Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 September 2003

Effects of Environmental Enrichment at Ages 3–5 Years on Schizotypal Personality and Antisocial Behavior at Ages 17 and 23 Years

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Methods to prevent two major mental disorders, schizophrenia and conduct disorder, have been elusive. This study assessed the effects of an early nutritional, educational, and physical exercise enrichment program on adult outcome for schizotypal personality, conduct disorder, and criminal behavior. METHOD: Eighty-three children were assigned to an experimental enrichment program from ages 3 to 5 years and matched on temperament, nutritional, cognitive, autonomic, and demographic variables with 355 children who experienced usual community conditions (control group). Both self-report and objective measures of schizotypal personality and antisocial behavior were obtained when the subjects were ages 17 and 23 years. RESULTS: Subjects who participated in the enrichment program at ages 3–5 years had lower scores for schizotypal personality and antisocial behavior at age 17 years and for criminal behavior at age 23 years, compared with the control subjects. The beneficial effects of the intervention were greater for children who showed signs of malnutrition at age 3 years, particularly with respect to outcomes for schizotypy at ages 17 and 23 and for antisocial behavior at age 17. CONCLUSIONS: The results are consistent with an increasing body of knowledge that implicates an enriched, stimulating environment in beneficial psychological and behavioral outcomes. These findings have potential implications for the prevention of schizophrenia and criminal behavior.
Although schizophrenia is a major mental illness that can be treated, primary prevention of this disorder has been elusive. Nevertheless, a population-based strategy for preventing this debilitating mental disorder may not so much involve addressing the end-state of the disease process as it would involve developing primary prevention programs that tackle the milder, prodromal, prepsychotic features of the disorder, which have much in common with the features of DSM-IV schizotypal personality disorder (e.g., unusual perceptual experiences, no close friends, cognitive disorganization) (1). If primary prevention programs could be developed to attenuate individual differences in schizotypal features in the general population, such efforts could provide the basis for developing new programs aimed at preventing schizophrenia (2). To our knowledge, no such primary prevention programs have been developed for either schizophrenia or schizotypal personality (3).
International epidemiological studies have shown higher rates of criminal offending in schizophrenia patients, compared with the general population (48). A number of the neurobiological and psychological risk factors for schizophrenia are also risk factors for crime (5, 6). Consequently, primary prevention efforts for schizotypy and schizophrenia may also reduce rates of conduct disorder and criminal behavior. Violence and crime are increasingly being viewed as major public health problems with origins in the early years of life (911), and prevention of these behaviors is one of the most important and pressing issues in society today. Yet, very few primary prevention studies have examined interventions in the early years of life to tackle the root causes of conduct disorder. Because animal research has clearly shown that environmental enrichment benefits both brain structure and function (1214), and because brain imaging and neuropsychological studies have provided evidence of brain dysfunction in both schizophrenia and antisocial behavior (1522), it is conceivable that early environmental enrichment could reduce both schizotypal personality and antisocial behavior.
Nutrition may be an important component of early environmental enrichment. Nutritional deficits have been implicated both in antisocial personality and in schizophrenia and schizoid personality (2325), and animal experimental studies have demonstrated the importance of nutrition in cognitive and brain functioning (26, 27). One of most successful early interventions for antisocial behavior to date involved home visits by nurses to mothers in which nutritional advice was a major component (28). Consequently, an enrichment program that provides better early nutrition could reduce schizotypal personality and antisocial behavior, especially in subjects with poor nutritional status at the start of the prevention program.
The goal of this study was to assess the effects of early environmental enrichment, including a nutritional component, from ages 3 to 5 years on later antisocial behavior and schizotypal personality (29). The study also assessed whether any beneficial effects were maximized in children with poor nutrition. Because this program had already been shown to produce long-term changes in psychophysiological functioning 8 years after the intervention (30), the potential moderating effect of psychophysiological functioning on the effects of the enrichment program was also assessed.

