Skip to main content

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study was to characterize the neuropsychological presentation of geriatric depression and to determine whether depression-related executive dysfunction is more pronounced during advanced age. METHOD: The attention and executive functioning of 40 adults with major depression were compared with those of 40 healthy comparison subjects; 20 subjects were 20–60 years old, and 20 were ≥61 years. It was hypothesized that depressed subjects, regardless of age, would perform more poorly than comparison subjects on both attention and executive tasks but that the older depressed adults would evidence significantly greater impairment on executive measures. RESULTS: A significant interaction between age and depressive status was noted for tasks of executive functioning, while no age-depression interaction was found for tasks of selective or sustained attention. Older depressed adults demonstrated the slowest psychomotor speed and the poorest performance on tasks requiring set shifting, problem solving, and initiation of novel responses. CONCLUSIONS: Patients with late-life depression have significant impairment in executive functioning. These findings can guide the development of stimulated functional neuroimaging paradigms that may clarify the pathophysiology of geriatric depression. Timely identification of attentional and executive processes fundamental to the daily functioning of depressed older adults may lead to compensatory strategies that will improve the outcomes of late-life depression.
Neuropsychological deficits are a potential part of the clinical presentation in late-life depression. Geriatric depression has been associated with impairment in short-term memory (1), visuospatial skills (2, 3), and psychomotor functioning (1). Studies of geriatric patients with major depression have documented disturbances in executive functioning (35), including impaired planning, organizing, initiating, sequencing, shifting, information processing speed, and working memory.
A review of the literature revealed inconsistent findings regarding executive deficits in depression and suggests that frontal impairment varies according to age and depression severity (6). Severely depressed older adults have been found to evidence impairment in set shifting, verbal fluency, psychomotor speed, recognition memory, and planning on the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (7). In another study using the same battery (8), moderately depressed middle-aged patients demonstrated deficits in planning, strategy development, spatial working memory, and verbal fluency despite exhibiting intact set shifting ability and psychomotor speed. In yet another study (9), severe depression was associated with deficits in set shifting but intact verbal fluency. Despite some equivocal findings in the literature, it is generally accepted that while performance on tasks requiring development of performance strategies may suffer in depressed patients, automatic processes are relatively preserved (10).
Although focal deficits or even severe global impairment are observed in some depressed elderly patients, the cognitive functioning of others remains intact. This variation may result from the biologic heterogeneity of depression. Studies of the cognitive response to psychopharmacological treatment of late-life depression indicate that a substantial number of patients continue to experience residual signs of the disorder, including neuropsychological deficits. Persisting mild memory and executive impairment have been observed in elderly depressed patients who achieved remission with antidepressant treatment (4).
Recently, one of us (11) proposed that depressive symptoms and executive impairment originate from related brain dysfunction, at least in a subgroup of elderly patients. The clinical presentation of elderly patients with the “depression–executive dysfunction syndrome” is characterized by psychomotor retardation and reduced interest in activities but a less pronounced vegetative syndrome than is seen in depressed elderly patients without significant executive dysfunction (12). The neuropsychological dysfunction of patients with depression–executive dysfunction syndrome consists of impaired verbal fluency and visual naming and poor performance on tasks of initiation and perseveration (12). Depressive ideation, psychomotor retardation, and executive dysfunction in depressed elderly patients are associated with compromised instrumental activities of daily living (13, 14).
Several symptoms of depression–executive dysfunction syndrome, namely psychomotor retardation, apathy, and fluency deficits, resemble impairments exhibited in frontal lobe syndromes. However, in contrast to patients with frontal lobe syndromes, patients with depression–executive dysfunction syndrome meet criteria for major depression and have depressive symptoms comparable in severity to those of geriatric patients with major depression who have unimpaired executive functions (15). Delayed or poor (16) and unstable (15) responses to antidepressants have been documented in elderly patients with major depression and executive dysfunction. In contrast, memory impairment does not predict antidepressant response, relapse, or recurrence of late-life depression (15).
Neuroimaging studies of major depression have identified neuroanatomical and neurophysiological abnormalities most consistently in the frontal lobes. Magnetic resonance imaging of depressed patients has revealed structural abnormalities in areas related to the cortical-striatal-pallidal-thalamus-cortical pathways (17), including the frontal lobes (18), caudate (19), and putamen (20). Positron emission tomography has shown hypometabolism in a variety of areas that mediate executive functions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (2123), anterior cingulate (24, 25), and basal ganglia (26). Hypometabolism of the rostral anterior cingulate was found to be associated with resistance to antidepressants in younger depressed patients (25). Notably, however, an increase in frontal hypometabolism has been reported after successful treatment with antidepressant medications (27, 28).
Subtle executive dysfunction occurs with normal aging (29), prompting questions about the concomitant effects of aging and depression on neuropsychological functioning. Some have argued that poor neuropsychological functioning may result from the combined effects of aging and depression. Raskin and colleagues (30, 31) maintain that an interaction between aging and depression minimizes cognitive flexibility and compromises processing speed. Neurobiological models of aging and of depression that have common neurophysiologic and neuroanatomic substrates have been proposed (32). Accordingly, delineation of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression is both theoretically and clinically important.
The purpose of the present study was to characterize the neuropsychological presentation of geriatric depression with a particular focus on attention and executive functions. On the basis of a neuropsychological approach (33, 34), four neurocognitive domains were studied: two involving attentional processes (selective attention and sustained attention) and two involving higher-order executive functions (inhibitory control and focused effort). Selective attention requires perceptual sensitivity to signal/noise differences, reflecting the ability to discriminate target items from distractor items. Sustained attention refers to the temporal consistency in performance, specifically the ability to persist over time as a function of overall productivity. Inhibitory control involves initiation, active switching, and inhibition of overlearned responses. Finally, focused effort refers to the intensity of directed attention and involves working memory, speed of processing, and complex mental operations. Selective and sustained attention tasks involve orienting responses and execution of learned sequences. In contrast, inhibitory control and focused effort involve novel situations that necessitate planning, error correction, suppression of temptation, or spontaneous development of performance strategies.
It was hypothesized that depressed subjects, regardless of age, would demonstrate poor performance on attention tasks and on tasks purportedly subserved by the frontal lobes (i.e., tasks requiring response initiation, behavioral inhibition, active set shifting, and quick processing speed). It was further hypothesized that older depressed adults would have a significantly greater degree of impairment than their younger depressed counterparts on these tasks, while no interaction between age and depression was anticipated for the tasks of selective or sustained attention.

