Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 February 2009

An Odor-Specific Threshold Deficit Implicates Abnormal Intracellular Cyclic AMP Signaling in Schizophrenia

Abstract

Objective: Although olfactory deficits are common in schizophrenia, their underlying pathophysiology remains unknown. Recent evidence has suggested that cAMP signaling may be disrupted in schizophrenia. Since cAMP mediates signal transduction in olfactory receptor neurons, this could contribute to the etiology of observed olfactory deficits. This study was designed to test this hypothesis by determining odor detection threshold sensitivities to two odorants that differ in their relative activations of this intracellular cAMP signaling cascade. Method: Thirty schizophrenia patients, 25 healthy comparison subjects, and 19 unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients were studied. Odor detection threshold sensitivities were measured for the two odorants citralva and lyral. Although both have fruity/floral scents, citralva strongly activates adenylyl cyclase to increase cAMP levels, while lyral is a very weak activator of adenylyl cyclase. Results: There was a significant group-by-odor interaction. Both schizophrenia patients and unaffected first-degree relatives were impaired in their ability to detect lyral versus citralva. Comparison subjects were equally sensitive to both odorants. This selective deficit could not be explained by differences in age, sex, smoking, clinical symptom profile, or medication use. Conclusions: This study establishes the presence of an odor-specific hyposmia that may denote a disruption of cAMP-mediated signal transduction in schizophrenia. The presence of a parallel deficit in the patients’ unaffected first-degree relatives suggests that this deficit is genetically mediated. Although additional physiological studies are needed to confirm the underlying mechanism, these results offer strong inferential support for the hypothesis that cAMP signaling is dysregulated in schizophrenia.
Olfactory deficits are recognized as a subtle but common element of schizophrenia pathology (1) . Behavioral studies of olfaction have focused primarily on impairments of odor identification, with less attention being paid to deficits in other olfactory domains, such as detection threshold sensitivity, discrimination, or memory. Although odor identification deficits appear to be sensitive and specific indicators of both the vulnerability to (24) and progressive course of (5) schizophrenia illness, they are relatively uninformative regarding underlying pathophysiological mechanisms. The ability to correctly identify odors requires more than just an intact olfactory system. It requires a previously learned inventory of recognized odors, the ability to retrieve this inventory, and the ability to associate a retrieved odor memory with a linguistic label. This is a complex, multifaceted, higher-order cognitive operation, and an abnormality at any level of processing can disrupt task performance.
Studies of olfactory acuity or detection threshold sensitivity (i.e., ability to detect a weak odor concentration) focus more directly on olfactory sensory processing isolated from other cognitive aspects of odor identification. Acuity deficits are thought to reflect impairments in the peripheral, as opposed to central, olfactory system. Lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex (6, 7) or the dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus (8), for example, may produce odor identification deficits but leave olfactory acuity intact. Schizophrenia patients exhibit impairments in olfactory acuity, although there have been fewer replications and greater cross-laboratory variability than has been seen for odor identification (1) .
One limitation of studies of olfactory acuity is that they almost exclusively assess odor detection threshold sensitivity using the single odorant phenyl ethyl alcohol, which is the de facto standard. This is important because odor-specific hyposmias are well documented and are thought to reflect genetic and/or physiological mechanism vulnerabilities associated with specific odor perception (9) . While detection threshold sensitivity decrements in schizophrenia have typically been interpreted as denoting a generalized acuity deficit, possibly reflecting structural abnormalities in the peripheral olfactory system (e.g., reference 10 ), there is little direct evidence to support this hypothesis. Only two schizophrenia studies have employed another traditional odorant, n -butanol, rather than phenyl ethyl alcohol. One of these observed a patient deficit (11), while the other did not (12) . Similarly, only two studies have examined odor acuity using more than one odorant in the same sample. Both of these reported a differential deficit—i.e., schizophrenia patients had impaired odor detection thresholds to one, but not the other, of the two odorants used in the study. In one case, the odorants differed in the dimension of pleasantness, and patients had reduced sensitivity to the unpleasant odor (13) . In the other, patients had impaired ability to detect a chemical linked to the unpleasant body odor associated with schizophrenia (12), an abnormality possibly related to olfactory habituation.
These two studies provide the first evidence to suggest that schizophrenia patients may not, in fact, have a generalized hyposmia that is independent of the specific odorant being tested. However, the subjective dimensions along which the odorants varied—pleasant/unpleasant and body odor/nonbody odor—are not especially informative concerning the neuropathological mechanisms that might underlie these odor-specific deficits. In the current study, we examined odor acuity in schizophrenia patients using odorants that differed in a more precise physiological manner—the ability to increase intracellular cAMP. Typically, when an odorant binds to a receptor on the membrane of an olfactory receptor neuron in the nasal epithelium, it induces g-protein mediated activation of adenylyl cyclase, increasing intracellular cAMP. This, in turn, causes cyclic nucleotide-gated ion channels to open, resulting in neuronal depolarization. There is a strong positive correlation (r=0.89), across a wide range of odorants, between odor-stimulated adenylyl cyclase activity and the summed electrical response of the olfactory epithelium (electro-olfactogram or EOG). This association confirms the essential role of cAMP in mediating olfactory signal transduction (14) . However, the magnitudes of these two correlated responses are highly variable across odorants. While some elicit robust activity, others elicit only minimal responses (14, 15) . Of importance, there is also a strong positive correlation between these in vitro adenylyl cyclase and EOG voltage responses and the subjective perception of odor intensity in vivo. When healthy subjects rated the intensities of different odorants, all presented at the same suprathreshold concentration, their subjective ratings correlated significantly with both adenylyl cyclase activity and EOG amplitude (16) . These in vivo, in vitro associations establish the validity of these odor-specific in vitro responses for translational human studies of olfactory processing.
We selected this dimension of adenylyl cyclase activation as our “dimension-of-interest” because a growing body of evidence suggests that intracellular cAMP signaling pathways may be dysregulated in schizophrenia (1720) . Perhaps more compelling is the fact that DISC1, the schizophrenia susceptibility gene on chromosome 1q42, sequesters phosphodiesterase 4B (PDE4B), the enzyme that inactivates cAMP, in an inactive state and releases it when cAMP levels increase (19) . Although this evidence remains indirect, it clearly supports the hypothesis that dysregulated cAMP signaling may contribute to schizophrenia pathology (21) . We theorized that if this is indeed the case, then it might be observable in differential acuity deficits for odorants that differentially activated the intracellular cAMP signaling cascade. We further hypothesized that given the genetic associations that appear to underlie cAMP dysregulation (1921), similar differential acuity deficits might be observed in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia probands.