Method

Subjects

Subjects were drawn from a larger population of 1,795 children from Mauritius and are described in full elsewhere (30, 31). All children born in 1969–1970 in two towns were recruited into the study at age 3 years. This total sample consisted of both male (51.4%) and female (48.6%) children. Ethnic distribution was as follows: Indian, 68.7%; Creoles, 25.7%; and others, 5.6%. After complete description of the study to the subjects, verbal informed consent was obtained from the mothers of the participants for the study phase when subjects were ages 3–5 years, and written informed consent was obtained from the subjects for later study phases. Early research activities were conducted according to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki (32), while research activities in later years were conducted by using the principles outlined in the Belmont Report (33). Institutional review board approval for the later research phases was obtained from the University of Southern California.
Stratified random sampling was conducted by using random-number tables to select 100 children from the larger population of 1,795 to enter the enrichment program. The remaining 1,695 children experienced the normal community (control) condition. The 100 children entering the enrichment program were grouped according to measures of autonomic functioning to assess the moderating effects of autonomic activity on the enrichment (see reference 31 and a subsequent paragraph in the Method section for further details). The control subjects were selected from the remaining 1,695 children to match the enrichment group on autonomic functioning as well as other key age-3 variables that could influence outcome. Of the original 100 children in the enrichment group, 83 provided follow-up outcome data. This enrichment group was matched on 10 age-3 variables (ethnicity, gender, age, nutritional status, cognitive ability, temperament, autonomic reactivity, parental social class, social adversity, and mother’s age at birth) with 355 control subjects for whom outcome data were available. The characteristics of the two groups are summarized in Table 1. No significant between-group differences were found (all p values >0.35), which shows close matching between groups.
At age 17, a total of 438 participants (including 83 participants in the enrichment group) had complete data on behavior problems. Among these 438 participants, there were no differences between the enrichment group and the control group in age (t=0.41, df=436, p=0.68), ethnicity (χ2=0.06, df=1, p=0.44), or gender (χ2=0.61, df=1, p=0.35). At age 17, a total of 339 participants (including 74 in the enrichment group) had complete data on schizotypy. Among these 339 participants, there were no differences between the enrichment group and the control group in age (t=0.54, df=337, p=0.58), ethnicity (χ2=0.33, df=1, p=0.57), or gender (χ2=0.56, df=1, p=0.46).
At ages 23–26, a total of 359 participants (including 75 in the enrichment group) had complete data on schizotypy. Among these 359 participants, the enrichment group did not differ from the control group in age (t=0.46, df=357, p=0.65), ethnicity (χ2=0.52, df=1, p=0.47), or gender (χ2=1.90, df=1, p=0.16). Among the 438 participants (83 in the enrichment group) who had complete data from court records on criminal activity, there were no differences between the enrichment group and the control group in age (t=0.41, df=436, p=0.68), ethnicity (χ2=0.06, df=1, p=0.44), or gender (χ2=0.61, df=1, p=0.35). Among the 360 participants (72 in the enrichment group) with complete data on self-reported crime, the enrichment group and the control group did not differ significantly in age (t=0.43, df=358, p=0.67), ethnicity (χ2=0.42, df=1, p=0.51), or gender (χ2=2.07, df=1, p=0.15). The participants included in these analyses (N=438) did not differ significantly from the remainder of the population (N=1,357) in age (t=0.50, df=1,793, p=0.62), ethnicity (χ2=0.19, df=1, p=0.66), or gender (χ2=1.48, df=1, p=0.22).

Enrichment and Community Control Conditions

The enrichment and control conditions have been fully described elsewhere (30). Briefly, the enrichment program started at age 3 years, lasted 2 years, and consisted of three key elements: nutrition, education, and physical exercise. The enrichment program was conducted in two specially constructed nursery schools that had a teacher-pupil ratio of 1:5.5. A special feature of the nurseries was the emphasis on nutrition, health care, basic hygiene, and exercise (31). Staff had training in physical health (nutrition, hygiene, anatomy and physiology, childhood disorders), physical activities (gymnastics and rhythm activities, outdoor activities, physiotherapy), and education (multimodal stimulation, use of toys, art, handicrafts, drama, music). A structured nutrition program provided the children with milk, fruit juice, a hot meal of fish or chicken or mutton, and a salad each day. A program of physical exercise consisting of gymnastics lessons, structured outdoor games, and free play was incorporated into the afternoon session of each day. The enrichment program also included walking, field trips, instruction in basic hygiene skills, and medical inspections (every 2 months) (30). An average of 2.5 hours of physical activities occurred each day. Educational activities focused on verbal skills, visuospatial coordination, conceptual skills, memory, and sensation and perception.
The children in the control condition underwent the traditional Mauritius community experience, which consisted of attendance at “petite ecoles” (30). These schools had a traditional grade-school curriculum and a teacher-pupil ratio of 1:30. No lunch, milk, or structured exercise was provided. For lunch, children typically ate bread only, rice and bread, or rice only.

Autonomic Functioning at Age 3 Years

Full details of the autonomic measures are given elsewhere (30, 31). Briefly, subjects were presented with a series of auditory tone stimuli while skin conductance was recorded from bipolar leads on the medial phalanges of the first and second fingers of the left hand by using a constant voltage system. Beckman miniature Ag/AgCl type (4-mm diameter) electrodes were filled with 0.5% KCl in 2% agar-agar as the electrolyte. Responses >0.05 microsiemens occurring within a 1–3-second poststimulus window were scored. Nonresponding was defined as giving three consecutive nonresponses to the first three tone stimuli.