Method

Study Group

Young and elderly adults with major depressive disorder (N=40) were consecutively recruited for participation through the Cornell Institute for Geriatric Psychiatry. Young and elderly healthy comparison subjects (N=40) with no psychiatric history were recruited from the community. Upon recruitment, all study candidates were assessed with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (35) and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia—Lifetime Version (SADS-L) (36) by an experienced clinician. The exclusion criteria included 1) significant medical illness (i.e., history of metastatic cancer, brain tumor, myocardial infarction, or stroke), 2) history of head trauma or epilepsy, 3) delirium, dementia, Huntington’s chorea, or Parkinson’s disease, 4) history of substance abuse, 5) hearing, vision, or motor impairment that precluded neuropsychological testing, and 6) an MMSE score lower than 24. All of the depressed subjects met the Research Diagnostic Criteria (37) for a current diagnosis of major depressive disorder without psychotic features. Depressed patients with a history of manic episodes, treatment with ECT, or comorbid psychiatric disorder were excluded. All subjects signed informed consent statements.
The subjects were grouped by diagnosis (depressed versus comparison) and by current age (young versus old; young=20–60 years, old=61 years and above). The four subject groups (young comparison, young depressed, old comparison, old depressed) were composed of 20 subjects each.

Instruments

All subjects were evaluated by means of a test battery comprising the following neuropsychological instruments: California Verbal Learning Test (38), Conners’ Computerized Continuous Performance Test (39), category fluency test (40, pp. 447–464), digit span and digit symbol subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition (WAIS-III) (41), finger tapping test (42, pp. 229–234), Stroop Color and Word Test (43), Trail Making Test (42, pp. 279–288), Visual Cancellation Test (44), Auditory Cancellation Test (44; 45, pp. 548–549), and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (46). Task variables selected from these instruments were grouped to render indices of performance on four neuropsychological domains: selective attention, sustained attention, inhibitory control, and focused effort (Appendix 1).

