Skip to main content
Full access
Articles
Published Online: 6 October 2015

Neural Correlates of the Propensity for Retaliatory Behavior in Youths With Disruptive Behavior Disorders

Abstract

Objective:

Youths with disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) (conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder) have an elevated risk for maladaptive reactive aggression. Theory suggests that this is due to an elevated sensitivity of basic threat circuitry implicated in retaliation (amygdala/periaqueductal gray) in youths with DBD and low levels of callous-unemotional traits and dysfunctional regulatory activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in youths with DBD irrespective of callous-unemotional traits.

Method:

A total of 56 youths 10–18 years of age (23 of them female) participated in the study: 30 youths with DBD, divided by median split into groups with high and low levels of callous-unemotional traits, and 26 healthy youths. All participants completed an ultimatum game task during functional MRI.

Results:

Relative to the other groups, youths with DBD and low levels of callous-unemotional traits showed greater increases in activation of basic threat circuitry when punishing others and dysfunctional down-regulation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during retaliation. Relative to healthy youths, all youths with DBD showed reduced amygdala-ventromedial prefrontal cortex connectivity during high provocation. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex responsiveness and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity were related to patients’ retaliatory propensity (behavioral responses during the task) and parent-reported reactive aggression.

Conclusions:

These data suggest differences in the underlying neurobiology of maladaptive reactive aggression in youths with DBD who have relatively low levels of callous-unemotional traits. Youths with DBD and low callous-unemotional traits alone showed significantly greater threat responses during retaliation relative to comparison subjects. These data also suggest that ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity is critical for regulating retaliation/reactive aggression and, when dysfunctional, contributes to reactive aggression, independent of level of callous-unemotional traits.
The term “reactive aggression” has been used to describe retaliatory actions in response to perceived threat, frustration, or provocation (1). Youths with disruptive behavior disorders (DBD), such as conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder, commonly display maladaptively higher levels of reactive aggression than youths without psychopathology (2, 3). Such high levels of aggression typically occur in the context of other forms of antisocial behavior (4), as well as decision-making deficits (5). In this study, we examined the neural correlates of retaliatory actions in healthy youths and youths with DBD.
Previous research has examined such neural correlates based on response to social provocation in variants of the ultimatum game (6). In one variant, a computer-simulated partner proposes to divide resources with participants. Participants accept or reject the division. Participants are more likely to reject an unfair offer if the partner could have offered a fair allocation, but the magnitude of this effect is reduced in incarcerated individuals (7, 8). Another ultimatum game variant involves participants choosing whether to “punish” the partner by spending money to remove the partner’s money. Such behaviors expressed in this variant serve to model retaliatory behavior in participants. Notably, increasing levels of retaliatory behavior are associated with increased activity in regions implicated in reactive aggression in animal studies (amygdala, periaqueductal gray) (912). Furthermore, neuroimaging studies implicate an inverse relationship between ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation and amygdala/periaqueductal gray response to basic threats (13) and provocation (9, 11). The ventromedial prefrontal cortex is thought to be critical for representing expected value (i.e., the subjective reward value that an individual associates with an action or object following learning [14]). Decreased ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation may reflect the representation of the diminishing expected value of increasing levels of retaliation, as increasing retaliation leads to diminishing monetary gains in the ultimatum game (9, 11). In threat induction paradigms, the increasing proximity of threat is associated with a diminishing likelihood of escape (and an increased likelihood of punishment); the value of future behavioral options diminishes as threat increases in proximity (13). We have suggested (15) that the increased risk for reactive aggression in patients with DBD reflects increased activity in threat response regions and/or dysfunction in ventromedial prefrontal cortex modulatory activity.
However, youths with DBD are heterogeneous. Some show elevated levels of callous-unemotional traits (DBD/+CU) (i.e., decreased guilt/empathy—referred to in DSM-5 as “with limited prosocial emotions”), while others do not (DBD/–CU). Youths with DBD/+CU and DBD/–CU have distinct developmental trajectories and neurobiology (15, 16). Both groups, however, show elevated levels of reactive aggression (3). The neurobiology of reactive aggression may be different for each group. We have hypothesized that youths with DBD/–CU show increased risk for reactive aggression because of heightened responsiveness of basic threat systems (amygdala/periaqueductal gray); the suggestion is that this increased responsiveness increases the probability that a given level of provocation will initiate fighting rather than flight or freezing (15). There is evidence that youths with DBD/–CU show an increased amygdala response to fearful expressions, whereas those with DBD/+CU show a decreased amygdala response (1719). Alternatively, there may be common neurobiological underpinnings of reactive aggression, which we have hypothesized reflect dysfunction in the modulatory role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in representing an action’s expected value (15). Expected value representation is disrupted in patients with DBD irrespective of level of callous-unemotional traits (5, 20). In the context of an ultimatum game, we expect that youths with DBD will fail to appropriately represent the diminishing value of outcomes as retaliation increases, leading to an increased propensity to retaliate. Here, we examine these possibilities through an investigation of retaliatory behavior and its neural underpinnings in the context of an ultimatum game.
This study tested four predictions: First, retaliatory propensity, measured by mean levels of punishment during the ultimatum game, would have a significantly greater association with reactive than proactive aggression. Second, the increase in amygdala/periaqueductal gray activity when retaliating would be exaggerated in youths with DBD/–CU relative to healthy youths, and the level of activity in these regions would be positively associated with reactive aggression. Third, the typical decrease in ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity when retaliating and in ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala functional connectivity would both be attenuated in youths with DBD/–CU and DBD/+CU relative to healthy youths. Fourth, the level of attenuation of modulated response and the connectivity during high provocation/retaliation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex would be negatively associated with retaliatory propensity.