Demographic and Psychosocial Measures at Age 3 Years

When the children included in the study were age 3 years, a social worker visited the homes of the subjects’ parents and collected demographic and psychosocial information (35). A total adversity score (35, 36) was created by adding 1 point for each of the following nine variables: father uneducated (no schooling; present for 30.0% of the subjects), mother uneducated (no schooling; present for 29.4% of the subjects), father with semiskilled or unskilled occupation (occupational status of 3 or less on an 8-point occupational scale: 0=unemployed, 4=factory worker, cook, 8=academic, head of large business; present for 55.5%), teenage mother (age 19 or younger when child was born; present for 14.2%), single parent (present for 2.1%), separation from parents (orphaned or raised by substitute mother; present for 0.9%), large family size (sibling order fifth or higher by age 3 years; present for 30.0%), poor health of mother (coded 1 on a 3-point scale: above average=3, average=2, below average=1; present for 3.3%), and overcrowded home (five or more family members/house room; present for 28.8%). Scores ranged from 0 to 7 (mean=1.93, SD=1.39). Subjects with two or more indicators were defined as having psychosocial adversity.

Malnutrition at Age 3 Years

Seven indicators of malnutrition were assessed in an examination conducted by a pediatrician when the child was age 3 years. The indicators were stunting (reduced height for age), based on the standing height of the child and expressed as observed height as a percentage of expected height for the child’s age, with expected height based on Mauritian norms from the entire birth cohort; wasting (reduced weight for height), based on the weight of the child in light clothes without shoes and expressed as the ratio of observed weight to expected weight based on Mauritian norms of expected weight for height; hemoglobin levels, assessed from blood samples taken from the child; angular stomatitis (cracking in the lips and corners of the mouth), which predominantly reflects riboflavin deficiency (vitamin B2) but also reflects niacin deficiency (37); hair dyspigmentation (e.g., red hair, a sign of kwashiorkor), reflecting protein malnutrition and zinc and copper deficiency (38); sparse, thin hair, a sign of protein-energy, zinc, and iron deficiency (39) as well as a sign of general malnutrition; and hair that is easy to pull out, reflecting protein-energy deficiency (40). A confirmatory factor analysis (conducted with EQS structural equation modeling software [Multivariate Software, Encino, Calif.]) of these seven variables established a one-factor (malnutrition) model with significant fit indices of 0.99 for the goodness of fit index, 0.98 for the adjusted goodness of fit index, and 0.05 for the root-mean-square error of approximation. A median split on standardized factor scores was used to designate subjects as either malnourished or nonmalnourished.

Behavioral Outcome Measures

Age 17 years

Schizotypal personality was assessed by using a self-report questionnaire that yielded two indicators of schizotypal personality: positive schizotypal personality (including unusual perceptual experiences and magical thinking) (41, 42) and cognitive disorganization (43). Details on the reliability and validity of this measure have been reported elsewhere (41, 42).
Ratings of behavior problems were obtained by using the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist (44). This instrument assesses six dimensions of behavior problems: conduct disorder, motor excess, attention problems, psychotic behavior, anxiety-withdrawal, and socialized aggression. Details on the reliability and validity of this instrument have been provided elsewhere (44).

Age 23 years

Schizotypal personality was assessed by using the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (45). Full details on the reliability and validity of this instrument have been provided elsewhere (36, 45). This instrument yields a total score and scores for three subfactors measuring cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal deficits, and disorganized features (46).
Subjects were interviewed about their histories of criminal offending by using a structured interview consisting of an adult extension of the self-report delinquency measure from the National Youth Survey (47). This instrument queries respondents about the perpetration of 41 criminal offenses that cover a wide range of property, drug, violent, and white-collar crimes over the past 5 years. The 33.6% of subjects who admitted to a criminal offense were categorized as offenders. The internal reliability (coefficient alpha) for the scale was 0.84. In addition, court records were searched for registrations of offenses that included property, drug, violence, and serious driving offenses (e.g., driving while drunk, dangerous driving) but excluded petty offenses (e.g., parking fines, lack of vehicle registration). Subjects with such a court record were categorized as criminal.

Results

Outcome at Age 17 Years

Behavior problems

The six dimensions of the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist were entered into a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). There was a significant main effect for group (F=3.04, df=6, 429, p=0.006). Univariate F tests indicated a significant group effect of enrichment on conduct disorder (F=8.80, df=1, 437, p=0.003, d=0.44), psychotic behavior (F=4.41, df=1, 437, p<0.04, d=0.31), and motor excess (F=6.44, df=1, 437, p<0.02, d=0.38) (Table 2). In addition, there were significant enrichment-by-nutrition interactions for conduct disorder (F=5.25, df=1, 434, p<0.03) and motor excess (F=4.51, df=1, 434, p<0.04). These interactions are illustrated in Figure 1 (left and center panels). The malnourished subjects in the enrichment group were more likely to have lower scores for conduct disorder (t=2.91, df=164, p=0.004, d=0.63) and motor excess (t=2.80, df=164, p=0.006, d=0.61), compared to the malnourished subjects in the control group. No other MANOVA main effects or interactions were significant (p>0.31).

