K, Nugent AC, et al: Functional anatomy of autobiographical memory recall deficits in depression. Psychol Med 2012; 42:345–357
36.
Schwartz RM, Caramoni GL: Cognitive balance and psychopathology: evaluation of an information processing model of positive and negative states of mind. Clin Psychol Rev 1989; 9:271–294
37.
Siegle GJ, Moore PM, Thase ME: Rumination: one construct, many features in healthy individuals, depressed individuals, and individuals with lupus. Cognit Ther Res 2004; 5:645–668
38.
Schmitz TW, Kawahara-Baccus TN, Johnson SC: Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 2004; 22:941–947

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 78 - 89
PubMed: 26541813

History

Received: 27 January 2015
Revision received: 5 June 2015
Accepted: 16 July 2015
Published online: 6 November 2015
Published in print: January 01, 2016

Authors

Details

Kymberly D. Young, Ph.D.
From the Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, Okla.; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh; the Biomedical Engineering Center, College of Engineering, University of Oklahoma, Norman; and Janssen Research and Development (of Johnson & Johnson), New Brunswick, N.J.
Greg J. Siegle, Ph.D.
From the Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, Okla.; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh; the Biomedical Engineering Center, College of Engineering, University of Oklahoma, Norman; and Janssen Research and Development (of Johnson & Johnson), New Brunswick, N.J.
Jerzy Bodurka, Ph.D.
From the Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, Okla.; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh; the Biomedical Engineering Center, College of Engineering, University of Oklahoma, Norman; and Janssen Research and Development (of Johnson & Johnson), New Brunswick, N.J.
Wayne C. Drevets, M.D.
From the Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, Okla.; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh; the Biomedical Engineering Center, College of Engineering, University of Oklahoma, Norman; and Janssen Research and Development (of Johnson & Johnson), New Brunswick, N.J.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Young ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

Dr. Drevets is an employee of and equity holder in Janssen Pharmaceuticals, LLC, of Johnson & Johnson, Inc. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Supported by the Laureate Institute for Brain Research and the William K. Warren Foundation.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share