Method

Subjects

The sample included 30 patients with DSM-IV-diagnosed schizophrenia, 25 healthy comparison subjects, and 19 unaffected first-degree relatives of patients. All healthy comparison subjects were unrelated individuals. Unaffected family members included five parents, 13 siblings, and one adult child of a schizophrenia patient, from 17 independent families. Two siblings came from one family, as did one sibling and one parent. The patient sample included five affected probands linked to seven of these relatives, plus 25 unrelated individuals. All patients were stable outpatients at the time of testing.
All subjects received the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies and the Family Interview for Genetic Studies. Patients were excluded for any concurrent axis I diagnosis other than schizophrenia. Healthy comparison subjects were excluded for any history of an axis I diagnosis, axis II cluster A personality disorder, or family history of axis I psychotic disorder in a first-degree relative. Family members were excluded for any axis I psychotic disorder or prodromal psychotic symptoms but not for a previous nonpsychotic axis I disorder if it resolved more than 1 year ago and was not associated with any current pharmacotherapy. Nor were they excluded for an axis II cluster A diagnosis. Among the 19 family members, two had histories of a prior depressive episode; none met criteria for schizoid or schizotypal personality disorder. Subjects were excluded for any history of neurological disorder, head trauma with loss of consciousness, lifetime history of substance dependence, substance abuse within the preceding 6 months, or any medical condition that might affect cerebral functioning. Subjects were also excluded for any obvious cranio-facial trauma or abnormality, including septal deviation, and any acute respiratory condition, cold, or allergy. Written informed consent was obtained after all procedures were fully explained, in compliance with guidelines established by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board.
Demographic characteristics of the groups are presented in Table 1 . There were no significant differences in sex distribution (χ 2 =12.67, df=2, p=0.26) or mean age (F=2.18, df=2, 71, p=0.12). There were also no overall differences in smoking status, whether assessed in terms of the number of active smokers within each group (χ 2 =2.61, df=2, p=0.27) or the mean number of packs smoked per day (F=2.75, df=2, 71, p=0.07). In paired contrasts, however, patients smoked significantly more than healthy comparison subjects (F=4.97, df=1, 53, p=0.03), while family members did not differ from either group.
Descriptive clinical information and standardized rating scale measures for patients are presented in Table 2 . The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (22), the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (23), and the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (24) were obtained at the time of testing. Ratings were completed by trained investigators with an interrater reliability of >0.90. BPRS items were summed to form an index of overall symptom severity. Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptom global ratings for five negative symptom subscales (affective flattening, alogia, anhedonia, avolition, attention) and Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms global ratings for four positive symptom subscales (hallucinations, delusions, bizarre behavior, formal thought disorder) assessed specific dimensions of psychotic symptom profiles. Overall, these ratings suggested a mild level of both positive and negative symptoms. Twenty-six of 30 patients were taking antipsychotic medications. Medication dosages were calibrated across subjects as chlorpromazine equivalents (25) .