Schizotypal personality

A MANOVA for the two self-report schizotypy measures produced a significant main effect for enrichment (F=4.23, df=2, 336, p<0.02). Univariate F tests indicated significant effects of enrichment for both positive schizotypal personality (F=4.46, df=2, 337, p<0.04, d=0.29) and disorganization (F=6.56, df=2, 335, p<0.02, d=0.34). In addition, there was a significant enrichment-by-nutrition interaction for cognitive disorganization (F=4.24, df=2, 335, p=0.04). This interaction is illustrated in Figure 1 (right panel), which shows that the malnourished subjects in the enrichment group were more likely to have lower cognitive disorganization scores, compared with the malnourished subjects in the control group (t=3.04, df=124, p=0.003, d=0.71). No other MANOVA main effects or interactions were significant (p>0.12).

Outcome at Age 23 Years

Schizotypal personality

There was no main effect of enrichment on total schizotypal personality scores (p=0.34) and no significant main effect of nutrition (F=3.69, df=1, 355, p=0.056), but the interaction of enrichment and nutritional status was significant (F=4.08, df=1, 355, p<0.05). This interaction is illustrated in Figure 2 (left panel), which shows that the malnourished subjects in the enrichment group had lower total Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire scores, compared with the malnourished subjects in the control group (t=2.07, df=135, p<0.05, d=0.42). No other main effects or interactions were significant (p>0.31).
An analysis of subfactors of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire indicated a significant enrichment-by-nutrition interaction for interpersonal deficits (F=5.80, df=1, 355, p<0.02), but no significant effects for disorganization (F=3.76, df=1, 355, p=0.053) or cognitive-perceptual deficits (p>0.27). The interaction is illustrated in Figure 2 (right panel), which shows that the malnourished subjects in the enrichment group had lower interpersonal deficit scores than the malnourished subjects in the control group (t=2.95, df=135, p=0.005, d=0.56).

Criminal offending

Rates of criminal offending at ages 23–26 years are given in Table 2. There was a significant relationship between group and self-reported offending (χ2=4.03, df=1, p<0.05, d=0.26), with a significantly lower rate of self-reported offending in the enrichment group (23.6%) than in the control group (36.1%). The rate of offending based on court records was lower in the enrichment group (3.6%) than in the control group (9.9%), but the difference was not statistically significant (χ2=3.31, df=1, p=0.07, d=0.22). There was no evidence that nutritional status interacted with the enrichment with respect to criminal outcome (χ2=0.73, df=1, p=0.39).

Moderating Effects of Autonomic Functioning and Psychosocial Adversity

To test the moderating effect of autonomic status on the relationship between group (enrichment versus control group) and behavior problems, autonomic status at age 3 years (nonresponders versus responders) was entered as a factor in MANOVAs analyzing outcomes at ages 17 and 23 years. Autonomic status did not interact with group for either schizotypal personality or behavior problems at any age (p>0.56). Similar analyses showed that the presence or absence of psychosocial adversity did not interact with group status (enrichment versus control group) in any analysis (p>0.21).