Method

Participants

A total of 56 youths 10–18 years of age (mean=14.74 years, SD=2.13), 23 of them female, participated: 30 youths with DBD and 26 healthy youths. Consistent with previous work (17), a median split on Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits score (median=43.5) was utilized to form DBD/+CU (mean=46.8, SD=3.57) and DBD/–CU (mean=33.9, SD=6.38) groups. The groups did not differ significantly in age, IQ, or gender composition (Table 1). Half of the youths with DBD (N=15) were comorbid for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and medications could not be withheld during scanning for 23.3% (N=7) of the DBD group. Youths were recruited from the community. After receiving a complete description of the study, children and parents provided written informed assent or consent. The NIH Institutional Review Board approved the study. Inclusion and exclusion criteria are listed in the supplemental methods section of the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article.
TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics and Performance Measures on the Social Fairness Game for Healthy Youths and Youths With Disruptive Behavior Disorders (DBD)
MeasureHealthy Youths (N=26)Youths With DBD (N=30)
 MeanSDMeanSDtp
Age (years)14.302.1915.042.091.130.26
IQ98.969.9996.3710.520.940.35
Reactive aggressiona0.850.973.971.439.43<0.01
Proactive aggressiona0.080.272.572.016.25<0.01
 N%N%χ2p
Female1246.21136.70.520.47
Medicated during scanning00.0723.36.93<0.01
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder00.01550.017.76<0.01
Conduct disorder00.02273.39.29<0.01
Oppositional defiant disorder  930.032.87<0.01
 MeanSDMeanSDtp
 Punishment dollars spentPunishment dollars spent  
Offer unfairness level      
 $10/$100.170.270.190.350.300.77
 $14/$61.170.711.560.781.980.05
 $16/$41.630.701.860.661.250.22
 $18/$22.180.622.250.700.360.72
 Time to retaliation (ms)Time to retaliation (ms)  
 $10/$10918.81235.12931.59297.960.180.86
 $14/$61121.67289.501174.56475.170.490.62
 $16/$41179.67293.671214.88452.250.340.74
 $18/$21019.54333.671112.19452.370.860.39
a
Reactive and proactive aggression scores are from the Reactive/Proactive Aggression Rating Scale.

Study Measures

Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits.

