Skip to main content
Full access
Articles
Published Online: 14 October 2021

Association Between Systemic Inflammation and Individual Symptoms of Depression: A Pooled Analysis of 15 Population-Based Cohort Studies

Abstract

Objective:

Evidence from anti-inflammatory drug trials for the treatment of depression has been inconsistent. This may be ascribed to the differing symptom-specific effects of inflammation. Accordingly, the authors explored the associations between systemic inflammation and an array of individual symptoms of depression across multiple studies.

Methods:

This random-effects pooled analysis included 15 population-based cohorts and 56,351 individuals age 18 years and older. Serum or plasma concentrations of C-reactive protein (CRP) and interleukin-6 (IL-6) were measured at baseline. Using validated self-report measures, 24 depressive symptoms were ascertained in 15 cross-sectional studies, and, in seven cohorts, were also assessed at follow-up (mean follow-up period, 3.2 years).

Results:

The prevalence of depressive symptoms ranged from 1.1% (suicidal ideation) to 21.5% (sleep problems). In cross-sectional analyses, higher concentrations of CRP were robustly associated with an increased risk of experiencing four physical symptoms (changes in appetite, felt everything was an effort, loss of energy, sleep problems) and one cognitive symptom (little interest in doing things). These associations remained after adjustment for sociodemographic variables, behavioral factors, and chronic conditions; in sex- and age-stratified analyses; in longitudinal analyses; when using IL-6 as the inflammatory marker of interest; in depressed individuals; and after excluding chronically ill individuals. For four exclusively emotional symptoms (bothered by things, hopelessness about the future, felt fearful, life had been a failure), the overall evidence was strongly against an association with inflammation.

Conclusions:

These findings suggest symptom-specific rather than generalized effects of systemic inflammation on depression. Future trials exploring anti-inflammatory treatment regimens for depression may benefit from targeting individuals presenting with symptom profiles characterized by distinct inflammation-related physical and cognitive symptoms.
It is well documented that depression is a growing public health concern and a major cause of disability worldwide (1), leading to a significant reduction in quality of life (2) and impaired psychosocial functioning (3). In addition to being an important condition in its own right, depression has been associated with an elevated risk of morbidity (4), cognitive decline (5), and mortality (6). However, the pathophysiology of depression is not fully understood. Approximately one-third of patients with depression fail to respond to conventional antidepressant therapies, and less than 40% obtain remission after initial treatment (7). Furthermore, there have been no clinically reliable predictors of treatment response to existing antidepressant treatment regimens (8).
Following the extensive search for biomarkers linked to depression, there has been emerging interest in the role of immune system disturbances, in particular systemic inflammation, in depression etiology. This view is supported by findings on shared genetic variants between the immune system and depression (9). In addition, proinflammatory systemic cytokines have been found to be capable of affecting depression-related endocrine functioning and neurotransmitter metabolism by traversing the blood-brain barrier or inducing activation of afferent fibers of the vagus nerve (10, 11), including the central synthesis and reuptake of amine transmitters (12, 13). To date, however, the collective evidence from population-based cohort studies and clinical trials on the association between systemic inflammation and depression has been inconsistent (1418). Since depression is a multifaceted mental disorder with varying types of symptom expressions (19), this discordance may be ascribed to symptom-specific effects of inflammation that are lost when a single aggregate measure of depression is used (20). According to the two most widely used classificatory diagnostic systems of mental disorders—ICD-11 (21) and DSM-5 (22)—depression can be broadly classified into emotional (e.g., depressed mood, anhedonia), cognitive (e.g., difficulties concentrating), and physical (e.g., sleep problems, fatigue, changes in appetite) symptoms. Different symptoms may have distinct underlying etiological pathways, but few studies to date have examined the associations between systemic inflammation and individual symptoms of depression (3, 2327). Further limitations in this field of research include the reliance on small sample sizes, insufficient control for potential confounding factors, and a lack of evaluation of temporality and consistency of potential symptom-specific associations across different subgroups and inflammatory markers.
In this multicohort study of 15 population-based cohorts comprising up to 56,351 individuals, we sought to address these limitations by exploring the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of two systemic inflammatory markers—C-reactive protein (CRP) and interleukin-6 (IL-6)—with 24 individual symptoms of depression, including physical, emotional, and cognitive symptom domains, and biased perceptions of self. To evaluate the robustness of evidence for or against an association with individual symptoms of depression, we explored these associations in subgroups of men and women, subgroups of younger and older adults, among individuals with depression, and after excluding those with high levels of inflammation or chronic illnesses.

METHODS

Study Population

We identified eligible large-scale cohort studies on inflammation and depressive symptoms by searching the collections of the UK Data Service (https://ukdataservice.ac.uk), the Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research (http://www.icpsr.umich.edu/icpsrweb/ICPSR/), and the Individual-Participant Data Meta-Analysis in Working Populations (IPD-Work) consortium (28). Study selection was based on three quality assessment criteria: first, studies provided individual-level data for adults; second, studies used validated assessment methods to measure circulating inflammatory biomarkers, depressive symptoms, and covariates; and third, studies adopted either a cross-sectional or a longitudinal (i.e., at least two waves of data collection) design.
As shown in Figure S1 in the online supplement, we identified 15 independent population-based cohort studies, which were initiated between 1985 and 2018. These were from the United Kingdom (Whitehall II, the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing [ELSA], and Understanding Society [UKHLS]), the United States (the Health and Retirement Study [HRS], the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey [NHANES], Midlife in the United States [MIDUS], and the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project [NSHAP]), Ireland (the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing [TILDA]), Mexico (the Mexican Health and Aging Study [MHAS]), Taiwan (the Social and Biomarkers of Aging Study [SEBAS]), and Costa Rica (the Costa Rican Longevity and Healthy Aging Study [CRELES]). Participants under age 18 and those with missing data on depressive symptoms, inflammatory markers, and/or covariates were excluded from the present analyses.
Ethical approval for the included studies was granted by the relevant local or institutional ethical review boards. All participants provided written informed consent prior to their participation in these studies. The downloaded data were anonymous. This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for cohort studies.