Selection of Odorants

The two odorants employed in the study were citralva (3,7-dimethyl-2,6-octadienenitrile) and lyral (4-[4-hydroxy-4-methylpentyl]-3-cyclohexene-1-carboxyaldehyde). These are both volatile organic compounds used as fragrance additives in many commercial products. Citralva has a molecular weight of 149.2 and a density of 0.859–0.870 g/ml. Lyral has a molecular weight of 210.3 and a density of 0.985–0.993 g/ml. They are qualitatively similar in having pleasant floral/fruity aromas. However, they differ markedly in the extent to which they activate the intracellular signaling cascade mediating chemosensory signal transduction in olfactory receptor neurons. The response elicited by citralva is quite strong, while the response elicited by lyral is relatively weak. In a study of odor-stimulated adenylyl cyclase activity, using ex vivo chemosensory cilia preparations, citralva ranked fourth in the magnitude of its adenylyl cyclase response, while lyral ranked 42nd out of 44 odorants (15) . Similarly, in a study of the odor-stimulated EOG response, citralva ranked fifth out of 36 odorants, whereas lyral ranked 21st in the magnitude of this electrical response (14) .

Experimental Procedures

Standardized psychophysical assessments of the subjects’ ability to detect citralva and lyral were conducted after they refrained from smoking for approximately 1 hour. Separate tests of right and left nostril sensitivity were conducted for each odorant, while the other nostril was occluded with durapore tape (3M Corporation, Minneapolis, Minn.) (26) . For a given subject, testing began with a specific odor-nostril combination followed by presentation of the same odorant to the opposite nostril. The second odorant was then presented to each nostril in the same order as the first. Test order was counterbalanced across subjects for both initial odorant and nostril.
A single staircase, forced-choice task was used to estimate detection threshold sensitivity. In this task, the subjects were asked to smell two vials, one containing pure mineral oil and the other containing an active odorant diluted in mineral oil, and identify which vial “smells stronger.” Concentrations of both lyral and citralva ranged from 10 –1 M (strongest) to 10 –9 M (weakest), in 0.5 log step dilution increments. The test began at the 10 –5 M step, and odor concentration was increased in full-molar steps until correct detection (i.e., active odorant identified as stronger) occurred on five consecutive trials at a given concentration. Odor concentration was then increased or decreased in half-molar increments, depending upon performance on two trials at each concentration step (i.e., odor concentration was decreased after 2 correct trials and increased after an incorrect trial). The geometric mean of the last four staircase reversal points (out of seven) was taken as the estimate of odor detection threshold sensitivity (i.e., the weakest odor concentration that could be reliably identified as stronger than mineral oil). The average number of odorant exposures to achieve a stable measure of detection threshold sensitivity was 12.6 (SD=2.9); this did not vary across diagnostic groups, odorants, or nostrils. Total test time was approximately 30 minutes.