Domain 1: selective attention

Selective attention was evaluated with d prime (d′) indices derived from five tests: a) the Continuous Performance Test—a fixed-interval computerized test of vigilance requiring the subject to quickly press a button in response to targets while refraining from responding to nontargets; b) the Visual Cancellation Test—a paper-and-pencil measure of visual scanning requiring cancellation of targets intermittently placed in a large array; c) the Auditory Cancellation Test—an auditory adaptation of visual cancellation requiring the subject to raise his or her hand in response to targets while listening to a standardized audiotape during which letters are read aloud; d) the Stroop Color and Word Test—a task requiring rapid naming of the color of ink in which words representing incongruent colors are printed; and e) the California Verbal Learning Test—a test requiring recall and recognition of items from an orally presented shopping list.
The d′ variables for these tests served as indices of perceptual sensitivity, that is, the ability to discriminate target items from distractor items. All measures of d′ were calculated by using the following formula adapted from signal detection theory (47): Zc–Zh (where Zc represents the normal curve deviate for the proportion of commission errors and Zh represents the normal curve deviate for the proportion of hits). Statistically, this sensitivity measure represents the distance along the x axis between the noise distribution and the signal distribution, in standard-score units.

Domain 2: sustained attention

Sustained attention was assessed by using decrement scores derived from four tests: a) the WAIS-III digit symbol subtest—a test requiring rapid copying of symbols paired with numbers; b) category fluency test—a test requiring rapid generation of animal names in 60 seconds; c) sustained finger tapping—tapping with the index finger of the dominant hand over 60 seconds; and d) the color-word task from the Stroop Color and Word Test. Data for these measures were recorded in blocks to provide an index of the temporal consistency in performance. Decrement scores were determined by calculating the discrepancy in productivity between the first and last halves of the test as a function of total productivity. For example, for the category fluency test, the number of words generated during the last half of the test was subtracted from word generation during the first half, and the difference was divided by the total number of words produced. This approach permitted investigation of attention functioning over time while avoiding erroneous generalizations about decrement in performance for the subjects with minimal overall productivity. In addition to these decrement scores, sustained attention was assessed by using a reaction time block change score from the Continuous Performance Test (slope of change in reaction times over the six time blocks).

Domain 3: inhibitory control

Measures of active set shifting and perseveration were used to assess inhibitory control. These included a) commission errors on the Continuous Performance Test—an index of nontarget responses on the computerized vigilance test; b) perseverative errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test—a test requiring the subject to sort cards with no explanation of the sorting principles; c) perseverative errors on the California Verbal Learning Test; d) perseverative errors on the category fluency test; and e) completion time for Trail Making Test part B—a paper-and-pencil test of alphanumeric sequencing.

Domain 4: focused effort

The focused effort domain was assessed by way of tasks requiring effortful processing, mental manipulation, or quick processing speed. These included a) number of items completed for the WAIS-III digit symbol subtest; b) items completed for the WAIS-III digits backward subtest—a task requiring verbal recall of digit strings in a backward sequence; c) total words produced for category fluency; d) items completed on the color-word trial of the Stroop Color and Word Test; and e) Continuous Performance Test reaction time—the mean response time in milliseconds for all target responses over the six time blocks.

Data Analysis

The performance ratings of the four subject groups on each of the four neuropsychological domains were compared by using multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs). Univariate analyses with pairwise comparisons were then performed to examine specific group differences. For the omnibus tests, alpha was set at 0.05. For the pairwise comparisons, the alpha level was adjusted by using Bonferroni correction (p<0.01).

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

The overall study group comprised 19 men (23.8%) and 61 women (76.3%); their mean age was 55.1 years (SD=21.3, range=20–87), and their mean education level was 15.3 years (SD=2.6, range=12–23). The subjects were predominantly Caucasian but included three African Americans. Depression severity was assessed with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (48), which revealed that as a whole, the depressed patients were severely depressed (mean score=30.38, SD=7.17). Of the depressed patients, 21 were taking psychotropic medications at the time of testing: 14 were taking an antidepressant only (eight young, six old), two were taking a benzodiazepine only (one young, one old), and five were taking an antidepressant coupled with a benzodiazepine (four young, one old). Table 1 lists the demographic and clinical characteristics of the four subject groups.
As shown in Table 1, the young comparison and young depressed subjects were well matched for age (t=–1.62, df=38, p=0.11), as were the old comparison and old depressed subjects (t=–1.53, df=38, p=0.14). There were no significant differences between the depressed and comparison groups in years of education (t=0.04, df=78, p=0.97). Nor did a one-way analysis of variance of education level indicate significant differences among all four subject groups (F=1.21, df=3, 76, p=0.32). No significant difference in depression severity, as assessed by the Hamilton depression scale, was found between the two depressed subgroups (t=1.69, df=38, p=0.10). Table 2 lists the neuropsychological scores of the four subject groups.