This 24-item parent-report scale (21), designed to assess callous-unemotional traits in youths, was derived from the callous-unemotional scale of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (22), which has been used in various youth samples. The construct validity of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits has been supported in community and juvenile justice samples (23, 24).

Reactive/Proactive Aggression Rating Scale.

Parents completed this six-item measure (1). Dodge and Coie (1) found that a three-item reactive aggression scale and a three-item proactive aggression scale yielded the best-fitting model. This measure was found to be useful in distinguishing aggression subtypes in children (1) and adolescents (25).

The social fairness game.

Participants were presented with a version of the ultimatum game, the social fairness game, which has been described elsewhere (11) (Figure 1; see also the online data supplement). Participants are offered either a fair ($10 to participant; $10 to partner) or an unfair ($6/$4/$2 to participant; $14/$16/$18 to partner) division of a $20 pot. Participants could then either accept the offer or reject it and, by spending $1, $2, or $3, punish the partner; each punishment dollar spent by the participant caused the partner to lose $7 from the $20 pot (Figure 1). Retaliatory propensity was defined as the participant’s average punishment level.
FIGURE 1. The Social Fairness Gamea
a Participants were presented with either a fair (A) or an unfair (B, C, and D) division $20. After a jittered interval, participants chose, via button press, either to accept the offer (E) or to retaliate by spending $1–$3 of a pot of punishment money. For every punishment dollar spent in retaliation, the offerer lost $7 (F, G, and H). In this example, (E) shows the outcome of accepting a fair offer, but unfair offers can also be accepted.

MRI Parameters and Preprocessing

Participants were scanned using a 3-T GE scanner, and data were analyzed using AFNI (26). Specific parameters have been reported elsewhere (11) and are also listed in the online data supplement.

General Linear Model Analysis

The model involved six motion regressors and the following task regressors: indicator functions for the offer phase, the decision phase, the outcome phase, the offer phase multiplied by offer unfairness (scored as 0 [fair: $10 kept and $10 offered to partner], 8 [unfair: $14/$6], 12 [unfair: $16/$4], 16 [unfair: $18/$2]), and the decision phase multiplied by the sample average punishment level at each level of unfairness (average response to $10/$10, 1.176; to $14/$6, 2.358; to $16/$4, 2.726; to $18/$2, 3.230). All regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function from the onset of the offer, decision, and outcome phases to account for the slow hemodynamic response. In accordance with findings that normalization of brain volumes from age 7–8 onward does not introduce major age-related distortions in localization or time course of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal in event-related functional MRI (fMRI) (27, 28), the participants’ anatomical scans were individually registered to the Talairach and Tournoux atlas (29). Participants’ functional echo planar image data were then registered to their Talairach anatomical scans. Linear regression modeling was performed using the five regressors described earlier, plus regressors to model a first-order baseline drift function. This produced beta coefficients and associated t statistics for each voxel and regressor.

fMRI Data Analysis

A three (diagnosis: DBD/+CU, DBD/–CU, healthy) by two (task phase: offer phase, decision phase) repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on the modulated regression coefficients. The AFNI ClustSim program was used to establish a family-wise-error-corrected threshold (594-mm3 clusters at p=0.005, corrected to p=0.05) for a whole-brain analysis. Because of their small size and/or theoretical importance, small-volume corrections for multiple comparisons were calculated for the amygdala, periaqueductal gray, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. The amygdala small-volume correction, calculated using an anatomically defined mask (Eickhoff-Zilles Architectonic Atlas: 50% probability) (30), yielded a threshold of 162 mm3 at an initial significance threshold of 0.02. As anatomically defined masks were not available in AFNI, small-volume corrections for the periaqueductal gray and ventromedial prefrontal cortex were calculated using 10-mm spheres centered on the peak coordinates from previous work (periaqueductal gray (x, y, z): 3, −23, −4; ventromedial prefrontal cortex: −4, 36, −5) (13) and yielded a threshold of 248 mm3 at an initial significance threshold of 0.02 for both regions. For these analyses, average percent signal change was measured across all voxels within each region of interest generated from the functional masks, and post hoc testing was conducted in SPSS (IBM, Armonk, N.Y.).
Generalized psychophysiological interaction analyses (31) were conducted to investigate differences in functional connectivity associated with DBD during the ultimatum game. As a parametric modulation strategy is incompatible with functional connectivity analyses, functional connectivity was compared using two three (diagnosis: DBD/+CU, DBD/–CU, healthy) by two (provocation level: low [fair/accept], high [unfair/punished]) by two (task phase: offer phase, decision phase) repeated-measures ANOVAs. The anatomically defined amygdala masks mentioned above were seed regions. (See the online data supplement for details.)