Discussion

Nutritional, educational, and physical exercise enrichment at ages 3–5 years was associated with lower scores for schizotypal personality and antisocial behavior 14–20 years later, compared with usual community conditions. The beneficial effects associated with the intervention tended to be greater for children who were malnourished at age 3, particularly with respect to outcomes for schizotypal personality at ages 17 and 23 and conduct disorder at age 17. The fact that the prevention program was associated with effects for two different forms of psychopathology that were assessed by using two different types of outcome measures (self-report and objective measures) at two follow-up points extending over 20 years indicates the robustness of the effects. The results cannot be attributed to pre-enrichment group differences in temperament, cognitive ability, nutritional status, autonomic reactivity, or demographic variables. To our knowledge, this program constituted the first attempt at primary prevention of schizotypal personality. The findings are consistent with an increasing body of knowledge that implicates an enriched, stimulating environment in beneficial psychological and behavioral outcomes (4850) and have potential implications for the prevention of crime and schizotypal personality, particularly in populations at risk for malnutrition.
One limitation of this study was the inability to determine which component(s) of the intervention (nutrition, exercise, or education) accounted for the later beneficial effects. Nevertheless, children who were malnourished at age 3 years were significantly more likely to benefit from the program at age 17 years and at age 23 years, compared with children who were not malnourished at age 3 years. Scores at age 17 years for subjects who were malnourished at age 3 years were 53.0% lower for conduct disorder, 41% lower for motor excess, 43.2% lower for psychotic behavior, and 32.4% lower for cognitive disorganization, compared with the scores at age 17 years for subjects who were not malnourished. Scores at age 23 years for subjects who were malnourished at age 3 years were 23.0% lower for total schizotypy and 31.6% lower for interpersonal deficits, compared with the scores at age 23 for subjects who were not malnourished at age 3 years. Nutritional deficits have been linked to both schizophrenia and schizoid personality disorder (24, 25, 51) and also to antisocial behavior (23). In particular, a recent double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized trial has shown the effectiveness of nutritional supplements in reducing antisocial and violent behavior in prisoners (52). It is conceivable that aspects of the nutritional intervention such as fish supplementation could benefit brain function because omega-3 fatty acids have been reported by some to have beneficial effects on both schizophrenia (5355) and externalizing behavior problems (5659). In contrast to nutrition, psychophysiological status was not a significant moderator. Taken together with the findings of other successful early prevention programs that have manipulated nutrition (28), the current findings suggest, but do not prove, that the nutrition component of the enrichment program may account for some of the benefits.
Alternative hypotheses must also be considered. For example, the prevention program included 2.5 hours each day devoted to physical exercise and outdoor play, and it is conceivable that exercise by itself could account for a significant proportion of the observed effects. Exercise in animals is known to increase mRNA in the hippocampus and to have other beneficial effects on brain structure and function (60). Furthermore, the effect of environmental enrichment in producing neurogenesis in mice has been attributed to the sole effects of running, independent of other components of enrichment such as maze training (13). As such, physical exercise could both benefit brain functioning and protect individuals from developing schizotypal personality and antisocial behavior, both of which have been linked to structural and functional brain deficits (1518, 61). Another hypothesis that needs to be considered is that the increased social interaction with positive, educated preschool teachers in the experimental enrichment program may in part account for the beneficial effects of the prevention program. Alternatively, it may be unreasonable to focus on any single component of the intervention. Instead, the multimodal nature of the prevention program, which incorporated social and cognitive components along with nutrition and exercise components, may have facilitated interactions between these components that critically affected later development. Follow-up studies that both replicate the multimodal enrichment strategy as well as tease apart the individual components could usefully address this issue.
One prior study has shown that educational enrichment has long-term effects in reducing antisocial and criminal behavior (28). Thus, it is possible that the educational component of the prevention program could have accounted for the long-term reduction in criminal offending. In the previous study, the participants in the educational enrichment program were children from poor homes, and it has been argued that enrichment may particularly benefit children affected by social adversity (62). No support was found for this argument in the current study, as social adversity did not moderate the effects of the enrichment program. Instead, children affected specifically by malnutrition were more likely to benefit from the enrichment. Consequently, nutritional status in particular, rather than social adversity in general, may be a particular factor to consider in future prevention programs intended to address schizotypy and antisocial behavior.