Results by Neuropsychological Domain

Domain 1: Selective Attention

MANOVA revealed no significant interaction between age and depressive state for the selective attention domain (Wilks’s lambda=0.98, F=0.32, df=5, 72, p=0.90). Relative to the comparison subjects, however, the depressed patients performed more poorly on this domain (Wilks’s lambda=0.61, F=9.40, df=5, 72, p<0.001). The depressed patients exhibited greater difficulty discriminating targets from nontargets in signal detection tasks (Auditory Cancellation Test d′, Continuous Performance Test d′, California Verbal Learning Test d′). As a group, older subjects performed more poorly than younger subjects on selective attention tasks (Wilks’s lambda=0.59, F=10.18, df=5, 72, p<0.001). They exhibited a deficit in the ability to appropriately select the relevant stimulus dimension (Stroop Color and Word Test d′) and less accurate differentiation of target and distractor stimuli (Visual Cancellation Test d′, Auditory Cancellation Test d′, California Verbal Learning Test d′). However, when the comparisons were restricted to the healthy subjects, the older subjects showed no significant deficits in selective attention.

Domain 2: Sustained Attention

In keeping with the results from the selective attention tests, MANOVA revealed no significant age-depression interaction associated with the sustained attention domain (Wilks’s lambda=0.93, F=1.67, df=5, 72, p=0.33). The depressed patients performed significantly worse than the comparison subjects on this domain (Wilks’s lambda=0.74, F=4.95, df=5, 72, p=0.001) but showed a more precipitous decline in performance from the initial to the latter stages for only a single test (category fluency). A significant age effect was found for the sustained attention domain (Wilks’s lambda=0.72, F=5.57, df=5, 72, p<0.001), although only on the category fluency task did the elderly patients as a group evidence significantly greater difficulty maintaining performance over the course of the task. Further, the sustained attention of the healthy older adults was comparable to that of their younger healthy counterparts.

Domain 3: Inhibitory Control

As hypothesized, MANOVA revealed a significant age-depression interaction for the inhibitory control domain (Wilks’s lambda=0.74, F=5.05, df=5, 72, p=0.001). The elderly depressed adults demonstrated the most difficulty with active set shifting, as evidenced by disproportionately poor scores on Trail Making Test part B. They also showed the greatest tendency to produce perseverative responses on tasks of conceptualization (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) and initiation (category fluency). The young depressed patients produced the highest number of commission errors on the Continuous Performance Test, and the depressed group as a whole produced a significantly higher number of commission errors than the comparison subjects.

Domain 4: Focused Effort

In keeping with the results from the inhibitory control tasks, MANOVA for the focused effort domain revealed a significant age-depression interaction (Wilks’s lambda=0.85, F=2.57, df=5, 72, p=0.03). The elderly depressed patients demonstrated the longest response latencies on the Continuous Performance Test and copied the fewest symbols on the WAIS-III digit symbol subtest, suggesting compromised processing speed. They also demonstrated the weakest capacity for initiation on the category fluency task and the slowest processing time on the color-word trial of the Stroop Color and Word Test (as indicated by fewer completed items).