Results

Behavioral Results

The relationship between retaliatory propensity (average punishment on the ultimatum game) and reactive aggression was significantly stronger than the relationship between retaliatory propensity and proactive aggression (whole group: r=0.26 and r=0.04, respectively; Steiger’s Z=2.13, p=0.03; patients with DBD only: r=0.37 and r=−0.13, respectively; Z=2.62, p<0.01). Additionally, among youths with DBD, there was a significantly stronger relationship between time to retaliation and reactive aggression relative to proactive aggression (r=−0.59 and r=−0.25, respectively; Z=2.00, p<0.05). Callous-unemotional traits among the youths with DBD were significantly related to proactive (r=0.45, p=0.01), but not reactive aggression (r=0.19, p=0.33), although these correlations were not significantly different (Z=1.44, p=0.15).
A four (offer unfairness: $10/$10, $14/$6, $16/$4, $18/$2) by two (diagnosis: DBD, healthy) repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the choice data (i.e., level of punishment selected: $0, $1, $2, $3). A significant main effect of offer was observed (F=420.93, df=3, 54, p<0.01). Participants increased retaliation as offer unfairness increased. Neither the main effect of diagnosis nor the unfairness level-by-diagnosis interaction was significant. However, a significant quadratic unfairness level-by-diagnosis interaction was observed (F=4.51, df=3, 54, p=0.04). While healthy youths and those with DBD were equally likely to accept fair offers and punished very unfair offers ($16/$4 and $18/$2) equally harshly, youths with DBD punished slightly unfair offers ($14/$6) more harshly than did healthy youths (Table 1).

fMRI Results

The groups did not differ significantly in movement during scanning.
A three (diagnosis: DBD/+CU, DBD/–CU, healthy) by two (task phase: offer phase, decision phase) repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the BOLD data modulated by offer unfairness in the offer phase and punishment level in the decision phase (Table 2; for complete results, see the supplemental results section and Tables S1–S3 in the online data supplement).
TABLE 2. Brain Regions Demonstrating Differential Functional Connectivity in Healthy Youths (N=28) and Youths With Disruptive Behavior Disorders and Low (N=15) or High (N=15) Levels of Callous-Unemotional Traits: Diagnosis-by-Provocation Level Analyses
 Peak Activationb   
RegionaSideBAxyzFpVoxels
Left amygdala        
Dorsomedial prefrontal gyrusLeft6–4.54.556.515.74<0.0001132
Right amygdala        
Ventromedial prefrontal cortexRight32/101.543.55.59.340.000322
Dorsomedial prefrontal cortexLeft8–4.528.544.511.98<0.000170
Middle temporal cortexRight3752.5–61.55.510.20<0.000150
Superior temporal gyrusRight2158.5–40.5–0.513.68<0.0001100
Paracentral/cingulate cortexRight24/3225.5–40.550.511.36<0.0001551
Parahippocampal gyrusRight 37.5–22.5–12.512.940.000240
Fusiform gyrusRight3728.5–46.5–12.512.410.000830
Postcentral gyrusLeft3–25.5–31.553.513.28<0.000198
DecliveRight 37.5–70.5–21.512.88<0.000189
ClaustrumLeft13–37.5–22.5–0.512.41<0.000188
Precentral gyrusLeft6–37.5–4.553.59.190.000473
DecliveLeft –49.5–58.5–21.513.31<0.000172
CuneusRight1928.5–82.529.58.510.000640
Precentral gyrusRight431.5–19.538.58.29<0.000130
a
According to the Talairach Daemon Atlas (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/tal-daemon/).
b
Based on the Tournoux and Talairach standard brain template. BA=Brodmann’s area.