Assessment of Systemic Inflammation and Baseline Covariates

Plasma or serum blood samples were used to assess baseline levels of CRP and IL-6 using standard operating protocols as detailed in appendix 1 in the online supplement. The selection of demographic, socioeconomic, behavioral, and chronic illness–related covariates was based on previous research (29). Demographic variables included age and sex. Educational qualification was used as a single indicator of socioeconomic position. Behavioral factors included self-reported smoking status (never smoker, ex-smoker, current smoker), alcohol consumption (frequency of drinking alcohol), physical activity (physically active or not active), and body mass index (BMI). Chronic illness–related covariates comprised self-reported indications of coronary heart disease, stroke, diabetes, and cancer (yes or no). In addition, we included systolic and diastolic blood pressure.
In sensitivity analyses of cohort studies with relevant data, additional covariates included adverse childhood experiences (a standardized sum score) and the time interval between blood collection and measurement of depressive symptoms at baseline (see appendix 1 in the online supplement) (30, 31).

Assessment of Depressive Symptoms

Overall depression status (i.e., elevated versus nonelevated levels of depressive symptoms) and a total of 24 individual symptoms of depression were ascertained from a variety of validated self-report measures of depressive symptoms, including the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (32) (cohorts ELSA, TILDA, Whitehall II, NSHAP, MIDUS, SEBAS, MHAS, HRS), the Depression Screening Questionnaire, based on the Patient Health Questionnaire (33) (NHANES), the General Health Questionnaire (34) (UKHLS), and the Geriatric Depression Scale (35) (CRELES). Standard (most commonly) or distribution-based threshold values were used to classify individuals with high overall depressive symptoms in each cohort (see Table S1 in the online supplement). The questionnaires assessed how often participants had experienced specific depressive symptoms during the past 7–14 days. Accordingly, respondents were asked whether they had experienced crying spells, changes in appetite, little interest in doing things, effort doing things, low energy levels, low mood, feelings of sadness, feelings of loneliness, sleep problems, trouble concentrating, hopelessness about the future, moving or speaking slowly or too fast, talking less than usual, were bothered by things, felt fearful, felt life had been a failure, felt bad about themselves, felt people disliked them, felt people were unfriendly, did not enjoy life, felt they would be better off dead, and could not shake off the blues. Response scales varied by measure and study and were therefore harmonized by coding items as dichotomous variables (coded 1 if the symptom was present and 0 if it was absent). Our domain classification of symptoms was informed by ICD-11 (21), DSM-5 (22), and a previous mixed-methods investigation on depression outcome domains that matter to patients, caregivers, and health care professionals (19). Symptoms were categorized as detailed in Table 1. In seven cohorts, depressive symptoms were measured repeatedly, at baseline when inflammatory markers were assessed, and 1 to 5 years later (mean follow-up period, 3.2 years) (see Table S4 in the online supplement).
TABLE 1. Summary of overall evidence for the association between systemic inflammation and 24 individual symptoms of depression
Depressive SymptomPrevalence (%)Symptom DomainStrength of Overall EvidenceEffect SizeaRobust to Multivariable AdjustmentSupport for TemporalityConsistent Across Inflammatory MarkersHeterogeneity (I2, τ2)bGeneralizable Across Subgroups
Support for an association
1. Changes in appetite7.4Physical symptomStrongLargeYesYesYesLow (0%, 0.0003)Yes
2. Felt everything was an effort16.0Physical symptomStrongLargeYesYesYesHigh (79%, 0.0142)Yes
3. Could not get going/loss of energy19.6Physical symptomStrongLargeYesYesYesLow (21%, 0.0023)Yes
4. Little interest in doing things/unmotivated11.2Cognitive symptomModerateLargeYesYescMissing dataLow (22%, 0.0010)Yes
5. Sleep was restless21.5Physical symptomModerateModerateYesYesYesHigh (67%, 0.0031)Yes
Evidence against an association
6. Bothered by things6.7Emotional symptomStrongNo associationNoNoYesLow (14%, 0.0027)N/A
7. Felt hopeless about the future18.2Emotional symptomStrongNo associationNoNoYesLow (0%, 0.0016)N/A
8. Felt fearful3.8Emotional symptomModerateNo associationNoNoYesHigh (61%, 0.0131)N/A
9. Life had been a failure3.5Emotional symptomModerateNo associationNoNoYesHigh (62%, 0.0160)N/A
Uncertain evidence
10. Difficulties concentrating8.7Cognitive symptomModerateModerateYesYesYesModerate (47%, 0.0031)No
11. Could not shake off the blues4.3Perception of selfMixedModerateNoNoNoLow (0%, 0.0016)N/A
12. Felt worse than others13.6Perception of selfMixedNo associationNoYesNoLow (0%, 0.0016)N/A
13. Felt depressed10.2Emotional symptomMixedModerateYesYesNoModerate (43%, 0.0026)N/A
14. Felt unhappy12.5Emotional symptomMixedModerateNoYesYesModerate (49%, 0.0068)N/A
15. Talked less than usual7.0Physical symptomMixedNo associationNoYesNoLow (0%, <0.0001)N/A
16. Felt lonely11.5Emotional symptomMixedModerateNoYesNoLow (0%, 0.0002)N/A
17. People were unfriendly3.1Cognitive symptomMixedNo associationNoNoYesModerate (36%, 0.0189)N/A
18. Did not enjoy life10.6Emotional symptomMixedModerateNoNoNoModerate (29%, 0.0028)N/A
19. Had crying spells2.5Physical symptomMixedLargeNoNoNoModerate (36%, 0.0180)N/A
20. Felt sad14.1Emotional symptomMixedSmallNoNoNoLow (21%, 0.0017)N/A
21. People dislike me2.4Perception of selfMixedNo associationNoNoNoHigh (57%, 0.0326)N/A
22. Feeling bad about yourself5.3Perception of selfMixed and lackingModerateNoMissing dataMissing dataLow (0%, 0.0002)N/A
23. Moving or speaking slowly/too fast3.6Physical symptomMixed and lackingModerateNoMissing dataMissing dataLow (0%, 0.0015)N/A
24. Thought you would be better off dead1.1Self-harm symptomMixed and lackingLargeNoMissing dataMissing dataLow (19%, 0.0095)N/A
a
Large: odds ratio ≥1.20 and p<0.05; moderate: odds ratio between 1.10 and 1.19 and p<0.05; small: odds ratio <1.10 and p<0.05; no association, p>0.05.
b
Low: I2<25%; moderate: I2 between 25% and 50%; high: I2>50%.
c
Results are limited to the Health and Retirement Study.

