Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: 1 March 1998

Selective Alteration of Personality and Social Behavior by Serotonergic Intervention

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The authors sought to test the causal hypothesis that serotonergic function modulates aspects of the normal spectrum of individual differences in affective experience and social behavior in humans. METHOD: A selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), paroxetine, 20 mg/day (N=26), or placebo (N=25) was administered to normal volunteers in a double-blind manner for 4 weeks, and personality variables and social behavior were assessed at baseline and at weeks 1 and 4 of treatment. RESULTS: Relative to placebo, SSRI administration reduced focal indices of hostility through a more general decrease in negative affect, yet did not alter indices of positive affect. In addition, SSRI administration increased a behavioral index of social affiliation. Changes in both negative affect and affiliative behavior were significantly related to volunteers' plasma SSRI levels at the end of the experiment. CONCLUSIONS: Central serotonergic function may modulate a dimension of normal personality characterized by reduced negative affective experience and increased affiliative behavior. SSRI administration has significant and detectable effects on these measures even in the absence of baseline clinical depression or other psychopathology.
Research indicates that individual differences in human personality can be summarized by three to five independent dimensions (1). Status on each of these dimensions is stable over the adult lifespan (2) and typically shows heritabilities on the order of 50% (3). However, the physiological substrates underlying these personality dimensions have not yet been elucidated. Spurred by advances in psychopharmacology, several theorists have proposed that brain biogenic amine mechanisms may contribute to the phenotypic expression of some of these dimensions, particularly those which involve affective and motivational processes (46).
Consistent with these speculations, clinical studies of psychiatric patients suggest that low brain serotonin activity may be related to psychiatric disorders involving hostile affect and aggressive behavior. For instance, people with a history of impulsively violent behavior (e.g., arsonists, violent criminals, people who die by violent methods of suicide) have low CSF serotonin metabolite levels (7). In addition, patients with violent histories (e.g., with antisocial personality disorder) show signs of compromised brain serotonin function, as assessed by neuroendocrine probes (8). Finally, pharmacological interventions that augment serotonergic efficacy can reduce hostile sentiment and violent outbursts in aggressive psychiatric patients (9).
In contrast, the impact of serotonergic interventions on hostile personality characteristics in normal individuals has received less investigation. Impairment of brain serotonin function by means of precursor depletion can induce depressive affect (10, 11) and may potentiate hostile behavior (12, 13) in normal control subjects. At the time this study was conducted, no one had investigated whether augmentation of brain serotonin would affect hostility variables in normal volunteers. If enhancement of brain serotonergic function reduces hostility, might such effects remain focal only to hostility variables, or might they be attributable to broader changes in affective personality variables? For example, might administration of a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) reduce negative affect (including sadness, anxiety, and other negative emotions in addition to hostility) or increase positive affect? To address these questions, we used a pharmacologically selective intervention to alter brain serotonin function in normal subjects and observed subsequent changes in standard affective personality variables over 4 weeks.
A second but related body of research suggests that brain serotonin function also plays a role in the social behavior of nonhuman primates. For instance, rhesus monkeys with low CSF serotonin metabolite levels show more spontaneous aggression toward conspecifics, receive more wounds, and die at a younger age (14, 15), while those with high CSF serotonin metabolite levels show greater proximity to and grooming of peers and have a greater number of neighbors living nearby (16). Serotonergic interventions change primate social behavior in ways that are consistent with these naturally occurring correlations. For example, chronic administration of agents that eventually deplete serotonin (i.e., fenfluramine) increases aggression and locomotor activity in male vervet monkeys (17), while serotonergic augmentation through either dietary increases in serotonin precursors (i.e., tryptophan) or a pharmacological blockade of serotonin reuptake (i.e., fluoxetine) reduces aggression, vigilance, and locomotion (18) and increases proximity to and grooming of peers (19). To address the hypothesis that augmentation of central serotonergic function would increase affiliative behavior in humans, we observed the effects of our serotonergic intervention on the social behavior of normal humans in the context of a cooperative dyadic puzzle task.