Statistical Analysis

Since the patient and family member groups were not strictly independent samples, group differences were assessed using the generalized linear latent and mixed models (GLLAMM) algorithm implemented in Stata 9.0 (Statacorp, College Station, Tex.), with subject and family as hierarchically nested random-effects factors. This effectively accounted for any shared variance between individual members of the same family. Group (patient/healthy comparison subject/relative), gender, nostril (left/right), odorant (citralva/lyral), and age were fixed-effects predictors. Significance levels of individual model parameters were assessed using the Wald test statistic with chi-square distribution. Significant group differences and interactions were parsed by post hoc computation of appropriate linear combinations of model coefficients, along with associated z-statistics. Post hoc contrasts were subject to Bonferroni-corrected p values of p<0.05 to minimize type I errors.

Results

The initial analysis revealed a significant effect of group (χ 2 =7.31, df=2, p=0.02), odor (χ 2 =44.85, df=1, p<0.0001), and group-by-odor interaction (χ 2 =12.22, df=2, p=0.002). There were no significant effects of sex (χ 2 =0.90, df=1, p=0.34), age (χ 2 =1.95, df=1, p=0.16), or nostril (χ 2 =0.04, df=1, p=0.85). When smoking status was included as a moderating factor, either as an indicator variable (smoker/nonsmoker) or as a continuous measure (packs/day), it was not significantly related to odor acuity (indicator: χ 2 =0.38, df=1, p=0.54; continuous: χ 2 =0.79, df=1, p=0.37), and it did not alter any of the other significant effects. Mean odor detection threshold sensitivities for each group, odor, and nostril are presented in Figure 1 .
Figure 1. Odor Threshold Detection Sensitivity to Citralva and Lyral a
a Mean (SD) odor detection threshold sensitivities for each group. Higher y-axis values indicate better odor detection threshold sensitivity.
b Within-group difference in detection threshold sensitivity to citralva versus lyral (p<0.001).
To determine if schizophrenia patients exhibited differential acuities to citralva and lyral as a result of the odors’ differential effects on cAMP signaling, we parsed the group-by-odor interaction by contrasting threshold detection sensitivities to the two odors separately within each group. Both schizophrenia patients (χ 2 =20.31, df=1, p<0.001) and unaffected family members (χ 2 =31.91, df=1, p<0.001) had differential acuity impairments to lyral relative to citralva. Healthy comparison subjects showed no difference in their relative sensitivity to the two odorants (χ 2 =1.54, df=1, p=0.64). Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for this citralva-lyral difference were 0.86 for patients and 1.47 for family members, indicating a large effect for both groups. The relatively larger effect size within the family group reflected their greater sensitivity to citralva but a comparable impairment in their ability to detect lyral. As illustrated in Figure 2, a small number of patients were markedly impaired in their ability to detect citralva as well as lyral.
Figure 2. Threshold Detection Sensitivity (negative log molar concentration) a
a Scatterplot of individual subject responses to lyral and citralva. Dotted line represents equal sensitivity to both odorants. Individuals below the line have worse threshold detection sensitivity for lyral. Ellipses indicate 68% (1 SD) prediction areas for each group.
We considered whether there were relationships, among patients, between odor acuity measures and measures of clinical symptom profiles or treatment status. Odor threshold sensitivities were dependent measures in a composite general linear model (GLM) with the following independent measures: Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms and Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms subscale ratings, antipsychotic medication dosage, age of illness onset, duration of illness, and total BPRS score. None of these clinical measures were related to any of the olfactory measures, even with an uncorrected value of p<0.05 for an exploratory analysis.