Statistical Analysis

CRP and IL-6 values were log-transformed because of their skewed distribution. Our primary analyses were based on CRP, as this indicator of systemic inflammation was available in all studies (N=15). A total of three cohorts had data on IL-6 (see Table S4 in the online supplement). We used a two-step individual-participant-data meta-analysis. Analyses were first conducted separately in each study cohort; study-specific estimates and standard errors were subsequently combined in a meta-analytical framework. Analyses were based on individuals with no missing data on the exposure, outcome, and covariates.
Study-specific cross-sectional associations between inflammatory markers and individual symptoms of depression were estimated using multivariate logistic regression analyses. Odds ratios and their 95% confidence intervals were computed. In addition to an unadjusted model (crude model), we generated five multivariable-adjusted effect estimates in a serial manner. In model 1, effect estimates were adjusted for age and sex (basic model); in model 2, estimates were adjusted as in model 1, additionally controlling for the influence of education; in model 3, estimates were adjusted as in model 1 with the addition of illness-related variables; and in model 4, estimates were adjusted as in model 1 and for behavioral factors. In model 5, analyses were adjusted for all of the above-mentioned potential confounders and mediators. Variables in each covariate group were entered simultaneously into the models. To examine whether robust associations were largely driven by high levels of inflammation, we repeated these analyses after excluding participants with CRP levels ≥10 mg/L. This exclusion threshold has previously been used in studies on systemic low-grade inflammation (36).
Study-specific effect estimates were pooled using random-effects meta-analyses. In comparison to fixed-effects models, random-effects models provide a more conservative estimate. Heterogeneity was examined by computing I2 and τ2 statistics. The first refers to the total proportion of variation in effect sizes that is not due to sampling error, and the latter indicates intercohort variance (37). To investigate whether systemic inflammation preceded individual depressive symptoms, analyses were repeated longitudinally, with individual symptoms of depression at follow-up as the outcome of interest, additionally adjusting the effect estimates for the respective depression symptom at baseline.
The strength of evidence for each inflammation-depressive symptom association was evaluated on the basis of the following criteria: magnitude of the effect (“large” was denoted by an odds ratio in the basic model ≥1.20 and p<0.05; “moderate” by an odds ratio between 1.10 and 1.19 and p<0.05; and “small” by an odds ratio <1.10, but p<0.05; “no association” was denoted by p>0.05); robustness to multivariable adjustments (“yes,” a significant effect estimate after adjustment for all covariates in the analysis of CRP and depressive symptoms, and a comparable point estimate for the same symptom in the smaller IL-6 data set; otherwise “no”); temporality (“yes,” a significant association in the longitudinal analysis; otherwise “no”); consistency across inflammatory markers (“yes,” a statistically significant association of both CRP and IL-6 with depressive symptoms in the basic model; otherwise “no”); heterogeneity in study-specific estimates (“low,” I2<25%; “moderate,” I2 between 25% and 50%; and “high,” I2>50%); and generalizability across subgroups (men, women, age groups 18–60 years and >60 years, and a subgroup of depressed people, that is, the potential target group for anti-inflammatory treatment trials for depression).
We also performed a number of additional sensitivity analyses. To explore whether adjustment for adverse childhood experiences affected the strength of the age- and sex-adjusted cross-sectional association between CRP and the symptoms that were robustly associated with inflammation in our main analysis, we computed a standardized adverse childhood experience sum score in each cohort with relevant data (mean=0, SD=1). To test whether the time interval between blood collection and measurement of depressive symptoms during baseline data collection influenced the robustness of the identified associations, analyses were stratified by dividing studies according to the timing of exposure and outcome ascertainment (i.e., blood collection and depression measured on the same day versus time interval ≥1 day). To examine whether the association between inflammation and depressive symptoms was independent of comorbid medical illnesses, we repeated the analysis in a subgroup of individuals without chronic illnesses.
Finally, we conducted a post hoc analysis to examine whether individuals with both high levels of CRP and high levels of the identified symptoms were a distinct subpopulation. In doing so, participants were divided into four groups: high CRP (≥3 mg/L) and high symptom level (top tertile); high CRP–low symptom level; low CRP–high symptom level; and low CRP–low symptom level. To assign values for missing data on individual symptoms of depression across the 15 cohort studies, we performed a multiple imputation analysis with 52 imputed data sets. Next, we computed the expected frequencies of belonging to each group, based on the assumption that the two dichotomous variables (high versus low CRP and high versus low levels of depressive symptoms) were independent. We then compared the observed frequencies (O) to the expected frequencies (E) within each group by calculating the observed-to-expected ratio (O/E), and we computed chi-square statistics to test the difference in the distributions across observed and expected counts. In addition, we explored how individuals with both high CRP and high levels of the identified symptoms may differ from other study members. Differences in means and proportions in sociodemographic, behavioral, and illness-related factors were tested using two-sided t tests and chi-square tests of independence (see Table S5 in the online supplement). These analyses were repeated in the subgroup of depressed individuals (Table S6 in the online supplement).
All study-specific analyses were conducted in Stata, version 14.1, and random-effects meta-analyses were performed using the metafor package (38) in RStudio, version 4.0.2. Statistical code is provided in appendix 5 in the online supplement.