The fact that the prevention program was associated with lower levels of schizotypal personality in late adolescence and in early adulthood in the malnourished group may help intervention efforts aimed at preventing or delaying the onset of schizophrenia (2, 3). Schizotypal personality is a disorder that shares with schizophrenia a range of neurocognitive, neurophysiological, genetic, psychophysiological, and symptom characteristics (21, 63) and has much in common with the prodromal, prepsychotic stage of schizophrenia. If features of schizotypal personality can be attenuated by early environmental enrichment, it is possible that the development of the prodromal features of schizophrenia may also be reduced, in turn delaying or preventing the onset of schizophrenia itself (3, 64). The fact that one of the strongest effect sizes in this study was obtained for interpersonal deficits in the malnourished group at age 23 years (d=0.56) is of particular relevance, as these negative schizotypal traits feature strongly in the schizophrenia prodrome, as described in DSM-IV (65, 66). Despite these findings, it must be reemphasized that this study showed only lower levels of schizotypal personality in a nonselected group, and it remains to be seen whether such prevention efforts can achieve results of clinical significance in relation to schizophrenia itself, as have been achieved with secondary prevention efforts (65).
Although the prevention program was associated with a significantly lower conduct disorder score for subjects at age 17 (d=0.44), we caution that the effect size for subjects at age 23 years was smaller (d=0.26 for self-reported criminal offending; d=0.22 for criminal offending according to court records). In this study, the level of criminal offending at age 23 years according to self-reports was significantly lower in the enrichment group than in the control group (p<0.05). The level of criminal offending by age 23 years according to court records was 63.6% lower for the enrichment group than for the control group (which equals or exceeds effects found in short-term intervention studies [67]), but the difference failed to reach statistical significance (p=0.07). On the other hand, base rates for criminal offending according to court records were lower than those for self-reported offending, and the possibility that floor effects precluded statistical significance must also be considered. A recent review of prevention programs for crime showed that even the very few model prevention programs that have shown success rarely if ever show effects beyond the teenage years (67). The large majority of prevention programs do not start early in life, when the pathway to antisocial behavior is believed to begin (68, 69). On the other hand, it may be questioned why any effect 20 years later was associated with a prevention program that lasted only 2 years early in life. Although stronger long-term effects may be expected of a longer environmental enrichment program, it has been found that early withdrawal from an environmental enrichment doubles the number of new hippocampal cells in mice, compared to more sustained enrichment (70). Thus, some long-term beneficial effect of the 2-year enrichment is not inconceivable. Future studies may assess whether a booster enrichment session later in childhood leads to stronger effect sizes for criminal behavior in adulthood.
In conclusion, these initial findings suggest that early environmental enrichment is associated with lower levels of antisocial behavior and schizotypal personality in adulthood and that benefits are greatest for children with poor nutrition. We caution that findings from a country that was still developing when the prevention program was conducted cannot be directly extrapolated to more developed countries such as the United States. Nevertheless, the findings may be particularly relevant to poor rural areas of the United States, such as the Mississippi delta region, and also to U.S. inner cites, where rates of both malnutrition and behavioral problems in children are relatively high (71, 72).
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
Figure 1. Scores on Conduct Disorder, Motor Excess, and Cognitive Disorganization Measures at Age 17 Years for Subjects Who Participated in an Environmental Enrichment Program at Ages 3–5 Years and Control Subjects Who Experienced Usual Community Conditions, by Nutritional Status at Ages 3–5 Yearsa
aSubjects in the enrichment program received nutritional, educational, and physical exercise interventions in nursery schools constructed especially for the program (30).
bMeasured with the Revised Behavior Problems Checklist (44).
cMeasured with a self-report schizotypy questionnaire (41, 42).
Figure 2. Total Schizotypy and Interpersonal Deficits Scores at Age 23 Years for Subjects Who Participated in an Environmental Enrichment Program at Ages 3–5 Years and Control Subjects Who Experienced Usual Community Conditions, by Nutritional Status at Ages 3–5 Yearsa
aSubjects in the enrichment program received nutritional, educational, and physical exercise interventions in nursery schools constructed especially for the program (30).
bMeasured with the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (45).