Discussion

The principal finding of this study is that geriatric depression is characterized by impaired executive functioning. Relative to their depressed younger counterparts and to the healthy elderly cohort, depressed geriatric subjects had disproportionately poor scores on neuropsychological tasks associated with frontal lobe integrity. Dissection of the neuropsychological impairment in the current group of depressed geriatric patients reveals primary impairment of response initiation and inhibition, active switching, processing speed, and complex mental manipulation. While the stability over time of these deficits requires further investigation, the results of this study are in keeping with previous descriptions (49, 50) of an association between late-life depression and executive deficits.
With respect to the effects of depression, the total depressed group, as hypothesized, evidenced mild weaknesses in perceptual sensitivity and isolated difficulties in maintaining their level of performance over time. These results are in keeping with reports of deficits in both selective attention (51) and sustained attention (52) for depressed patients and suggest that depression may contribute to susceptibility to interference, with a resultant compromise in the ability to filter noise and attend to signals. In the present study, depressed patients also had a greater tendency to perseverate and committed significantly more Continuous Performance Test commission errors than healthy comparison subjects. These findings support previous work documenting difficulties in set shifting in depression (7) and impairments on tests requiring inhibition of responses to distractor stimuli, such as the Continuous Performance Test (53, 54).
With respect to age effects, the healthy older adults had no more difficulty discriminating relevant and distractor stimuli than did the young healthy adults, nor was older age in healthy adults associated with poor ability to maintain performance over the course of tasks. Similar reports indicating intact selective attention for healthy older adults are found in the literature (55), as are reports of a lack of fatigue effects during neuropsychological tasks (56). In contrast, executive dysfunction in set formation and set shifting (for both verbal and nonverbal domains) and slowed psychomotor speed have been documented for healthy older adults (29). The results from the current study reflect similar findings: the older healthy adults demonstrated deficits in set shifting and slower psychomotor performance relative to their healthy young counterparts.
The potential contribution of age effects to neurocognitive deficits found in this study must be considered, particularly since a frontal lobe hypothesis of age-related cognitive changes has been proposed (57). Our findings support such a hypothesis, given that relative to healthy young adults, the only deficits exhibited by the healthy older adults were in executive domains. Most critical to the study objectives, however, is the finding that executive deficits were associated with both older age and depression and that an interaction between these variables was documented for both neuropsychological domains of executive function. The results suggest deficits in processing resources for both depressed patients and older adults, with these impairments compounded for depressed geriatric patients. These deficits are most evident on tasks requiring set shifting, inhibition of prepotent responses, spontaneous use of learning strategies, and quick psychomotor speed.
The exact mechanisms underlying executive dysfunction in geriatric depression remain to be identified. A potential mechanism may be mediated by the cortical-striatal-pallidal-thalamus-cortical pathways, as these circuits are strongly implicated in the development of spontaneous performance strategies demanded by executive tasks. In particular, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex mediates planning and response sequencing (58), as well as focusing and active switching (33), while the orbitofrontal pathway subserves response initiation and inhibition (59). Response intention and focusing are also influenced by the cingulate cortex (60). Notably, abnormalities (of both cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism) in these areas have been consistently documented in positron emission tomographic studies of patients with major depression. In a recent review of functional neuroimaging studies, Liotti and Mayberg (61) concluded that a majority of findings implicate the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as a primary site of functional abnormalities associated with poor externally directed attention in major depression. They further contended that the anterior cingulate cortex may be the site of metabolic abnormalities associated with impaired ability to inhibit inappropriate responses in depressed individuals. Compounding the executive dysfunction in older depressed adults may be age-related hypometabolism (62) and volumetric reductions in prefrontal and orbital-frontal areas (63).