RESULTS

The pooled data from 15 cohort studies comprised 56,351 participants with a mean age of 57.8 years (SD=12.0). Of these, 27,125 (48.5%) were men and 29,226 (51.5%) women. The geometric mean was 0.89 mg/L for CRP (95% CI=0.85, 0.94) and 0.74 pg/mL for IL-6 (95% CI=0.70, 0.78). The number of cohorts included in the symptom-specific meta-analyses ranged from 2 to 14, depending on the depressive symptom. Across all cohorts, the weighted mean prevalence of depressive symptoms varied from 1.1% (suicidal ideation) to 21.5% (sleep problems) (see Table 1). The mean prevalence for overall elevated symptoms of depression was 14.0% (see Table S1 in the online supplement).
Figure 1 shows the pooled effect estimates of the cross-sectional associations between CRP, depression status, and 24 individual depressive symptoms. In analyses adjusted for age and sex, the odds ratio per one-standard-deviation-higher CRP level for depression was 1.18 (95% CI=1.14, 1.22), but it was attenuated to 1.05 (95% CI=1.02, 1.09) after additional adjustment for sociodemographic, illness-related, and behavioral factors. In symptom-specific analyses adjusted for age and sex, higher CRP concentrations were associated with increased odds of reporting six of seven physical symptoms, two of three cognitive symptoms, five of nine emotional symptoms, two of four biased self-perceptions, and one of one self-harm-related symptom. After further adjustment for socioeconomic, chronic illness–related, and behavioral risk factors, CRP remained robustly associated with four physical symptoms (changes in appetite, felt everything was an effort, could not get going or loss of energy, sleep problems), two cognitive symptoms (trouble concentrating, little interest in doing things/unmotivated), and one emotional symptom (felt depressed). A similar pattern of associations emerged in the sensitivity analysis excluding participants with CRP concentrations ≥10 mg/L, although the association with “felt everything was an effort” was weakened after additional adjustment for behavioral factors (see Figure S6 in the online supplement). In domain-specific analyses (basic model), CRP was most strongly associated with physical and cognitive symptoms and least associated with emotional symptoms (see Figure S9 in the online supplement).
FIGURE 1. Unadjusted and serially adjusted cross-sectional association between C-reactive protein (CRP) and 24 symptoms of depression (random-effects meta-analysis)
aModel 1 was adjusted for age and sex; model 2 was adjusted as in model 1 and additionally adjusted for education; model 3 was adjusted as in model 1 and additionally adjusted for health-related factors; model 4 was adjusted as in model 1 and additionally adjusted for behavioral factors; and model 5 was adjusted for all of the above-listed covariates.
b Statistically significant after all adjustments.
Table 2 depicts the pooled effect estimates from cross-sectional analyses for IL-6 and depression status, as well as the individual symptoms of depression that were robustly associated with CRP in our main analysis. Higher IL-6 levels were associated with an increased risk of depression (age- and sex-adjusted odds ratio=1.22, 95% CI=1.09, 1.38), although statistical significance at conventional levels was lost after adjustment for all available covariates (odds ratio=1.08; 95% CI=0.95, 1.23). In symptom-specific analyses (basic model), higher IL-6 levels were associated with increased odds of experiencing five of the seven symptoms previously identified to be robustly associated with CRP in our main analysis (no association was observed with the symptom “felt depressed,” and no data were available to test the symptom “little interest in doing things”). In multivariable-adjusted analyses, point estimates for the association between IL-6 and these five symptoms (odds ratios ranged from 1.09 to 1.31) were similar to or higher than the corresponding point estimates observed in the larger analysis of CRP (odds ratios ranged from 1.05 to 1.14).
TABLE 2. Serially adjusted cross-sectional association between interleukin-6 (IL-6) and five depressive symptoms (random-effects meta-analysis)
  Odds Ratio per 1 SD Higher IL-6a
  Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4Model 5
Depression Status and Depressive Symptom (Outcome)bN Sample / N StudiesOdds Ratio95% CIOdds Ratio95% CIOdds Ratio95% CIOdds Ratio95% CIOdds Ratio95% CI
Depression5,373 / 31.221.09, 1.381.221.08, 1.371.191.06, 1.351.100.97, 1.251.080.95, 1.23
Felt everything was an effortc5,374 / 31.471.21, 1.781.431.17, 1.751.411.15, 1.721.391.13, 1.721.311.12, 1.54
Changes in appetitec5,375 / 31.461.27, 1.681.451.26, 1.671.411.22, 1.631.351.16, 1.581.311.06, 1.63
Could not get going/loss of energy5,376 / 31.311.03, 1.671.301.02, 1.661.250.98, 1.601.160.97, 1.381.120.92, 1.35
Sleep problems5,376 / 31.161.03, 1.311.161.02, 1.311.131.01, 1.261.131.00, 1.281.090.97, 1.22
Trouble concentrating4,773 / 21.201.01, 1.431.211.01, 1.431.160.99, 1.361.170.99, 1.381.130.98, 1.30
a
Model 1 was adjusted for age and sex; model 2 was adjusted as in model 1 and additionally adjusted for education; model 3 was adjusted as in model 1 and additionally adjusted for health-related factors; model 4 was adjusted as in model 1 and additionally adjusted for behavioral factors; and model 5 was adjusted for all of the above-listed covariates.
b
The symptoms listed are the individual symptoms of depression that were robustly associated with CRP in the main analysis, excluding “little interest in doing things,” for which no data were available.
c
Statistically significant in all adjustments.
To examine generalizability, we assessed heterogeneity (I2 and τ2) in study-specific estimates for symptoms that were consistently associated with systemic inflammation in our main analyses (Table 1). Heterogeneity in study-specific estimates was small or moderate for “changes in appetite” (I2=0%, τ2=0.0003), “could not get going/loss of energy” (I2=21%, τ2=0.0023), “trouble concentrating” (I2=22%, τ2=0.0031), and “little interest in doing things/unmotivated” (I2=47%, τ2=0.0010), but high for “sleep problems” (I2=67%, τ2=0.0031) and “felt everything was an effort” (I2=79%, τ2=0.0142). Of the 14 studies with data on “sleep problems,” 13 supported excess risk (odds ratios >1), and one study favored a protective effect (odds ratio <1). For the symptom “felt everything was an effort,” seven of eight studies supported excess risk in relation to higher CRP concentrations, although the magnitude of the effect estimates varied between cohorts.
Overlapping point estimates and accompanying 95% confidence intervals in analyses stratified by age, sex, and timing of exposure and outcome measurement at baseline suggest that the inflammation–depressive symptom associations varied little between subgroups (Figure 2; see also Table S7 in the online supplement). In depressed individuals, higher levels of CRP were significantly associated with increased odds of reporting “changes in appetite,” “could not get going/loss of energy levels,” “little interest in doing things/unmotivated,” and “sleep problems.” The effect estimate for “felt everything was an effort” was of similar magnitude but with wider 95% confidence intervals. These five robust cross-sectional associations were replicated in analyses excluding chronically ill individuals and in longitudinal analyses (Figure 2). Additionally, the previously identified longitudinal associations largely remained after multivariable adjustments (see Figure S8 in the online supplement). In contrast, the association with “trouble concentrating” was attenuated after adjustment for adverse childhood experiences, and no association with this particular symptom was observed in the age- and sex-adjusted analyses among depressed individuals (Figure 2).
FIGURE 2. Association between C-reactive protein (CRP) and six depressive symptoms in subgroups, after additional adjustments and longitudinally (random-effects meta-analysis)
aAdjusted for age and sex as appropriate.
b Adjusted for age, sex, and adverse childhood experiences (ACE).
c These analyses excluded individuals with a self-reported history of coronary heart disease, stroke, cancer, hypertension, or diabetes.
d Adjusted for age, sex, and depressive symptom at baseline.
The overall evidence on the symptom-specific associations with systemic inflammation is summarized in Table 1. In terms of the effect size, robustness to multivariable adjustments, evidence on temporality, consistency across inflammatory markers, and consistency of associations across cohorts and subgroups, strong evidence for an association with systemic inflammation was obtained for three physical symptoms (changes in appetite: age- and sex-adjusted odds ratio=1.23, 95% CI=1.18, 1.27; felt everything was an effort: odds ratio=1.23, 95% CI=1.12, 1.36; could not get going/loss of energy: odds ratio=1.20, 95% CI=1.16, 1.24). Moderate evidence was found for one further physical symptom (sleep problems: odds ratio=1.13, 95% CI=1.09, 1.17) and one cognitive symptom (little interest in doing things/unmotivated: odds ratio=1.21, 95% CI=1.16, 1.26). Moreover, the overall evidence was strongly against an association with four emotional symptoms (bothered by things: odds ratio=1.05, 95% CI=0.94, 1.16; felt hopeless about the future: odds ratio=1.04, 95% CI=0.97, 1.11; felt fearful: odds ratio=1.03, 95% CI=0.87, 1.23; life had been a failure: odds ratio=1.15, 95% CI=0.95, 1.38), while for the remaining 14 symptoms the evidence was mixed (see also Figure S10 in the online supplement).