METHOD

Subjects and Treatment

We examined the effects of a serotonergic reuptake blockade on personality and social behavior in a double-blind protocol by randomly assigning 51 medically and psychiatrically healthy volunteers to treatment with either an SSRI, paroxetine, 20 mg/day p.o. (N=25), or placebo (N=26). Paroxetine was selected because of its relatively potent and specific inhibition of the serotonin reuptake mechanism in comparison with other SSRIs (20), although any SSRI may have secondary effects on other monoamine systems (21). Volunteers were recruited by advertisements in a local weekly newspaper. They gave written informed consent and were screened to exclude subjects with a history of or with first-degree relatives with a history of axis I disorders or dysthymia, as determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R—Non-Patient Edition (22). Volunteers were also screened to exclude subjects with a history of psychotropic medication or substance abuse and subjects taking concurrent medications (including birth control pills in the case of women). Volunteers were informed of the possibility of physical side effects but not of the hypotheses. After complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained from volunteers. One man and one woman in the paroxetine group did not complete the experiment because of side effects, while a second woman in the control group did not complete the experiment because of scheduling conflicts, leaving a total of 23 volunteers in the SSRI group (nine women and 14 men) and 25 in the placebo group (11 women and 14 men). The mean ages of SSRI-treated (mean=26.7 years, SD=2.9) and placebo-treated (mean=27.9, SD=4.2) volunteers did not differ.

Psychometric Measures

Standard psychometric and behavioral measures were administered at three times: at baseline, after 1 week of treatment, and after 4 weeks of treatment (i.e., at the end of the study). At each assessment, hostility was assessed with the assaultiveness and irritability subscales of the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (23), as suggested by Siever and Trestman (24). To determine whether changes in hostility could be accounted for by more general changes in affective dimensions of personality, we also administered the Positive and Negative Affect Scales. Prior research has shown that different negative affects (e.g., hostility, fear) tend to be correlated in incidence, as do different positive affects (e.g., happiness, excitement). However, negative and positive affects tend to be uncorrelated with each other rather than inversely correlated, and so they are conceptualized as independent trait dimensions (25). To enhance the sensitivity of all psychometric measures to change over time, participants were asked to respond to the questionnaires in terms of their experience during the previous week.

Behavioral Measures

Objective behaviors were elicited at each assessment in the context of a standardized dyadic puzzle task. The task was designed to elicit face-valid social behaviors that could be reliably coded within the context of a cooperative interaction and subsequently compared across repeated measurement occasions. During the task, each subject collaborated with a partner in planning and implementing solutions for different spatial puzzles (also known as tangrams). Subject pairs were given 10 minutes to combine a set of seven puzzle pieces into configurations that matched as many target shapes as possible, with the stipulation that only one partner could touch the puzzle pieces at a time. Pairs always consisted of one SSRI- and one placebo-treated subject, and novel partners were assigned at each assessment. Neither the subjects nor the experimenter knew the experimental condition of pair members. One of the subjects in the SSRI group did not participate in the baseline puzzle task because of scheduling conflicts.
Puzzle-solving sessions were videotaped without volunteers' knowledge by a hidden camera placed behind a one-way mirror, so as to preserve the authenticity of the volunteers' nonverbal behavior. At the end of the experiment, an interviewer explained the rationale for the hidden camera and offered volunteers an opportunity to have their behavioral records removed from the analyses and deleted. None chose this option; all released their behavioral records for analysis. Coders who were blind to the volunteers' condition subsequently scored the videotapes for various objective social behaviors indicative of cooperation. Specifically, making suggestions while a partner handled the puzzle pieces was considered cooperative, but issuing commands while the partner handled the puzzle pieces was not. In addition, grasping the pieces with the intent of arriving at a unilateral solution was not considered cooperative. Coders attained significant agreement (intraclass r=0.73, p<0.001) on an aggregate measure composed of these behaviors (i.e., behavioral aggregate=(number of suggestions–number of commands)–number of unilateral grasps), which served as the index of affiliative behavior in subsequent analyses.