The clinical significance of this study is that the delineation of specific executive and attention deficits in depressed elderly patients may facilitate the development of effective treatment interventions. Timely identification of attentional and executive processes fundamental to the daily functioning of depressed older adults may lead to compensatory strategies that will improve the outcomes of late-life depression. Such interventions are much needed in these patients, because at least some of them may be resistant to pharmacotherapy (15, 16).
The heuristic significance of this study is that it outlines the cognitive functions most affected in geriatric depression and may be used for the development of models for understanding the role of age-related changes in late-life depression. If specific executive dysfunctions are attributable to impairment of cortical-striatal-pallidal-thalamus-cortical systems, at least two models can be proposed. In the first model, depression is viewed as a condition unmasking executive dysfunctions in patients with compromised cortical-striatal-pallidal-thalamus-cortical systems. In this model no pathophysiological relationship between dysfunction of these systems and the mechanisms of depression is assumed. In the second model, dysfunction of cortical-striatal-pallidal-thalamus-cortical pathways is conceptualized as mediating or predisposing to both depression and specific executive dysfunction (i.e., depression and executive dysfunctions are not independent). This assertion is supported by studies suggesting that the course of geriatric depression is influenced by some executive dysfunctions (49, 50). The theoretical advantage of the second model is that it can guide neuroimaging studies to investigate the functional neuroanatomy of cortical-striatal-pallidal-thalamus-cortical circuitry and its role in geriatric depression. Tasks of inhibitory control and focused effort may be modified and used as stimuli in neuroimaging studies, as these processes were found to be abnormal in this study.
This study addressed some of the limitations of previous investigations by using rigorous methodological constraints, including use of well-matched healthy comparison groups and stringent exclusion criteria. Assessment with a state measure (Hamilton depression scale) assured inclusion of depressed subjects with similar levels of current depression severity. Controlling for education and health status helped to prevent introduction of variance that might obscure group differences. Use of a comprehensive neuropsychological battery lent additional rigor by not only allowing for assessment of specific aspects of attention and executive functions, but also by tapping multiple performance modalities (verbal, visual, motor).
This study is limited by its cross-sectional design with discrete age groupings, a feature that precludes conclusions about the timing of age effects in patients with major depression. Ideally, a study of this nature would have examined age as a continuous variable. The small number of subjects prohibited such a design. Future studies should address the issue of whether age-related declines in depressed and nondepressed older adults are similar in their onset. The focus on subjects with severe depression led to the inclusion of some subjects who were receiving medication, although there were no significant differences in the mean number of young and old medicated patients in this study. While we cannot exclude an effect of medications on test performance, we believe that this concern is mitigated by the fact that additional analyses did not demonstrate differences between patient groups based on medication status for any of the four neuropsychological domains. Further, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and venlafaxine, which were the antidepressants used by most of our subjects, do not have a strong effect on neuropsychological test performance.
In conclusion, the theoretical contribution of this study is the delineation of attentional and executive processes in depressed older patients, which can be used to devise stimulated functional neuroimaging studies that would clarify the pathophysiology of geriatric depression. Findings from this study may be used clinically to guide the design of focused interventions. Specific cognitive retraining and/or remediation, as well as assistance with disability resulting from executive dysfunction, may prove to be effective therapeutic additions to the current treatment armamentarium for geriatric depression, which leaves many patients symptomatic and suffering.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
APPENDIX 1.