Post Hoc Analysis

Further analyses confirmed that individuals with both elevated levels of CRP and high levels of the five inflammation-related symptoms represented a distinct subpopulation. The number of individuals with high CRP concentrations and high symptom levels was 1.3 times higher than expected (O/E=1.31, χ2=472.5, df=3, N=56,351, p<0.001). They differed from other participants in terms of sociodemographic, behavioral, and illness-related profiles, the most observable differences being their lower educational qualifications, lower physical activity levels, higher BMI, and higher prevalence of diabetes, coronary heart disease, stroke, and cancer, but lower alcohol consumption (all p<0.001) (see Table S5 in the online supplement). Among depressed people, a similar pattern emerged: elevated levels of CRP and high inflammation-related symptom levels also appeared to denote a specific subpopulation (O/E=1.11, χ2=108.1, df=3, N=6,814, p<0.001) with a specific risk factor profile (see Table S6 in the online supplement).

DISCUSSION

Our findings from up to 15 cohort studies comprising 56,351 adults across multiple countries suggest that systemic inflammation is robustly associated with a distinct set of symptoms, both physical (changes in appetite, felt everything was an effort, could not get going/loss of energy, sleep problems) and cognitive (little interest in doing things/unmotivated). These associations were evident across subgroups and two inflammatory biomarkers, in analyses excluding participants with CRP values ≥10 mg/L, after adjustment for socioeconomic, behavioral, and disease-related factors, and in analyses additionally controlling for adverse childhood experiences. In contrast, we found strong evidence against an association with a number of exclusively emotional symptoms, including fearfulness, feeling bothered by things, hopelessness about the future, and feeling life had been a failure.
These results confirm the findings of previous cross-sectional studies showing differential associations between systemic inflammation and changes in appetite (3, 23, 27), lower energy levels (3, 23, 24), and sleep problems (3, 23, 24). Furthermore, in a recent report using NHANES data on 15,071 U.S. adults (23), a weak association was reported between CRP and little interest in doing things.
In individuals with depression, inflammation was associated with “changes in appetite,” “could not get going/loss of energy,” “little interest in doing things/unmotivated,” and “sleep problems.” The effect estimate for “felt everything was an effort” was comparable to or higher than those for the other four symptoms, but less precisely estimated. In contrast, although we found a robust association with the symptom “difficulties concentrating” in our main analysis, this relationship was absent in the subgroup of depressed individuals. Thus, our findings support the notion that in depressed individuals, systemic inflammation is primarily associated with physical symptoms and the anhedonia-related symptom “little interest in doing things.” If causality is confirmed in future studies, the identified symptom profile could be used to define a subpopulation of depressed people most likely to benefit from anti-inflammatory therapies.
Findings from our longitudinal analyses are partially consistent with two previously published smaller-scale studies. For example, a longitudinal investigation of 2,731 children found that higher IL-6 levels, but not CRP levels, were linked to an increased risk of experiencing concentration difficulties at follow-up, in addition to diurnal mood variation, sleep problems, and fatigue (26). In an analysis of 2,872 Dutch adults, a higher basal inflammation index, as indicated by increased mean levels of CRP, IL-6, and tumor necrosis factor–alpha, was associated with subsequent physical symptoms (e.g., changes in appetite, sleep problems, and lower energy levels) (39). However, the latter findings should be interpreted with caution, given the study’s relatively small sample size, an overrepresentation of women, and the limited set of covariates controlled for in the analyses. Moreover, collapsing biologically distinct inflammatory markers into a single mean index of basal inflammation may mask biomarker-specific effects on depressive symptomatology. Sources of heterogeneity in previous studies include differences in study design, sample size, methodology, sample characteristics, and varying statistical adjustments used to control for the influence of potential confounding factors. In the present study, we attempted to move beyond these differences by harmonizing the data from 15 population-based cohort studies, employing a rigorous statistical approach, and adjusting for the effects of a wide range of covariates.
Our results lend support to the sickness behavior theory (40), which posits that peripherally localized inflammatory activity can initiate a cascade of initially adaptive depressive-like symptoms in a subset of people, collectively known as sickness behavior. These include a lack of energy (lethargy), changes in appetite, sleepiness, reduced social exploration, and, at times, confusion. Sickness behavior is also characterized by depressed mood and increased sensitivity to pain (hyperalgesia). In the present study, evidence on the association between inflammation and depressed mood was mixed, and no data were available on hyperalgesia. In addition, we found strong evidence against an association between inflammation and four exclusively emotional symptoms: “bothered by things,” “felt hopeless about the future,” “felt fearful,” and “life had been a failure,” most of which have previously been classified as non–sickness behaviors (39).
Our multivariable-adjusted findings highlight the potential contributions of lifestyle factors to the CRP-depression association because adjustment for lifestyle covariates led to substantial attenuation, an observation that accords with findings from a recent large-scale case-control study (41). The latter investigation found that the association between CRP and overall depression was strongly attenuated but remained statistically significant at conventional levels after controlling for the effects of age, sex, early-life trauma, self-reported health status, alcohol intake, smoking, and BMI. Of these, smoking and BMI appeared to be the most influential factors. Similarly, BMI exerted the greatest attenuating effect in our analyses. This suggests that a high BMI may contribute to both inflammation and depressive symptoms, lie on the causal pathway between inflammation and depressive symptoms, or both (3, 18, 24, 27, 42). IL-6 and CRP are synthesized in response to adipocytes within adipose tissue (43), and higher levels of body fat, in particular visceral fat, are related to metabolic inflammation and depression (44, 45). Moreover, we found that adjustment for BMI particularly reduced the strength of the relationships between inflammation and physical or energy-conserving symptoms, including “changes in appetite,” “lower energy levels,” and “sleep problems”—a finding supported elsewhere (46). The precise drivers and mechanisms by which increased levels of circulating inflammatory markers exert an influence on depressive symptoms remain unclear. Further research is needed to dissect the interplay between metabolic conditions, inflammation, and individual symptoms of depression and to examine whether other sources, such as genetic susceptibility to systemic inflammation or stress-related responses of the body, may underlie the inflammation–depressive symptom association (47).