Plasma Measures

To determine whether individual differences in SSRI bioavailability might exist and potentially influence results, we assayed paroxetine levels in peripheral blood after 4 weeks of treatment. Assays were performed on samples of venous blood drawn at baseline and week 4. The baseline sample was drawn following an overnight fast, and the week 4 sample was also drawn following an overnight fast, 10 hours after the final dose of paroxetine. Plasma paroxetine was quantitated from these samples by using liquid chromatography with fluorescence detection following precolumn derivatization with dansyl chloride (26). Intra- and interassay coefficients of variation were all less than 10%.

Physical Symptoms

Participants rated the severity of 16 physical side effects (e.g., nausea, insomnia, trembling) on 7-point Likert scales in a semistructured interview format. Differences between groups in physical symptoms were analyzed by separate variance t tests with a Bonferroni correction (p for 32 comparisons=0.0016).

Statistical Analysis

Exploratory data analysis revealed that plasma paroxetine levels varied dramatically among treated individuals (range=0–44 ng/ml, with an approximate fivefold variation across the interquartile range: 5–24 ng/ml) and that changes in both psychometric and behavioral indices were strongly correlated with changes in plasma paroxetine level. Thus, psychometric and behavioral data were analyzed in two ways: 1) with a traditional group-based approach (i.e., a 2-by-3 [treatment-by-time within] repeated measures analysis of variance), and 2) with a similar statistical model that substituted continuous changes in plasma paroxetine levels for the dichotomous grouping variable as a predictor of psychometric and behavioral changes (27). Five individuals in the SSRI-treated group showed plasma paroxetine levels below 5 ng/ml, suggesting either failure to comply with the treatment regimen or negligible bioavailability. Thus, these individuals were excluded a priori from the group-based (but not plasma-based) analyses. All individuals were included in the plasma analysis, and the plasma change value assigned to control subjects was 0, since they did not take the medication.
Differential rates of change in the SSRI-treated and placebo-treated groups were assessed by a series of pairwise contrasts on change scores from one time point to another. Specifically, effects related to early physiological changes were assessed by a planned contrast comparing changes from baseline to week 1 across groups. Effects related to later physiological changes were assessed by a second planned contrast comparing changes from baseline to week 4 across groups. Finally, differential effects of early and later physiological changes were assessed by a third planned contrast comparing changes from week 1 to week 4. All data were standardized by using the pooled mean and standard deviation of scores at baseline. All a priori hypothesis tests were two-tailed and used p<0.05 as the significance criterion.

RESULTS

Preliminary analyses of psychometric variables, behavioral variables, and physical symptoms with sex and treatment as dual factors yielded no significant interactions of sex with treatment (although women did have significantly lower assaultiveness scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory than did men at all assessment points). Thus, sex was not included as a factor in the analyses that follow in order to preserve adequate statistical power to test the hypotheses. Results for group analyses are listed in table 1, while results for plasma analyses are listed in table 2.