Footnote

Received May 3, 2001; revision received Feb. 1, 2002; accepted Feb. 19, 2002. Presented in part at the 108th annual convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., Aug. 4–8, 2000. From the Joan and Sanford I. Weill College of Medicine, Cornell University, New York; and the Institute of Geriatric Psychiatry, New York-Presbyterian Hospital. Address reprint requests to Dr. Alexopoulos, Institute of Geriatric Psychiatry, New York-Presbyterian Hospital, 21 Bloomingdale Rd., White Plains, NY 10605; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by NIMH grants MH-49762, MH-19132, MH-19132, MH-51842, and MH-59366 and grants from the Joseph Sanchez Foundation and the Dr. I Foundation.

References

1.
Hart RP, Kwentus JA, Taylor JR, Harkins SW: Rate of forgetting in dementia and depression. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:101-105
2.
Boone KB, Lesser IM, Miller BL, Wohl M, Berman N, Lee AB, Palmer B, Back C: Cognitive functioning in older depressed outpatients: relationship of presence and severity of depression to neuropsychological test scores. Neuropsychology 1995; 9:390-398
3.
Lesser IM, Boone KB, Mehringer CM, Wohl MA, Miller BL, Bergman NG: Cognition and white matter hyperintensities in older depressed adults. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:1280-1287
4.
Butters MA, Becker JT, Nebes RD, Zmuda MD, Mulsant BH, Pollock BG, Reynolds CF III: Changes in cognitive functioning following treatment of late-life depression. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1949-1954
5.
Nebes RD, Butters MA, Mulsant BH, Pollack BG, Zmuda MD, Houck PR, Reynolds CF: Decreased working memory and processing speed mediate cognitive impairment in geriatric depression. Psychol Med 2000; 30:679-691
6.
Goodwin GM: Neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal lobes in depression. J Psychopharmacol 1997; 11:115-122
7.
Beats BC, Sahakian BJ, Levy R: Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed. Psychol Med 1996; 26:591-603
8.
Elliot R, Sahakian BJ, McKay AP, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES: Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychol Med 1996; 26:975-989
9.
Jones B, Henderson M, Welch CA: Executive functions in unipolar depression before and after electroconvulsive therapy. Int J Neurosci 1988; 38:287-297
10.
Channon S, Green PS: Executive function in depression: the role of performance strategies in aiding depressed and non-depressed participants. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999; 66:162-171
11.
Alexopoulos GS: The depression-executive dysfunction syndrome of late life: a target for D3 receptor agonists. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001; 9:1-8
12.
Alexopoulos GS, Vrontou C, Kakuma T, Meyers BS, Young RC, Klausner E, Clarkin J: Disability in geriatric depression. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:877-885
13.
Kiosses DN, Alexopoulos GS, Murphy C: Symptoms of striatofrontal dysfunction contribute to disability in geriatric depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000; 15:992-999
14.
Lockwood KA, Alexopoulos GS, Kakuma T, van Gorp WG: Subtypes of cognitive impairment in depressed older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000; 8:201-208
15.
Alexopoulos GS, Meyers BS, Young RC, Kalayam B, Hull J, Kakuma T: Executive dysfunction increases the risk for relapse and recurrence of geriatric depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:285-290
16.
Kalayam B, Alexopoulos G: Prefrontal dysfunction and treatment response in geriatric depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:713-718
17.
George MS, Ketter TA, Post RM: Prefrontal cortex dysfunction in clinical depression. Depression 1994; 2:59-72
18.
Krishnan KR, McDonald WM, Doraiswamy PM, Tupler LA, Husain M, Boyko OB, Figiel GS: Neuroanatomical substrates of depression in the elderly. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1993; 243:41-46
19.
Krishnan KR, McDonald WM, Escalona PR, Doraiswamy PM, Husain MM, Figiel GS, Boyko OB, Ellinwood EH, Nemeroff CB: Magnetic imaging of the caudate nuclei in depression: preliminary observations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:553-557
20.
Husain MM, McDonald WM, Doraiswamy PM, Figiel GS, Escalona PR, Boyko OB, Nemeroff CB, Krishnan KR: A magnetic resonance imaging study of putamen nuclei in major depression. Psychiatry Res 1991; 40:95-99
21.
Baxter LR Jr, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC, Schwartz JM, Gerner RH, Selin CE, Sumida RM: Cerebral metabolic rates for glucose in mood disorders: studies with positron emission tomography and fluorodeoxyglucose F 18. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42:441-447
22.
Martinot JL, Hardy P, Feline A, Huret JD, Mazoyer B, Attar-Levy D, Pappata S, Syrota A: Left prefrontal glucose hypometabolism in the depressed state: a confirmation study. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:1313-1317
23.
Bench CJ, Friston KJ, Brown RG, Scott LC, Franckoviak RS, Dolan RS: The anatomy of melancholia: focal abnormalities of cerebral blood flow in major depression. Psychol Med 1992; 22:607-615
24.
Drevets WC, Price JL, Simpson JR, Todd RD, Reich T, Vannier M, Raichle ME: Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature 1997; 386:824-827
25.
Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, Brickman JS, Tekell JL, Silva JA, McGinnis S, Glass TG, Martin CC, Fox PT: Cingulate function in depression: a potential predictor of treatment response. Neuroreport 1997; 8:1057-1061
26.
Buchsbaum MS, Wu J, DeLisi LE, Holcomb H, Kessler R, Johnson J, King AC, Hazlett E, Langston K, Post RM: Frontal cortex and basal ganglia metabolic rates assessed by positron emission tomography with [18F]2-deoxyglucose in affective illness. J Affect Disord 1986; 10:137-152