Discussion

This study clearly demonstrates that schizophrenia patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives exhibit odor-specific acuity deficits for lyral but not citralva. These cannot be explained by nonspecific factors such as age, sex, smoking history, acute symptom profile, or antipsychotic medication. Given that these two odorants are distinguished by their abilities to activate the cAMP intracellular signaling cascade-mediating olfactory neuron depolarization, this odor-specific hyposmia may indicate a genetically regulated disruption of cAMP-mediated signal transduction. As noted in the introduction, there is growing evidence to suggest that cAMP is dysregulated in schizophrenia (1820) . While not dispositive of such a mechanistic disturbance, the results of this study are clearly consistent with this hypothesis. Of importance, this hypothesis is not inconsistent with other models of schizophrenia pathophysiology. Altered cAMP levels can be secondary to either glutamatergic (27) or dopaminergic (28) dysregulation.
There are, however, alternative explanations that must be considered. The original studies demonstrating differential cAMP responses to citralva versus lyral (14, 15) examined aggregate multicellular responses. A more recent study of single cell responses confirmed that citralva induces a transmembrane current that is approximately twice the magnitude of that induced by lyral (29) . This study also indicated, though, that fewer olfactory neurons respond to lyral than to citralva. It is possible, therefore, that reduced sensitivity to lyral simply reflects the fact that fewer neurons are being stimulated. However, there are several reasons to think that this is not the case. First, if threshold detection sensitivity reflects the total number of neurons capable of responding to a given odorant, then we should see a lyral-citralva acuity difference in healthy subjects, as well as in schizophrenia patients and family members. These individuals, though, had identical thresholds for the two odorants. Conversely, if schizophrenia patients have a simple loss of olfactory receptor neurons that impairs their odor detection sensitivity, then they should exhibit impairments to both lyral and citralva. There is no reason to expect selective loss of neurons that respond to lyral but not citralva, since receptor-specific neurons are distributed diffusely throughout the olfactory epithelium and any given odorant is capable of binding to several different receptors. Finally, the observation that more neurons respond to citralva than to lyral was made using suprathreshold concentrations of odorants. By definition, odor detection threshold represents the odor concentration required to stimulate the minimum number of receptors to enable an odor to be detected. It is likely that this minimum required number of activated neurons is relatively the same across odorants and is not dependent upon the total number of neurons capable of responding to higher odor concentrations. The total number of response-capable neurons is more likely to affect the perceived intensity of a suprathreshold odorant, which was found to be greater for citralva even in healthy subjects (16) .
There is also evidence to suggest that intracellular pathways other than cAMP may contribute to mammalian olfactory signal transduction. The presence of alternate signaling pathways has been well-established in nonmammalian species (e.g., reference 30 ) and also proposed for human olfaction (31) . Only recently, though, has it been clearly demonstrated that cAMP-independent pathways are capable of mediating odor perception in the mammalian olfactory system (32) . We cannot rule out the possibility that lyral and citralva are mediated through different intracellular signaling mechanisms, with one being impaired and one remaining intact. However, we think this is unlikely for the following reasons. While disruption of cAMP-mediated signaling does not result in complete anosmia, cAMP remains the principal mediator of olfactory receptor neuron responses. Decreased cyclic nucleotide levels in the nasal mucosa are associated with impaired odor threshold sensitivities (33) . In the absence of an intact cAMP pathway, only a subset of previously detectable odors remains detectable, and the magnitudes of the responses evoked by these odorants are reduced exponentially. Of most importance, there is no detectable response to lyral following disruption of the cAMP pathway (32) . So, despite establishing the functional importance of alternate signal transduction pathways, these data actually support the hypothesis that impaired ability to detect lyral reflects dysregulation of the cAMP pathway.
If this olfactory deficit does denote a disturbance in cAMP signaling, why would the deficit be expressed only in response to an odorant associated with lower levels of cAMP activity? The explanation is likely related to the fact that fluctuations in basal concentrations of cAMP cause olfactory signal transduction to be intrinsically noisy (34) . Since the magnitude of the lyral-evoked response is relatively small (29), it is more difficult to distinguish this response from baseline fluctuations than it is to distinguish the response evoked by citralva. If this odor-evoked response is further attenuated in schizophrenia patients relative to healthy individuals, this would further reduce the signal-to-noise ratio, resulting in an undetectable near-baseline response. The response to citralva might be similarly attenuated but, being intrinsically larger, still remain strong enough to produce reliable signal detection.
We must emphasize, though, that studies implicating cAMP dysregulation in schizophrenia have not clearly established the direction of this functional dysregulation. Although we have discussed it in terms of an attenuated response, the data remain inconsistent. For example, given the ability of DISC1 to sequester PDE4B in an inactive state, a genetic abnormality in DISC1 should decrease PDE4B sequestration which, in turn, would decrease cAMP. However, a genetic abnormality in PDE4B itself, which disrupts PDE4B-mediated inactivation of cAMP and results in increased cAMP levels, has also been associated with schizophrenia (18) . The complexity of the situation is illustrated by the animal model in which mice expressing a constitutively active isoform of Gas exhibit prepulse inhibition deficits. As predicted from its ability to stimulate adenylyl cyclase, this active isoform increased cAMP levels in the striatum. However, because of compensatory increases in PDE4B, cAMP levels in the forebrain were decreased (20) . Whether cAMP levels are increased or decreased in the olfactory system remains unknown, although the results of this study are most consistent with a functional decrement.
These findings clearly need to be replicated using other odorants associated with high versus low adenylyl cyclase activity to confirm that this is, in fact, the “dimension-of-interest” underlying this differential deficit. Additional physiological, rather than behavioral, studies are also needed. We would anticipate, based on these behavioral findings, that the EOG depolarization response recorded from the nasal epithelium would be similarly disrupted following stimulation with lyral but not citralva. Studies using olfactory epithelial biopsy material obtained from schizophrenia patients could also be employed to clarify the relationships between behavioral deficits, electrophysiological responses, and biochemical pathways in the olfactory receptor neurons. Nevertheless, the results of this study offer strong inferential support for the hypothesis that cAMP signaling is dysregulated in schizophrenia.