Region-of-Interest Results

Amygdala.

A significant main effect of diagnosis was observed (Figure 2). Youths with DBD/–CU showed significantly greater right amygdala (coordinates: 23, −8, −6; k=10) responses relative to healthy youths (t=3.33, p<0.01) and youths with DBD/+CU (t=2.84, p<0.01), who did not differ significantly from each other. Among the youths with DBD, modulated amygdala response was inversely associated with callous-unemotional traits (r=−0.49, p<0.01], but not with reactive aggression.
FIGURE 2. Regions of Basic Threat Circuitry Showing Group Differences Between Healthy Youths (N=28) and Youths With Disruptive Behavior Disorders and Low (N=15) or High (N=15) Levels of Callous-Unemotional Traitsa
a DBD/+CU=disruptive behavior disorders with high levels of callous-unemotional traits; DBD/–CU=disruptive behavior disorders with low levels of callous-unemotional traits. In panel A, during the task, healthy youths showed increased modulated activation in the right amygdala relative to youths with DBD/+CU and DBD/–CU. In panel B, during the decision phase but not the offer phase, youths with DBD/–CU showed increased activation as a function of retaliation in the periaqueductal gray relative to youths with DBD/+CU and healthy youths. Asterisks indicate post hoc tests that were significant at p<0.05.

Periaqueductal gray.

A significant diagnosis-by-task phase interaction was observed in the periaqueductal gray (coordinates: 10, −21, 4; k=6). During the decision phase, youths with DBD/–CU showed a significantly greater modulated periaqueductal gray activation relative to healthy youths (t=2.14, p=0.04) and youths with DBD/+CU (t=2.23, p=0.03), who did not differ significantly from each other. There were no significant between-group differences in the offer phase (Figure 2).

Ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

A significant main effect of diagnosis was observed, in which youths with DBD/–CU showed significantly reduced attenuation of modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex (coordinates: −11, 33, −2; k=13) response relative to healthy youths (t=3.228, p=0.003). Youths with DBD/+CU did not differ significantly from youths with DBD/–CU or healthy youths (Figure 3).
FIGURE 3. Group Differences in the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Between Healthy Youths (N=28) and Youths With Disruptive Behavior Disorders and Low (N=15) or High (N=15) Levels of Callous-Unemotional Traitsa
a DBD/+CU=disruptive behavior disorders with high levels of callous-unemotional traits; DBD/–CU=disruptive behavior disorders with low levels of callous-unemotional traits; vmPFC=ventromedial prefrontal cortex. In panel A, youths with DBD/–CU showed less reduction in ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation as a function of retaliation relative to healthy youths and youths with DBD/+CU. In panel B, healthy youths showed increased functional connectivity during high provocation trials relative to youths with DBD/–CU and DBD/+CU. In panel C, a mediation analysis indicated that attenuated modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation mediated the relationship between reactive aggression and retaliatory propensity. In panel D, another mediation analysis indicated that ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala functional connectivity mediated the relationship between reactive aggression and retaliatory propensity. Asterisks indicate post hoc tests that were significant at p<0.05.

Generalized Psychophysiological Interaction Analyses

A significant diagnosis-by-provocation level interaction was observed in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex using the right amygdala seed (Figure 3; see also the online data supplement). During high provocation conditions, greater functional connectivity was observed between the right amygdala and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in healthy youths relative to both youths with DBD/–CU (t=2.98, p<0.01) and those with DBD/+CU (t=2.16, p=0.04), who did not differ significantly from each other.