Our results may have important implications for future research by suggesting a more targeted, symptom-focused approach to exploring the link between systemic inflammation and depression, particularly in anti-inflammatory drug trials. Current evidence from early-stage clinical trials on the effect of anti-inflammatory drug therapies on depression is sparse, and interpretation of findings is hampered by methodological heterogeneity across studies (48, 49). First results indicate modest antidepressant effects of cytokine inhibitors and celecoxib add-on therapy to conventional antidepressants in depressed patients with prior elevated levels of systemic inflammation (50). However, clinical studies on possible symptom-specific effects of anti-inflammatory therapies are still missing.
The present study has a number of strengths, including its large sample size, which offers higher statistical precision than extant studies; the use of more than one inflammatory marker to assess systemic inflammation; ascertainment of the robustness of associations in multivariable-adjusted analyses; confirmation of the generalizability of the findings across multiple cohorts and subgroups; and the use of longitudinal data to assess temporality. However, our results need to be interpreted in light of some limitations. Causal inference is limited by the use of observational data. Our exposure variable, systemic inflammation, was measured only once at baseline; repeated measurement of inflammatory markers would have enabled us to better capture the time-varying nature of inflammation. Measurement error due to diurnal changes in IL-6, fasting status, and analytical methods may have biased the estimates of our secondary exposure. The primary analysis was based on CRP—a largely stable biomarker which is unaffected by diurnal variations (51). However, IL-6 is less stable and fluctuates throughout the day. In this study, measurement of IL-6 was based on serum blood samples collected in the morning after overnight fasting in all cohorts. Hence, confounding due to variability in the time of blood collection, analytical method, and fasting status is likely to be limited in our sample. A further limitation is that the assessment of depressive symptoms was based on self-report rather than clinical interviews, which are considered to be the gold-standard method in psychiatric research. In addition, although we included a wide range of covariates, we were not able to control for the influence of anti-inflammatory, antidepressant, or anticoagulation drugs, which may have modified the association between systemic inflammation and depressive symptoms.
In summary, we found that circulating inflammatory markers were robustly associated with a defined set of physical and cognitive symptoms. There was equally strong evidence against an association with exclusively emotional symptoms characterized by fearfulness and negative feelings about life and the future. These findings are largely consistent with the sickness behavior theory of depression. The scientifically reliable identification of symptom-specific associations with inflammation is valuable, as it demonstrates a differential rather than a generalized effect of inflammation on depression. Hence, our findings may pave the way toward a new inflammatory depression phenotype and can guide systematic efforts to develop novel inflammation-targeted treatments. Patient recruitment to future anti-inflammatory drug trials should be based on symptom profiles characterized by the inflammation-related depressive symptoms observed in this study.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2021.20121776.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Vos T, Abajobir AA, Abate KH, et al: Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet 2017; 390:1211–1259
2.
Dunn BD: Helping depressed clients reconnect to positive emotion experience: current insights and future directions. Clin Psychol Psychother 2012; 19:326–340
3.
Fried EI, von Stockert S, Haslbeck J, et al: Using network analysis to examine links between individual depressive symptoms, inflammatory markers, and covariates. Psychol Med 2020; 50:2682–2690
4.
Frasure-Smith N, Lespérance F: Depression: a cardiac risk factor in search of a treatment. JAMA 2003; 289:3171–3173
5.
Jorm AF: Is depression a risk factor for dementia or cognitive decline? A review. Gerontology 2000; 46:219–227
6.
Cuijpers P, Vogelzangs N, Twisk J, et al: Comprehensive meta-analysis of excess mortality in depression in the general community versus patients with specific illnesses. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:453–462
7.
Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, et al: Acute and longer-term outcomes in depressed outpatients requiring one or several treatment steps: a STAR*D report. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:1905–1917
8.
Zeier Z, Carpenter LL, Kalin NH, et al: Clinical implementation of pharmacogenetic decision support tools for antidepressant drug prescribing. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:873–886
9.
Barnes J, Mondelli V, Pariante CM: Genetic contributions of inflammation to depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2017; 42:81–98
10.
Konsman JP, Parnet P, Dantzer R: Cytokine-induced sickness behaviour: mechanisms and implications. Trends Neurosci 2002; 25:154–159
11.
Miller AH, Raison CL: The role of inflammation in depression: from evolutionary imperative to modern treatment target. Nat Rev Immunol 2016; 16:22–34
12.
Eisenberger NI, Moieni M, Inagaki TK, et al: In sickness and in health: the co-regulation of inflammation and social behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 2017; 42:242–253
13.
Miller AH, Haroon E, Raison CL, et al: Cytokine targets in the brain: impact on neurotransmitters and neurocircuits. Depress Anxiety 2013; 30:297–306
14.