Psychometric Measures

Group analyses indicated that assaultiveness scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory declined significantly for the SSRI-treated group relative to placebo control subjects at both week 1 and week 4 (treatment-by-time interaction: F=5.04, df=2,82, p<0.01) (table 1). While omnibus tests did not indicate a statistically significant decrement in irritability scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory for the SSRI-treated group overall (treatment-by-time interaction: F=2.45, df=2,82, p<0.10), pairwise contrasts suggested a significant reduction at week 4. There were no significant group differences in changes in either assaultiveness or irritability scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory from week 1 to week 4. Plasma-based analyses indicated that decreases in assaultiveness and irritability scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory at week 4 were both significantly related to increased plasma paroxetine at the end of the experiment (table 2).
Group analyses also indicated that negative affect scores on the Positive and Negative Affect Scales decreased for the SSRI-treated group relative to placebo subjects at both week 1 and week 4 (treatment-by-time interaction: F=3.30, df=2,82, p<0.05). There were no significant group differences in changes in negative affect scores from week 1 to week 4. Plasma-based analyses also revealed that reductions in negative affect scores at week 1 and week 4 were significantly related to plasma paroxetine at the end of the experiment (table 2 and figure 1). Unlike negative affect scores, positive affect scores did not change significantly as a function of SSRI treatment (treatment-by-time interaction: F=1.38, df=2,82, p=0.26), and changes in positive affect scores were not related to plasma paroxetine levels at the end of the experiment.
Analyses of covariance were conducted to determine whether global decreases in negative affect scores on the Positive and Negative Affect Scales could account for more focal changes in assaultiveness and irritability scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory from baseline to the treatment period (the average of weeks 1 and 4) (28). Statistically controlling for changes in negative affect scores rendered relationships between plasma paroxetine levels and changes in assaultiveness and irritability scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory nonsignificant. However, the relationship between plasma paroxetine and changes in negative affect scores remained statistically significant despite control for changes in assaultiveness scores (F=7.17, df=1,45, p<0.01) and irritability scores (F=4.63, df=1,45, p<0.05). Similar results obtained when items directly related to hostility were removed from the negative affect scale. Thus, in this group, SSRI-induced reductions in negative affect scores on the Positive and Negative Affect Scales (including feelings not directly related to hostility such as anxiety and depression) could statistically account for more specific reductions in assaultiveness and irritability scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory.

Behavioral Measure

Group analyses indicated that the SSRI-treated group showed more affiliative behavior in the puzzle task than the placebo-treated group at week 1, but not at week 4 (treatment-by-time interaction: F=3.17, df=2,80, p<0.05). However, the SSRI- and placebo-treated groups did not show differential change in their affiliative behavior from week 1 to week 4. Plasma-based analyses indicated that affiliative behavior at both week 1 and week 4 was significantly related to plasma paroxetine levels at the end of the experiment (table 2 and figure 2).

Physical Symptoms

Of all physical symptoms assessed, the SSRI-treated group reported significantly more sleepiness at week 1 and significantly delayed orgasms at week 4 (men and women combined). Increased sleepiness at week 1 was not significantly correlated with changes in the psychometric or behavioral variables at week 1, and delayed orgasm at week 4 was not significantly correlated with changes in psychometric or behavioral variables at week 4. Thus, these physical symptoms were not included as covariates in any of the preceding analyses. However, increased sleepiness at week 1 (but not delayed orgasm at week 4) was significantly correlated with plasma paroxetine levels at the end of the experiment (r=0.51, N=23, p<0.05).