27.
Mayberg HS, Liotti M, Brannan SK, McGinnis S, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, Silva JA, Tekell JL, Martin CC, Lancaster JL, Fox PT: Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:675-682
28.
Drevets WC: Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:813-819
29.
Keys BA, White DA: Exploring the relationship between age, executive abilities, and psychomotor speed. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6:76-82
30.
Raskin A, Friedman AS, DiMascio A: Cognitive and performance deficits in depression. Psychopharmacol Bull 1982; 18:196-202
31.
Raskin A: Partialing out the effects of depression and aging on cognitive functions: experimental data and methodological issues, in Handbook for Clinical Memory Assessment of Older Adults. Edited by Poon LW. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1986, pp 244-256
32.
Veith RC, Raskind MA: The neurobiology of aging: does it predispose to depression? Neurobiol Aging 1988; 9:101-117
33.
Cohen RA: The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York, Plenum Press, 1993
34.
Cohen RA, Malloy P, Jenkins M: Disorders of attention, in Clinical Neuropsychology. Edited by Snyder PJ, Nussbaum PD. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1998, pp 541-572
35.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189-198
36.
Spitzer RL, Endicott J: Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia—Lifetime Version, 3rd ed. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1979
37.
Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research Diagnostic Criteria: rationale and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35:773-782
38.
Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA: The California Verbal Learning Test Manual. New York, Psychological Corp, 1987
39.
Conners KC: Conners’ Continuous Performance Test Computer Program User’s Manual. Toronto, Multi-Health Systems, 1995
40.
Spreen O, Strauss E: A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests, 2nd ed. New York, Oxford University Press, 1998
41.
Wechsler D: Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1997
42.
Reitan RM, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery, 2nd ed. Tucson, Ariz, Neuropsychology Press, 1993
43.
Golden CJ: Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Chicago, Stoelting, 1978
44.
Diller L, Ben-Yishay Y, Gertman LJ, Goodkin R, Gordon W, Weinberg J: Studies in Cognition and Rehabilitation in Hemiplegia: Rehabilitation Monograph 50. New York, New York University Medical Center Institute of Rehabilitation Medicine, 1974
45.
Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment, 2nd ed. New York, Oxford University Press, 1983
46.
Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1993
47.
McNicol D: A Primer of Signal Detection Theory. London, Allen & Unwin, 1972
48.
Hamilton M: Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol 1967; 6:278-296
49.
Alexopoulos GS, Meyers BS, Young RC, Kakuma T, Silbersweig D, Charlson M: Clinically defined vascular depression. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:562-565
50.
Krishnan KR: Neuroanatomic substrates of depression in the elderly. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1993; 6:39-58
51.
Corwin J, Peselow E, Feenan K, Rotrosen J, Fieve R: Disorders of decision in affective disease. Biol Psychiatry 1990; 27:813-833
52.
Lemelin S, Baruch P, Vincent A, Laplante L, Everett J, Vincent P: Attention disturbance in clinical depression: deficient distractor inhibition or processing resource deficit? J Nerv Ment Dis 1996; 184:114-121
53.
Buchsbaum MS, Lee S, Haier R, Wu JC, Green M, Tang SW: Effects of amoxapine and imipramine on evoked potentials in the Continuous Performance Test in patients with affective disorder. Neuropsychobiol 1988; 20:15-22
54.
Cornblatt BA, Lenzenweger MF, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L: The Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs Version, II: contrasting attentional profiles in schizophrenic and depressed patients. Psychiatry Res 1989; 29:65-85
55.
Plude DJ, Hoyer WJ, Lazar J: Age, response complexity, and target consistency in visual search. Exp Aging Res 1982; 8:99-102
56.
Uttl B, Graf P, Cosentino S: Exacting assessments: do older adults fatigue more quickly? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2000; 22:496-507
57.
West R: In defense of the frontal lobe hypothesis of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6:727-729
58.
Shimamura AP: Memory and frontal lobe function, in The Cognitive Neurosciences. Edited by Gazzaniga M. Cambridge, Mass, MIT Press, 1995, pp 803-813
59.
Fuster JM: The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, 3rd ed. Philadelphia, Lippincott-Raven, 1997
60.
Liotti M, Woldorff MG, Perez R, Mayberg HS: An ERP study of temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:30-42
61.
Liotti M, Mayberg HS: The role of functional neuroimaging in the neuropsychology of depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2001; 23:121-136
62.
Petit-Taboue MC, Landeau B, Desson JI, Desgranges B, Baron JC: Effects of healthy aging on the regional cerebral metabolic rate of glucose assessed with statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage 1998; 7:176-184
63.
Haug H, Eggers R: Morphometry of the human cortex cerebri and corpus striatum during aging. Neurobiol Aging 1991; 12:336-338

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1119 - 1126
PubMed: 12091189

History

Published online: 1 July 2002
Published in print: July 2002

Authors

Details

Kathryn A. Lockwood, Ph.D.
George S. Alexopoulos, M.D.
Wilfred G. van Gorp, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share