BOLD Response and Behavior

Retaliatory propensity was inversely associated with attenuation of modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex response (r=−0.44, p<0.01) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity during high provocation/retaliation conditions (r=−0.34, p=0.01). Given the significant relationship between retaliatory propensity and both modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex response and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala functional connectivity, a hierarchical linear regression analysis was used to determine whether the modulated and connectivity data were independently related to retaliatory propensity beyond reactive aggression and callous-unemotional traits. Both modulated BOLD response (β=0.36, ΔR2=0.11, p<0.01) and functional connectivity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (β=−0.29, ΔR2=0.09, p=0.02) were observed to contribute unique variance in the prediction of retaliatory propensity.
Attenuation of modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex response and retaliatory propensity were both significantly correlated with reactive aggression (r=−0.39 and r=0.26, respectively, p<0.05), although the correlation of ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity with reactive aggression fell short of significance (r=−0.24, p=0.08). The relationship between retaliatory propensity and reactive aggression was not significant when controlling for attenuation in modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex response or ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity. However, the relationships between retaliatory propensity and attenuated ventromedial prefrontal cortex response (rpartial=−0.38, p<0.01) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity (rpartial=−0.29, p=0.03) remained significant after controlling for reactive aggression and callous-unemotional traits. This suggests that modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex response and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity mediate (32) the association between reactive aggression and retaliatory propensity.

Confounding Factors

Among the youths with DBD, medication could not be withheld during scanning for 23.3% (N=7), and 50% (N=15) had comorbid ADHD. Therefore, the preceding analyses were rerun with these youths removed from the sample. In all regions, activations in similar regions were observed when only unmedicated youths were included and when youths with comorbid ADHD and DBD were excluded (see Tables S4–S7 in the online data supplement).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to link retaliatory propensity to reactive aggression in youths with DBD. In addition, it provided the first demonstration of increased periaqueductal gray responding to provocation in youths with DBD/–CU, but not those with DBD/+CU. Critically, this study demonstrated the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala functional connectivity in regulating retaliatory behavior. Furthermore, these results suggest that disruption in ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala functional connectivity accounts for the common risk of increased retaliation/reactive aggression in DBD irrespective of callous-unemotional traits.
In line with our first prediction, retaliatory propensity on our ultimatum game task was significantly more positively associated with propensity for reactive relative to proactive aggression. Patients with DBD retaliated more severely during an ultimatum game, particularly to slightly unfair offers, than healthy youths. These data are consistent with other behavioral findings in antisocial adolescents (8). In contrast, level of callous-unemotional traits did not predict level of reactive aggression (although it was associated with proactive aggression), consistent with previous research (3).
According to our position (15), while youths with DBD/+CU and DBD/–CU both show increased retaliatory behavior, the neurobiological underpinnings of the increase differ by level of callous-unemotional traits. Consistent with our second prediction, youths with DBD/–CU showed exaggerated basic threat circuitry (periaqueductal gray/amygdala) activation (33) as a function of level of retaliation relative to youths with DBD/+CU and healthy youths. These data extend previous work reporting increased amygdala responses to provocation in youths with DBD/–CU relative to youths with DBD/+CU and healthy youths (1719). According to our position, this heightened basic threat circuit sensitivity increases the likelihood of reactive aggression (rather than freezing or flight) in response to a threatening or provocative stimulus (15). However, it should be noted that neither amygdala nor periaqueductal gray response predicted general propensity for retaliation in the task or reactive aggression levels more generally. Whether this reflects a type II error or is an indication that this pathology is more related to other associated symptomatology (e.g., anxiety) will be determined in future work.