DISCUSSION

These data indicate that SSRI administration can modulate some aspects of personality in normal human volunteers. Furthermore, these changes show some functional selectivity. First, SSRI administration reduced psychometric assaultiveness relative to placebo; this finding extends the generalizability of clinical observations that SSRIs can reduce hostile sentiment and violent outbursts in aggressive psychiatric patients (29). Second, SSRI administration reduced negative affect relative to placebo, and this shift could statistically account for reductions in assaultiveness, which suggests that SSRIs may modulate a broader range of affective variables than those strictly related to hostility. Third, SSRI administration did not significantly alter positive affect, which indicates that global effects on arousal (as in the case of sedation) could not account for the observed reductions in negative affect. While changes in positive affect did show a nonsignificant trend toward diminution at week 1, the change was not significant and was unrelated to plasma paroxetine levels at the study's conclusion. Overall, these findings support theories which posit that separable neurochemical substrates modulate the expression of negative and positive affect (30, 31).
Besides modulating psychometric indices of negative affect, SSRI administration also enhanced behavioral indices of social affiliation in a cooperative task. While collaboratively solving a puzzle, SSRI-treated partners scored higher on an affiliative behavioral composite consisting of increased suggestions, decreased commands, and decreased unilateral solution attempts at week 1 of testing. It is unclear why this group difference did not persist until week 4, but affiliative behavior at week 4 nevertheless remained correlated with plasma paroxetine levels. Drops in affiliative behavior for both groups across time may have diminished potential group differences at week 4. These findings in human volunteers complement primate studies in which chronic SSRI treatment of male vervet monkeys enhanced a constellation of affiliative behaviors, which in turn led to increased status (19). However, affiliative behavior may raise one's status only in certain social contexts (e.g., in the absence of a preexisting dominance hierarchy, in the presence of peers who reciprocate affiliative behavior [32, 33]). Thus, while this work suggests that chronic SSRI administration can enhance affiliative behavior of normal volunteers in a cooperative task with novel partners, more research is needed on interpersonal effects of SSRI treatment in other types of social scenarios (e.g., competitive).
Analyses across time points suggested that both psychometric and behavioral effects at week 1 did not change appreciably by week 4. This may indicate that early SSRI-induced physiological changes (i.e., increased synaptic availability of serotonin) are at least sufficient to initiate psychometric and behavioral effects in normal subjects. However, it is not clear whether the persistence of these effects until week 4 is also due to early physiological changes or to later ones more commonly associated with antidepressant response (e.g., autoreceptor desensitization). Further, our data do not address whether the observed effects would persist or dwindle over longer periods of SSRI administration.
Besides differences between groups, the magnitude of changes in psychometric assaultiveness, irritability, negative affect, and behavioral affiliation was correlated with plasma levels of SSRI among SSRI-treated volunteers. Indeed, plasma SSRI predicted functional changes more robustly than did group assignment. This association is somewhat surprising, given that plasma SSRI levels do not consistently predict antidepressant response in clinically depressed patients (34). On the other hand, depressed patients who eventually respond to tricyclic antidepressants do show early changes in negative affect (e.g., at 10 days) that are more strongly associated with subsequent shifts in cerebrospinal serotonin metabolites than with depressive status per se (35). Thus, our measures may be tapping a continuum of early subsyndromal changes that either presage or bear no relation to later therapeutic responses in psychiatric patients. Others have recently reported that 6 weeks of SSRI (fluoxetine) administration did not alter the affect of normal volunteers (36), but these investigators sampled a smaller number of subjects (N=6) and did not include individual difference measures of SSRI metabolism. Verification of the utility of these measures in predicting affective change awaits further replication.
Clearly, volunteers were not all equally affected by SSRI administration. An open-ended interview at the study's conclusion revealed that SSRI effects were often manifested in subtle and seemingly idiosyncratic ways. For example, SSRI-treated volunteers' responses to the question “What did you experience during the experiment?” ranged from “I used to think about good and bad, but now I don't; I'm in a good mood” (highest plasma SSRI level) to “The side effects were intense at first but then tapered off” (lowest plasma SSRI level). Individual differences in responsiveness to the manipulation may have arisen from several sources including (but not limited to) peripheral metabolic differences (e.g., enzymatic breakdown in the liver), central neurochemical differences (e.g., efficacy of the reuptake mechanism, postsynaptic receptor sensitivity), preexisting personality differences (e.g., baseline levels of chronic negative affect), or a combination of these factors. Unfortunately, the current design did not allow us to test for predisposing predictors of change, since correlation of change scores with the baseline scores used in their calculation invokes collinearity. Future studies might address these questions through repeated measures of relevant constructs at baseline.
In sum, this is the first empirical demonstration that chronic administration of a selective serotonin reuptake blockade can have significant personality and behavioral effects in normal humans in the absence of baseline depression or other psychopathology. These findings concur with the recent discovery of an association between a genetic polymorphism that regulates the efficacy of serotonin reuptake and negative affective components of the trait neuroticism. No such association has been observed between the polymorphism and other traits, including positive affective components of extraversion (37). While the primary application of SSRIs lies within the realm of treating psychiatric disorders, the present work indicates that these agents may provide psychological researchers with powerful tools for the “pharmacological dissection” of distinct phenomenological aspects of normal personality (38). Studies of normal volunteers with other pharmacologically selective agents may help to further delineate the specificity of serotonergic mechanisms in modulating affect and thereby may enable researchers to elucidate other psychobiological substrates of personality.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
FIGURE 1. Correlation of Changes in Negative Affect With Plasma Paroxetine Levels at the End of the Experiment for Normal Volunteers Receiving an SSRIa
ar=–0.44, N=23, p<0.05. Change scores were calculated as the mean of week 1 and week 4 scores minus baseline.
FIGURE 2. Correlation of Changes in Affiliative Behavior With Plasma Paroxetine Levels at the End of the Experiment for Normal Volunteers Receiving an SSRIa,b
ar=0.65, N=22, p<0.01. Change scores were calculated as the mean of week 1 and week 4 scores minus baseline.
bOne subject's behavioral data were not available because of scheduling conflicts.