Köhler CA, Freitas TH, Maes M, et al: Peripheral cytokine and chemokine alterations in depression: a meta-analysis of 82 studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2017; 135:373–387
15.
Haapakoski R, Mathieu J, Ebmeier KP, et al: Cumulative meta-analysis of interleukins 6 and 1β, tumour necrosis factor α, and C-reactive protein in patients with major depressive disorder. Brain Behav Immun 2015; 49:206–215
16.
Smith KJ, Au B, Ollis L, et al: The association between C-reactive protein, interleukin-6, and depression among older adults in the community: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Exp Gerontol 2018; 102:109–132
17.
Valkanova V, Ebmeier KP, Allan CL: CRP, IL-6, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. J Affect Disord 2013; 150:736–744
18.
Horn SR, Long MM, Nelson BW, et al: Replication and reproducibility issues in the relationship between C-reactive protein and depression: a systematic review and focused meta-analysis. Brain Behav Immun 2018; 73:85–114
19.
Chevance A, Ravaud P, Tomlinson A, et al: Identifying outcomes for depression that matter to patients, informal caregivers, and health-care professionals: qualitative content analysis of a large international online survey. Lancet Psychiatry 2020; 7:692–702
20.
Frank P, Ajnakina O, Steptoe A, et al: Genetic susceptibility, inflammation, and specific types of depressive symptoms: evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Transl Psychiatry 2020; 10:140
21.
World Health Organization: ICD-11 for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics (2019 version). Geneva, World Health Organization, 2019
22.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2013
23.
Jokela M, Virtanen M, Batty GD, et al: Inflammation and specific symptoms of depression. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:87–88
24.
White J, Kivimäki M, Jokela M, et al: Association of inflammation with specific symptoms of depression in a general population of older people: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Brain Behav Immun 2017; 61:27–30
25.
Schmidt FM, Schröder T, Kirkby KC, et al: Pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokines, but not CRP, are inversely correlated with severity and symptoms of major depression. Psychiatry Res 2016; 239:85–91
26.
Chu AL, Stochl J, Lewis G, et al: Longitudinal association between inflammatory markers and specific symptoms of depression in a prospective birth cohort. Brain Behav Immun 2019; 76:74–81
27.
Lamers F, Milaneschi Y, de Jonge P, et al: Metabolic and inflammatory markers: associations with individual depressive symptoms. Psychol Med 2018; 48:1102–1110
28.
Kivimäki M, Nyberg ST, Batty GD, et al: Job strain as a risk factor for coronary heart disease: a collaborative meta-analysis of individual participant data. Lancet 2012; 380:1491–1497
29.
O’Connor M-F, Bower JE, Cho HJ, et al: To assess, to control, to exclude: effects of biobehavioral factors on circulating inflammatory markers. Brain Behav Immun 2009; 23:887–897
30.
Hughes K, Bellis MA, Hardcastle KA, et al: The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health 2017; 2:e356–e366
31.
Danese A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, et al: Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2009; 163:1135–1143
32.
Radloff LS: The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977; 1:385–401
33.
Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB: The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med 2001; 16:606–613
34.
Goldberg DP: The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire: A Technique for the Identification and Assessment of Non-Psychotic Psychiatric Illness (Maudsley Monographs, No 21). London, Oxford University Press, 1972
35.
Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA, Brooks JO 3rd, et al: Proposed factor structure of the Geriatric Depression Scale. Int Psychogeriatr 1991; 3:23–28
36.
Pearson TA, Mensah GA, Alexander RW, et al: Markers of inflammation and cardiovascular disease: application to clinical and public health practice: a statement for healthcare professionals from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American Heart Association. Circulation 2003; 107:499–511
37.
Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, et al: Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 2003; 327:557–560
38.
Viechtbauer W: Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw 2010; 36:1–48
39.
van Eeden WA, van Hemert AM, Carlier IVE, et al: Basal and LPS-stimulated inflammatory markers and the course of individual symptoms of depression. Transl Psychiatry 2020; 10:235
40.
Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Freund GG, et al: From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9:46–56
41.
Pitharouli MC, Hagenaars SP, Glanville KP, et al: Elevated C-reactive protein in patients with depression, independent of genetic, health, and psychosocial factors: results from the UK Biobank. Am J Psychiatry 2021; 178:522–529
42.
Kappelmann N, Arloth J, Georgakis MK, et al: Dissecting the association between inflammation, metabolic dysregulation, and specific depressive symptoms: a genetic correlation and 2-sample mendelian randomization study. JAMA Psychiatry 2021; 78:161–170
43.
Tilg H, Moschen AR: Adipocytokines: mediators linking adipose tissue, inflammation, and immunity. Nat Rev Immunol 2006; 6:772–783
44.
Speed MS, Jefsen OH, Børglum AD, et al: Investigating the association between body fat and depression via mendelian randomization. Transl Psychiatry 2019; 9:184
45.
Milaneschi Y, Simmons WK, van Rossum EFC, et al: Depression and obesity: evidence of shared biological mechanisms. Mol Psychiatry 2019; 24:18–33
46.
Lamers F, Milaneschi Y, Vinkers CH, et al: Depression profilers and immuno-metabolic dysregulation: longitudinal results from the NESDA study. Brain Behav Immun 2020; 88:174–183
47.
Beurel E, Toups M, Nemeroff CB: The bidirectional relationship of depression and inflammation: double trouble. Neuron 2020; 107:234–256
48.
Köhler O, Benros ME, Nordentoft M, et al: Effect of anti-inflammatory treatment on depression, depressive symptoms, and adverse effects: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71:1381–1391
49.
Eyre HA, Stuart MJ, Baune BT: A phase-specific neuroimmune model of clinical depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2014; 54:265–274
50.
Köhler-Forsberg O, N Lydholm C, Hjorthøj C, et al: Efficacy of anti-inflammatory treatment on major depressive disorder or depressive symptoms: meta-analysis of clinical trials. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2019; 139:404–419
51.
Meier-Ewert HK, Ridker PM, Rifai N, et al: Absence of diurnal variation of C-reactive protein concentrations in healthy human subjects. Clin Chem 2001; 47:426–430