Our third prediction was that youths with DBD, irrespective of level of callous-unemotional traits, are at increased risk for reactive aggression because of dysfunction in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex’s putative role in instrumentally selecting the form of retaliatory behavior as a function of expected rewards or punishments (5, 14, 34). It is possible, however, that ventromedial prefrontal cortex responsiveness on this task might alternatively represent other putative roles for this structure—for example, direct suppression of the amygdala (35), self-referential processing (36), and representation of social and emotional structured event complexes (37). We predicted that patients with DBD would show less reduction in ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity as a function of punishment level (i.e., less representation of the cost of retaliating) (11). This hypothesis was confirmed for patients with DBD/–CU, but not those with DBD/+CU. However, patients in both DBD groups showed reduced functional connectivity between the right amygdala and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during high provocation trials relative to healthy youths. This suggests a failure in the appropriate interaction of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in patients with DBD, irrespective of level of callous-unemotional traits, when responding to high provocation. In line with our fourth prediction, both modulated ventromedial prefrontal cortex response and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala functional connectivity contributed unique variance in the prediction of retaliatory propensity, and a mediation analysis supported the critical role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in regulating retaliation and reactive aggression. These data suggest that ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity is critical for regulating retaliation and reactive aggression, describing putative common impairment in youths with DBD/+CU and DBD/–CU that contributes to retaliatory behavior.
Several caveats should be considered with respect to these data. First, an ADHD comparison group was not included, although a group analysis excluding youths with DBD and comorbid ADHD revealed similar results, with similar activations for all contrasts. Second, the medications of two youths with DBD could not be withheld for scanning; here again, group analyses excluding these participants identified similar regions for all contrasts. Third, the three-group analyses utilized relatively small samples. Thus, it is possible that type II error accounts for the failure of youths with DBD/+CU to show the hypothesized problems in BOLD response in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex as a function of punishment level (although this group did show the predicted reduced ventromedial prefrontal cortex-amygdala connectivity in response to high provocation). Our results in this study will need to be replicated with larger samples. Fourth, the periaqueductal gray is a small structure that is not expressly defined in AFNI’s probability maps. Therefore, we cannot be certain that the signal detected in the periaqueductal gray is exclusively from that structure; neighboring structures may also be involved.
In summary, three features of this study are particularly important for our understanding of disruptive disorders: First, this was the first study to document increased periaqueductal gray response to provocation in youths with DBD/–CU, but not in those with DBD/+CU. These data suggest that interventions designed to reduce emotional responsiveness would be most efficiently applied only to youths with DBD/–CU. Second, our results highlight the critical role of the ventromedial frontal cortex in the regulation of retaliatory behavior and that this structure is dysfunctional (at least its interaction with the amygdala) in patients with DBD irrespective of level of callous-unemotional traits. This dysfunction would appear to represent an important treatment target for future interventions. And third, level of ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction in the ultimatum game task predicted level of reactive aggression in youths with DBD. This is important, as no fMRI markers of reactive aggression have been identified previously. Moreover, it is important to remember that the dysfunction seen here in patients with DBD is likely seen in patients with other psychiatric disorders who are at risk for maladaptively increased levels of reactive aggression, for example, posttraumatic stress disorder, borderline personality disorder, and disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (38). Thus, this study provides a marker task of potential relevance to researchers who are concerned about maladaptively increased levels of reactive aggression both in patients with DBD and in those with other disorders.