Footnote

Received May 14, 2002; revisions received Nov. 25, 2002, and Jan. 22, 2003; accepted Feb. 6, 2003. From the Department of Psychology, University of Southern California; the Mauritius Child Health Project, Quatre Bornes, Mauritius; the School of Nursing, University of California, Los Angeles; and the Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York, U.K. Address reprint requests to Dr. Raine, Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-1061; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by NIMH Independent Scientist Award MH-01114-01, NIMH grant MH-46435-02, and a Scholar-in-Residence award from the Borchard Foundation to Dr. Raine; grants from the United Kingdom Medical Research Council, the Leverhulme Trust, and the Mental Health Foundation of the United Kingdom to Dr. Venables; NIMH Research Scientist Award MH-00619-08 to Dr. Mednick; and a grant from the Ministry of Health of the Government of Mauritius. The authors thank Marie-Clare Calambay, Meena Calinghen, Athene Chiriaca, Cyril Dalais, and Devi Jaganathen for help in data collection and scoring.

References

1.
Cornblatt B, Lencz T, Obuchowski M: The schizophrenia prodrome: treatment and high-risk perspectives. Schizophr Res 2002; 54:177–186
2.
Tsuang MT, Stone WS, Faraone SV: Towards the prevention of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:349–356
3.
McGorry PD: The nature of schizophrenia: signposts to prevention. Aust NZ J Psychiatry 2000; 34:S14-S21
4.
Volavka J, Laska E, Baker S, Meisner M, Czobor P, Krivelevich I: History of violent behaviour and schizophrenia in different cultures: analyses based on the WHO Study on Determinants of Outcome of Severe Mental Disorders. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 171:9–14
5.
Volavka J: The neurobiology of violence: an update. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999; 11:307–314
6.
Raine A: The Psychopathology of Crime: Criminal Behavior as a Clinical Disorder. San Diego, Calif, Academic Press, 1993
7.
Raesaenen P, Tiihonen J, Isohanni M, Rantakallio P, Lehtonen J, Moring J: Schizophrenia, alcohol abuse, and violent behavior: a 26-year followup study of an unselected birth cohort. Schizophr Bull 1998; 24:437–441
8.
Fishbein DH: The Science, Treatment, and Prevention of Antisocial Behavior: Applications to the Criminal Justice System. New York, Civic Research Institute, 1999
9.
Fishbein D: Biobehavioral Perspectives in Criminology. Belmont, Calif, Wadsworth, 2001
10.
Mercy JA, Hammond WR: Combining action and analysis to prevent homicide: a public health perspective, in Homicide: A Sourcebook of Social Research. Edited by Smith MD, Zahn MA. Thousand Oaks, Calif, Sage Publications, 1999, pp 297–310
11.
Guerra NG, Tolan PH, Hammond WR: Prevention and treatment of adolescent violence, in Reason to Hope: A Psychosocial Perspective on Violence and Youth. Edited by Eron LD, Gentry JH, Schlegel P. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1994, pp 383–403
12.
Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Gage FH: More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature 1997; 386:493–495
13.
van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH: Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:266–270
14.
Pham TM, Soderstrom S, Winblad B, Mohammed AH: Effects of environmental enrichment on cognitive function and hippocampal NGF in the non-handled rats. Behav Brain Res 1999; 103:63–70
15.
Andreasen NC: A unitary model of schizophrenia: Bleuler’s “fragmented phrene” as schizencephaly. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:781–787
16.
Buchsbaum MS, Hazlett EA: Positron emission tomography studies of abnormal glucose metabolism in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1998; 24:343–364
17.
Raine A, Buchsbaum M, LaCasse L: Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission tomography. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 42:495–508
18.
Raine A, Lencz T, Bihrle S, LaCasse L, Colletti P: Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:119–127
19.
Buchsbaum MS, Nenadic I, Hazlett EA, Spiegel-Cohen J, Fleischman MB, Akhavan A, Silverman JM, Siever LJ: Differential metabolic rates in prefrontal and temporal Brodmann areas in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res 2002; 54:141–150
20.
Gur RE, Cowell PE, Latshaw A, Turetsky BI, Grossman RI, Arnold SE: Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:761–768
21.
Siever LJ, Koenigsberg HW, Harvey P, Mitropoulou V, Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, Goodman M, Buchsbaum M: Cognitive and brain function in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res 2002; 54:157–167
22.
Cannon TD, van Erp TGM, Glahn DC: Differential metabolic rates in prefrontal and temporal Brodmann areas in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res 2002; 54:141–150
23.
Fishbein DH, Pease SE: Diet, nutrition, and aggression. J Offender Rehabilitation 1994; 21:117–144
24.
Hoek HW, Susser E, Buck KA, Lumey LH, Lin SP, Gorman JM: Schizoid personality disorder after prenatal exposure to famine. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:1637–1639
25.
Tsuang M: Schizophrenia: genes and environment. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:210–220
26.
Lim S, Suzuki H: Changes in maze behavior of mice occur after sufficient accumulation of docosahexaenoic acid in brain. J Nutr 2001; 131:319–324
27.
Salvati S, Attorri L, Avellino C, Di Biase A, Sanchez M: Diet, lipids and brain development. Dev Neurosci 2000; 22:481–487
28.
Olds D, Henderson CR Jr, Cole R, Eckenrode J, Kitzman H, Luckey D, Pettitt L, Sidora K, Morris P, Powers J: Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on children’s criminal and antisocial behavior: 15-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 1998; 280:1238–1244
29.
Schulsinger F, Mednick SA, Venables PH, Raman AC, Bell B: Early detection and prevention of mental illness: the Mauritius project: a preliminary report. Neuropsychobiology 1975; 1:166–179
30.
Raine A, Venables PH, Dalais C, Mellingen K, Reynolds C, Mednick SA: Early educational and health enrichment at age 3–5 years is associated with increased autonomic and central nervous system arousal and orienting at age 11 years: evidence from the Mauritius Child Health Project. Psychophysiology 2001; 38:254–266
31.
Venables PH: Psychophysiology and psychometrics. Psychophysiology 1978; 15:302–315
32.
World Medical Association: Declaration of Helsinki: Recommendations Guiding Doctors in Clinical Research Adopted by the 18th World Medical Assembly. London, World Medical Association, 1964
33.
National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research: The Belmont Report: Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research. Washington, DC, US Department of Health and Human Services, 1979