Footnote

Presented in part at the 149th annual meeting of the American Psychiatric Association, New York, May 4–9, 1996, and at the New York Academy of Sciences Conference on Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation, Washington, D.C., March 14–17, 1996. Received April 15, 1997; revision received Sept. 3, 1997; accepted Oct. 9, 1997. From the Langley Porter Psychiatric Institute, University of California, San Francisco. Address reprint requests to Dr. Reus, Langley Porter Psychiatric Institute, 401 Parnassus Ave., San Francisco, CA 94143-0984; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by University of California, San Francisco, Research Allocation and Evaluation Committee grant 524985-35835 (to Dr. Reus and Dr. Knutson) and by NIMH Postdoctoral Training grants MH-18931 (to Dr. Knutson) and MH-15750 (to Dr. Cole). The authors thank Paul Ekman for the use of his laboratory, Thomas Cooper for plasma paroxetine assays, Francesca Manfredi for assistance with data collection, and Dennis Murphy for comments on the manuscript.

References

1.
Digman JM: Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model. Annu Rev Psychol 1990; 41:417–440
2.
Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR: Set like plaster: evidence for the stability of adult personality, in Can Personality Change? Edited by Heatherton TF, Weinberger JL. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1994, pp 21–40
3.
Bouchard TJ Jr: Genes, environment, and personality. Science 1994; 264:1700–1701
4.
Cloninger CR: A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: a proposal. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:573–588
5.
Depue RA, Luciana M, Arbisi P, Collins P, Leon A: Dopamine and the structure of personality: relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. J Pers Soc Psychol 1994; 67:485–498
6.
Zuckerman M: Good and bad humors: biochemical bases of personality and its disorders. Psychol Science 1995; 6:325–332
7.
Virkkunen M, Rawlings R, Tokola R, Poland RE, Guidotti A, Nemeroff C, Bissette G, Kalogeras K, Karonen SL, Linnoila M: CSF biochemistries, glucose metabolism, and diurnal activity rhythms in alcoholic, violent offenders, fire setters, and healthy volunteers. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:20–27
8.
Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ: Neurotransmitter correlates of impulsive aggression, in Aggression and Violence: Genetic, Neurobiological, and Biosocial Perspectives. Edited by Stoff DM, Cairns RB. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1996, pp 67–85
9.
Fuller RW: The influence of fluoxetine on aggressive behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 1996; 14:77–81
10.
Ellenbogen MA, Young SN, Dean P, Palmour RM, Benkelfat C: Mood response to acute tryptophan depletion in healthy volunteers: sex differences and temporal stability. Neuropsychopharmacology 1996; 15:465–474
11.
Smith SE, Pihl RO, Young SN, Ervin FR: A test of possible cognitive and environmental influences on the mood lowering effect of tryptophan depletion in normal males. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987; 91:451–457
12.
Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Swann AC, Collins D, Davis CM, Cherek DR: Tryptophan depletion and aggressive responding in healthy males. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996; 126:97–103
13.
Cleare AJ, Bond AJ: The effect of tryptophan depletion and enhancement on subjective and behavioral aggression in normal male subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995; 118:72–81
14.
Higley JD, Mehlman PT, Higley SB, Fernald B, Vickers J, Lindell SG, Taub DM, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Excessive mortality in young free-ranging male nonhuman primates with low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:537–543
15.
Higley JD, King ST Jr, Hasert MF, Champoux M, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Stability of interindividual differences in serotonin function and its relationship to severe aggression and competent social behavior in rhesus macaque females. Neuropsychopharmacology 1996; 14:67–76
16.