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1107 - 1118
PubMed: 34645276

History

Received: 23 December 2020
Revision received: 28 May 2021
Revision received: 19 July 2021
Accepted: 4 August 2021
Published online: 14 October 2021
Published in print: December 2021

Keywords

  1. Inflammation
  2. C-Reactive Protein
  3. Interleukin-6
  4. Depression Symptoms
  5. Biological Markers
  6. Epidemiology

Authors

Details

Philipp Frank, M.Sc. [email protected]
Research Department of Epidemiology and Public Health (Frank, Batty, Kivimäki) and Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health (Frank, Cadar, Steptoe), University College London; Department of Psychology and Logopedics (Jokela) and Clinicum Unit (Kivimäki), Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki.
Markus Jokela, Ph.D.
Research Department of Epidemiology and Public Health (Frank, Batty, Kivimäki) and Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health (Frank, Cadar, Steptoe), University College London; Department of Psychology and Logopedics (Jokela) and Clinicum Unit (Kivimäki), Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki.
G. David Batty, Ph.D., D.Sc.
Research Department of Epidemiology and Public Health (Frank, Batty, Kivimäki) and Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health (Frank, Cadar, Steptoe), University College London; Department of Psychology and Logopedics (Jokela) and Clinicum Unit (Kivimäki), Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki.
Research Department of Epidemiology and Public Health (Frank, Batty, Kivimäki) and Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health (Frank, Cadar, Steptoe), University College London; Department of Psychology and Logopedics (Jokela) and Clinicum Unit (Kivimäki), Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki.
Andrew Steptoe, D.Phil., F.Med.Sci.
Research Department of Epidemiology and Public Health (Frank, Batty, Kivimäki) and Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health (Frank, Cadar, Steptoe), University College London; Department of Psychology and Logopedics (Jokela) and Clinicum Unit (Kivimäki), Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki.
Mika Kivimäki, Ph.D., F.Med.Sci.
Research Department of Epidemiology and Public Health (Frank, Batty, Kivimäki) and Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health (Frank, Cadar, Steptoe), University College London; Department of Psychology and Logopedics (Jokela) and Clinicum Unit (Kivimäki), Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki.

Notes

Send correspondence to Mr. Frank ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

All authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Mr. Frank is supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) and the Biotechnology and Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) (ES/P000347/1). Dr. Batty is supported by the U.K. Medical Research Council (MRC) (MR/P023444/1). Dr. Cadar is funded by the National Institute on Aging (grant RO1AG017644). Dr. Kivimäki has received grants from the Wellcome Trust (221854/Z/20/Z), NordForsk (75021), the MRC (K013351, S011676, R024227), the National Institute on Aging (R01AG056477), the Academy of Finland (329202), the Helsinki Institute of Life Science (H970), and the Finnish Work Environment Fund (190424).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share