34.
Boehm AE: Boehm Test of Basic Concepts, Preschool Edition. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1986
35.
Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA: Low resting heart rate at age 3 years predisposes to aggression at age 11 years: evidence from the Mauritius Child Health Project. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:1457–1464
36.
Reynolds CA, Raine A, Mellingen K, Venables PH, Mednick SA: Three-factor model of schizotypal personality: invariance across culture, gender, religious affiliation, family adversity, and psychopathology. Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:603–618
37.
Wong DL, Hess CS: Clinical Manual of Pediatric Nursing. St Louis, Mosby, 2000
38.
Subotzky EF, Heese HD, Sive AA, Dempster WS, Sacks R, Malan H: Plasma zinc, copper, selenium, ferritin and whole blood manganese concentrations in children with kwashiorkor in the acute stage and during refeeding. Ann Trop Paediatr 1992; 12:13–22
39.
Friedman M, Orraca-Tetteh R: Hair as an index of protein malnutrition. Adv Exp Med Biol 1978; 105:131–154
40.
Spencer LV, Callen JP: Hair loss in systemic disease. Dermatol Clin 1987; 5:565–570
41.
Venables PH: The measurement of schizotypy in Mauritius. Pers Individ Dif 1990; 11:965–971
42.
Venables PH, Wilkins S, Mitchell DA, Raine A, Bailes K: A scale for the measurement of schizotypy. Pers Individ Dif 1990; 11:481–495
43.
Venables PH, Bailes K: The structure of schizotypy, its relation to subdiagnoses of schizophrenia and to sex and age. Br J Clin Psychol 1994; 33:277–294
44.
Quay HC, Peterson DR: Manual for the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist. Coral Gables, Fla, University of Miami, Department of Psychology, 1987
45.
Raine A: The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull 1991; 17:555–564
46.
Raine A, Reynolds C, Lencz T, Scerbo A, Triphon N, Kim D: Cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized features of schizotypal personality. Schizophr Bull 1994; 20:191–201
47.
Elliott DS, Ageton S, Huizinga D, Knowles B, Canter R: The Prevalence and Incidence of Delinquent Behavior:1976–1980. National Youth Survey, Report 26. Boulder, Colo, Behavior Research Institute, 1983
48.
Farrington DP: Criminal career research: lessons for crime prevention. Studies on Crime & Crime Prevention 1992; 1:7–29
49.
Gorey KM: Early childhood education: a meta-analytic affirmation of the short- and long-term benefits of educational opportunity. School Psychol Q 2001; 16:9–30
50.
Schweinhart LJ, Weikart DP: The High/Scope Preschool Curriculum Comparison Study through age 23. Early Child Res Q 1997; 12:117–143
51.
Dalman C, Allebeck P, Cullberg J, Grunewald C, Koster M: Obstetric complications and the risk of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study of a national birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:234–240
52.
Gesch CB, Hamond SM, Hampson SE, Eves A, Crowder MJ: Influence of supplementary vitamins, minerals and essential fatty acids on the antisocial behavior of young adult prisoners. Br J Psychiatry 2002; 181:22–28
53.
Mahadik SP, Evans D, Lal H: Oxidative stress and role of antioxidant and omega-3 essential fatty acid supplementation in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2001; 25:463–493
54.
Post RM: Comparative pharmacology of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1999; 39:153–158
55.
Warner R, Laugharne J, Peet M, Brown L, Rogers N: Retinal function as a marker for cell membrane omega-3 fatty acid depletion in schizophrenia: a pilot study. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:1138–1142
56.
Mellor JE, Laugharne JDE, Peet M: Schizophrenic symptoms and dietary intake of n-3 fatty acids (letter). Schizophr Res 1995; 18:85–86
57.
Stevens LJ, Zentall SS, Abate ML, Kuczek T: Omega-3 fatty acids in boys with behavior, learning, and health problems. Physiol Behav 1996; 59:915–920
58.
Arnold LE, Kleykamp D, Votolato N, Gibson RA: Potential link between dietary intake of fatty acids and behavior: pilot exploration of serum lipids in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 1994; 4:171–182
59.
Corrigan F, Gray R, Strathdee A, Skinner R, Van Rhijn A, Horrobin D: Fatty acid analysis of blood from violent offenders. J Forensic Psychiatry 1994; 5:83–92
60.
Gomez-Pinilla F, Dao L, So V: Physical exercise induces FGF-2 and its mRNA in the hippocampus. Brain Res 1997; 764:1–8
61.
Buchsbaum MS, Trestman RL, Hazlett E, Siegel BVJ, Schaefer CH, Luu-Hsia C: Regional cerebral blood flow during the Wisconsin Card Sort Test in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res 1997; 27:21–28
62.
Weikart DP: Changing early childhood development through educational intervention. Prev Med 1998; 27:233–237
63.
Raine A, Lencz T, Mednick SA (eds): Schizotypal Personality Disorder. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1995
64.
Tsuang MT, Stone WS, Faraone SV: Genes, environment and schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 2001; 178:18–24
65.
McGorry PD, Edwards J: The feasibility and effectiveness of early intervention in psychotic disorders: the Australian experience. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1998; 13(suppl 1):S47-S52
66.
Tsuang MT, Stone WS, Tarbox SI, Faraone SV: An integration of schizophrenia with schizotypy: identification of schizotaxia and implications for research on treatment and prevention. Schizophr Res 2002; 54:169–175
67.
Youth Violence: A Report of the Surgeon General. Washington, DC, US Department of Health and Human Services, 2001
68.
Moffitt TE: Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev 1993; 100:674–701
69.
Tremblay RE: The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: what have we learned in the past century? Int J Behav Dev 2000; 24:129–141
70.
Kempermann G, Gage FH: Experience-dependent regulation of adult hippocampal neurogenesis: effects of long-term stimulation and stimulus withdrawal. Hippocampus 1999; 9:321–332
71.
Murphy JM, Wehler CA, Pagano ME, Little M, Kleinman RE, Jellinek MS: Relationship between hunger and psychosocial functioning in low-income American children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998; 37:163–170
72.
Smith J, Lensing S, Horton JA, Lovejoy J, Zaghloul S, Forrester I, McGee BB, Bogle ML: Prevalence of self-reported nutrition-related health problems in the Lower Mississippi Delta. Am J Public Health 1999; 89:1418–1421

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1627 - 1635
PubMed: 12944338

History

Published online: 1 September 2003
Published in print: September 2003

Authors

Details

Kjetil Mellingen, M.A.
Sarnoff A. Mednick, D.Med.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share