Footnote

ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT00104039.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2015.15020250.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Dodge KA, Coie JD: Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987; 53:1146–1158
2.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2013
3.
Frick PJ, Dickens C: Current perspectives on conduct disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2006; 8:59–72
4.
Frick PJ, Stickle TR, Dandreaux DM, et al: Callous-unemotional traits in predicting the severity and stability of conduct problems and delinquency. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2005; 33:471–487
5.
White SF, Pope K, Sinclair S, et al: Disrupted expected value and prediction error signaling in youths with disruptive behavior disorders during a passive avoidance task. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170:315–323
6.
Güth W, Schmittberger R, Schwarze B: An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J Econ Behav Organ 1982; 3:367–388
7.
Radke S, Brazil IA, Scheper I, et al: Unfair offers, unfair offenders? Fairness considerations in incarcerated individuals with and without psychopathy. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:406
8.
van den Bos W, Vahl P, Güroğlu B, et al: Neural correlates of social decision-making in severely antisocial adolescents. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 9:2059–2066
9.
White SF, Brislin SJ, Meffert H, et al: Callous-unemotional traits modulate the neural response associated with punishing another individual during social exchange: a preliminary investigation. J Pers Disord 2013; 27:99–112
10.
Nelson RJ, Trainor BC: Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007; 8:536–546
11.
White SF, Brislin SJ, Sinclair S, et al: Punishing unfairness: rewarding or the organization of a reactively aggressive response? Hum Brain Mapp 2014; 35:2137–2147
12.
Krämer UM, Jansma H, Tempelmann C, et al: Tit-for-tat: the neural basis of reactive aggression. Neuroimage 2007; 38:203–211
13.
Mobbs D, Petrovic P, Marchant JL, et al: When fear is near: threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science 2007; 317:1079–1083
14.
O’Doherty JP: Contributions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to goal-directed action selection. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2011; 1239:118–129
15.
Blair RJ, Leibenluft E, Pine DS: Conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits in youth. N Engl J Med 2014; 371:2207–2216
16.
Frick PJ, White SF: Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49:359–375
17.
Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR, et al: Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:1109–1116
18.
Lozier LM, Cardinale EM, VanMeter JW, et al: Mediation of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71:627–636
19.
White SF, Marsh AA, Fowler KA, et al: Reduced amygdala response in youths with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits: decreased emotional response versus increased top-down attention to nonemotional features. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:750–758
20.
White SF, Fowler KA, Sinclair S, et al: Disrupted expected value signaling in youth with disruptive behavior disorders to environmental reinforcers. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2014; 53:579–588
21.
Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Skeem JL, et al: Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: validation of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. Int J Law Psychiatry 2008; 31:241–251
22.
Frick PH, Hare RD: The Antisocial Process Screening Device. Toronto, Multi-Health Systems, 2001
23.
Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Skeem JL, et al: Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: validation of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. Int J Law Psychiatry 2008; 31:241–252
24.
Essau CA, Sasagawa S, Frick PJ: Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment 2006; 13:454–469
25.
Pellegrini AD, Bartini M: Dominance in early adolescent boys: affiliative and aggressive dimensions and possible functions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology 2001; 47:142–163
26.
Cox RW: AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 1996; 29:162–173
27.
Kang HC, Burgund ED, Lugar HM, et al: Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space. Neuroimage 2003; 19:16–28
28.
Burgund ED, Kang HC, Kelly JE, et al: The feasibility of a common stereotactic space for children and adults in fMRI studies of development. Neuroimage 2002; 17:184–200
29.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Stuttgart, Thieme, 1988
30.
Amunts K, Kedo O, Kindler M, et al: Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region, and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps. Anat Embryol (Berl) 2005; 210:343–352
31.
McLaren DG, Ries ML, Xu G, et al: A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. Neuroimage 2012; 61:1277–1286
32.
Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986; 51:1173–1182
33.
Gregg TR, Siegel A: Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: implications for human aggression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2001; 25:91–140
34.
Rubia K, Smith AB, Halari R, et al: Disorder-specific dissociation of orbitofrontal dysfunction in boys with pure conduct disorder during reward and ventrolateral prefrontal dysfunction in boys with pure ADHD during sustained attention. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:83–94
35.
Motzkin JC, Philippi CL, Wolf RC, et al: Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 77:276–284
36.
Kelley WM, Macrae CN, Wyland CL, et al: Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 2002; 14:785–794
37.
Moll J, Zahn R, de Oliveira-Souza R, et al: Opinion: the neural basis of human moral cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005; 6:799–809
38.
Cuthbert BN, Insel TR: Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med 2013; 11:126

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 282 - 290
PubMed: 26441155

History

Received: 25 February 2015
Accepted: 27 July 2015
Published online: 6 October 2015
Published in print: March 01, 2016

Authors

Details

Stuart F. White, Ph.D.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Michelle VanTieghem, B.A.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Sarah J. Brislin, B.A.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Isaiah Sypher, B.S.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Stephen Sinclair, Ph.D.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Daniel S. Pine, M.D.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Soonjo Hwang, M.D.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
R. James R. Blair, Ph.D.
From the Section on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience and the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York; and the Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. White ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Supported by the Intramural Research Program of NIMH (grant 1-ZIA-MH002860, principal investigator, Dr. Blair; and grant 1-ZIA-MH002782, principal investigator, Dr. Pine).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share