Footnotes

Presented in part at the 62nd annual meeting of the Society of Biological Psychiatry; San Diego, Calif.; May 17–19, 2007. Received July 30, 2007; revisions received July 29 and Aug. 28, 2008; accepted Sept. 3, 2008 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071210). From the Neuropsychiatry Division, Department of Psychiatry; and the Smell and Taste Center, Department of Otorhinolaryngology: Head and Neck Surgery, University of Pennsylvania. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Bruce Turetsky, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, 10th Floor, Gates Building, 3400 Spruce St., Philadelphia, PA 19104; [email protected].
Dr. Turetsky reports unrelated research grant support from Astra-Zeneca. Dr. Moberg reports no competing interests.
Supported by NIMH grants MH59852 (to Dr. Turetsky) and MH63381 (to Dr. Moberg).

References

1.
Moberg PJ, Agrin R, Gur RE, Gur RC, Turetsky BI, Doty RL: Olfactory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 21:325–340
2.
Kopala LC, Good KP, Morrison K, Bassett AS, Alda M, Honer WG: Impaired olfactory identification in relatives of patients with familial schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1286–1290
3.
Brewer WJ, Wood SJ, McGorry PD, Francey SM, Phillips LJ, Yung AR, Anderson V, Copolov DL, Singh B, Velakoulis D, Pantelis C: Impairment of olfactory identification ability in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1790–1794
4.
Roalf DR, Turetsky BI, Owzar K, Balderston CC, Johnson SC, Brensinger CM, Gur RE, Siegel SJ, Moberg PJ: Unirhinal olfactory function in schizophrenia patients and first-degree relatives. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006; 18:389–396
5.
Moberg PJ, Doty RL, Turetsky BI, Arnold SE, Mahr RN, Gur RC, Bilker W, Gur RE: Olfactory identification deficits in schizophrenia: correlation with duration of illness. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1016–1018
6.
Potter H, Butters N: An assessment of olfactory deficits in patients with damage to prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 1980; 18:621–628
7.
Jones-Gotman M, Zatorre RJ: Olfactory identification deficits in patients with focal cerebral excision. Neuropsychologia 1988; 26:387–400
8.
Adams R, Victor M: Principles of Neurology. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1985
9.
Bartoshuk LM, Beauchamp GK: Chemical senses. Annu Rev Psychol 1994; 45:419–449
10.
Turetsky BI, Moberg PJ, Yousem DM, Doty RL, Arnold SE, Gur RE: Reduced olfactory bulb volume in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:828–830
11.
Rupp CI, Fleischhacker WW, Kemmler G, Kremser C, Bilder RM, Mechtcheriakov S, Szeszko PR, Walch T, Scholtz AW, Klimbacher M, Maier C, Albrecht G, Lechner-Schoner T, Felber S, Hinterhuber H: Olfactory functions and volumetric measures of orbitofrontal and limbic regions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005; 74:149–161
12.
Brewer WJ, Wood SJ, Pantelis C, Berger GE, Copolov DL, McGorry PD: Olfactory sensitivity through the course of psychosis: relationships to olfactory identification, symptomatology and the schizophrenia odour. Psychiatry Res 2007; 149:97–104
13.
Rupp CI, Fleischhacker WW, Kemmler G, Oberbauer H, Scholtz AW, Wanko C, Hinterhuber H: Various bilateral olfactory deficits in male patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2005; 31:155–165
14.
Lowe G, Nakamura T, Gold GH: Adenylate cyclase mediates olfactory transduction for a wide variety of odorants. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1989; 86:5641–5645
15.