Mehlman PT, Higley JD, Faucher I, Lilly AA, Taub DM, Vickers J, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Correlation of CSF 5-HIAA concentration with socialty and the timing of emigration in free-ranging primates. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:907–913
17.
Raleigh JM, Brammer GL, Ritvo ER, Geller E, McGuire MT, Yuwiler A: Effects of chronic fenfluramine on blood serotonin, cerebrospinal fluid metabolites, and behavior in monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988; 90:503–508
18.
Raleigh MJ: Differential behavioral effects of tryptophan and 5-hydroxytryptophan in vervet monkeys: influence of catecholaminergic systems. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987; 93:44–50
19.
Raleigh MJ, McGuire MT, Brammer GL, Pollack DB, Yuwiler A: Serotonergic mechanisms promote dominance acquisition in adult male vervet monkeys. Brain Res 1991; 559:181–190
20.
Hyttel J: Pharmacological characterization of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1994; 9(suppl 1):19–26
21.
Carlson JN, Visker KE, Nielsen DM, Keller RW Jr, Glick SD: Chronic antidepressant drug treatment reduces turning behavior and increases dopamine levels in the medial prefrontal cortex. Brain Res 1996; 707:122–126
22.
Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R—Non-Patient Edition (SCID-NP, Version 1.0). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1990
23.
Buss AH, Durkee A: An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. J Consult Psychol 1957; 21:343–349
24.
Siever L, Trestman RL: The serotonin system and aggressive personality disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1993; 8(suppl 2):33–39
25.
Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A: Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54:1063–1070
26.
Brett MA, Dierdorf HD, Zussman BD, Coates PE: Determination of paroxetine in human plasma, using high-performance liquid chromatography with fluorescence detection. J Chromatogr 1987; 419:438–444
27.
Rogosa DR, Willett JB: Understanding correlates of change by modeling individual differences in growth. Psychometrika 1985; 50:203–208
28.
Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986; 51:1173–1182
29.
Coccaro EF: Impulsive aggression and central serotonergic system function in humans: an example of a dimensional brain-behavior relationship. Int J Psychopharmacol 1992; 7:3–12
30.
Tellegen A: Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report, in Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders. Edited by Tuma AH, Maser J. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1985, pp 681–706
31.
Depue RA, Luciana M, Arbisi P, Collins P, Leon A: Dopamine and the structure of personality: relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. J Pers Soc Psychol 1994; 67:485–498
32.
Raleigh MJ, Brammer GL, McGuire MT, Yuwiler A: Dominant social status facilitates the behavioral effects of serotonergic agonists. Brain Res 1985; 348:274–282
33.
Knutson B, Panksepp J: Effects of fluoxetine treatment on play dominance in juvenile rats. Aggressive Behavior 1996; 22:297–307
34.
van Harten J: Clinical pharmacokinetics of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Clin Pharmacokinet 1993; 24:203–220
35.
Katz MM, Maas JW, Frazer A, Koslow SH, Bowden CL, Berman N, Swann AC, Stokes PE: Drug-induced actions on brain neurotransmitter systems and changes in the behaviors and emotions of depressed patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 1994; 11:89–100
36.
Barr LC, Heninger GR, Goodman W, Charney DS, Price LH: Effects of fluoxetine administration on mood response to tryptophan depletion in healthy subjects. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:949–954
37.
Lesch K-P, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Benjamin J, Muller CR, Hamer DH, Murphy DL: Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 1996; 274:1527–1531
38.
Klein DF: Anxiety reconceptualized: gleaning from pharmacological dissection: early experience with imipramine and anxiety. Mod Probl Pharmacopsychiatry 1987; 22:1–35

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 373 - 379
PubMed: 9501748

History

Published online: 1 March 1998
Published in print: March 1998

Authors

Affiliations

Owen M. Wolkowitz, M.D.
Elizabeth A. Moore, Ph.D.
Ronald C. Johnson, Ph.D.
Rebecca A. Turner, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share