Sklar PB, Anholt RRH, Snyder SH: The odorant-sensitive adenylate cyclase of olfactory receptor cells. J Biol Chem 1986; 261:15538–15543
16.
Doty RL, Kreiss DS, Frye RE: Human odor intensity perception: correlation with frog epithelial adenylate cyclase activity and transepithelial voltage response. Brain Res 1990; 527:130–134
17.
Natsukari N, Kulaga H, Baker I, Wyatt RJ, Masserano JM: Increased cyclic AMP response to forskolin in Epstein-Barr virus-transformed human B-lymphocytes derived from schizophrenics. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997; 130:235–241
18.
Kelly MP, Isiegas C, Cheung YF, Tokarczyk J, Yang X, Esposito MF, Rapoport DA, Fabian SA, Siegel SJ, Wand G, Houslay MD, Kanes SJ, Abel T: Constitutive activation of galphas within forebrain neurons causes deficits in sensorimotor gating because of PKA-dependent decreases in cAMP. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007; 32:577–588
19.
Millar JK, Pickard BS, Mackie S, James R, Christie S, Buchanan SR, Malloy MP, Chubb JE, Huston E, Baillie GS, Thomson PA, Hill EV, Brandon NJ, Rain J-C, Camargo LM, Whiting PJ, Houslay MD, Blackwood DHR, Muir WJ, Porteous DJ: DISC and PDE4B are interacting genetic factors in schizophrenia that regulate cAMP signaling. Science 2005; 310:1187–1191
20.
Minoretti P, Politi P, Coen E, Di Vito C, Bertona M, Bianchi M, Emanuele E: The T393C polymorphism of the GNAS1 gene is associated with deficit schizophrenia in an Italian population sample. Neurosci Lett 2006; 397:159–163
21.
Sawa A, Snyder SH: Two genes link two distinct psychoses. Science 2005; 310:1128–1129
22.
Overall JR, Gorham DR: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. J Oper Psychiatry 1980; 11:48–64
23.
Andreasen NC: The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1983
24.
Andreasen NC: The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984
25.
Woods SW: Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics. J Clin Psychiatry 2003; 64:663–667
26.
Bromley SM, Doty RL: Odor recognition memory is better under bilateral than unilateral test conditions. Cortex 1995; 31:25–40
27.
Chetkovich DM, Sweatt JD: NMDA receptor activation increases cyclic AMP in area CA1 of the hippocAMPus via calcium/calmodulin stimulation of adenylyl cyclase. J Neurochem 1993; 61:1933–1942
28.
Neves SR, Ram PT, Iyengar R: G protein pathways. Science 2002; 296:1636–1639
29.
Takeuchi H, Imanaka Y, Hirono J, Kurahashi T: Cross-adaptation between olfactory responses induced by two subgroups of odorant molecules. J Gen Physiol 2003; 122:255–264
30.
Restrepo D, Miyamoto T, Bryant BP, Teeter JH: Odor stimuli trigger influx of calcium into olfactory neurons of the channel catfish. Science 1990; 249:1166–1168
31.
Rawson NE, Gomez G, Brand JG, Lowry LD, Pribitkin EA, Restrepo D: Selectivity and response characteristics of human olfactory neurons. J Neurophysiol 1997; 77:1606–1613
32.
Lin W, Arellano J, Slotnick B, Restrepo D: Odors detected by mice deficient in cyclic nucleotide-gated channel subunit A2 stimulate the main olfactory system. J Neurosci 2004; 24:3703–3710
33.
Henkin RI, Velicu I: cAMP and cGMP in nasal mucus related to severity of smell loss in patients with smell dysfunction. Clin Invest Med 2008; 31:E78–E84
34.
Lowe G, Gold GH: Olfactory transduction is intrinsically noisy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995; 92:7864–7868

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 226 - 233
PubMed: 19074977

History

Published online: 1 February 2009
Published in print: February, 2009

Authors

Details

Bruce I. Turetsky, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share