Skip to main content
Full access
New Research
Published Online: 1 September 2012

Cognitive Dysfunction and Anxious-Impulsive Personality Traits Are Endophenotypes for Drug Dependence

Abstract

Objective

Not everyone who takes drugs becomes addicted, but the likelihood of developing drug addiction is greater in people with a family history of drug or alcohol dependence. Relatively little is known about how genetic risk mediates the development of drug dependence. By comparing the phenotypic profile of individuals with and without a family history of addiction, the authors sought to clarify the extent to which cognitive dysfunction and personality traits are shared by family members—and therefore likely to have predated drug dependence—and which aspects are specific to drug-dependent individuals.

Method

The authors assessed cognitive function and personality traits associated with drug dependence in stimulant-dependent individuals (N=50), their biological siblings without a history of drug dependence (N=50), and unrelated healthy volunteers (N=50).

Results

Cognitive function was significantly impaired in the stimulant-dependent individuals across a range of domains. Deficits in executive function and response control were identified in both the stimulant-dependent individuals and in their non-drug-dependent siblings. Drug-dependent individuals and their siblings also exhibited elevated anxious-impulsive personality traits relative to healthy comparison volunteers.

Conclusions

Deficits in executive function and response regulation as well as anxious-impulsive personality traits may represent endophenotypes associated with the risk of developing cocaine or amphetamine dependence. The identification of addiction endophenotypes may be useful in facilitating the rational development of therapeutic and preventive strategies.
Drug dependence is a major contemporary public health issue (1) involving harmful effects not only for the affected individuals but also for their families, communities, and society as a whole (2). Community-based family studies indicate that relatives of drug-dependent individuals have eight times the risk of developing drug abuse disorders themselves (3), but the role of preexisting vulnerability in addiction remains poorly understood. Twin and adoption studies have produced compelling evidence for genetic influences in the development of drug dependence (47) but have also shown that the high prevalence of drug dependence within families may be caused by environmental factors or interactions between environmental and genetic influences (8, 9).
The concept of endophenotypes offers a useful strategy for elucidating the underlying factors that render an individual vulnerable to drug dependence. Endophenotypes have been defined as quantitative traits that are intermediate between the predisposing genes (genotype) and the clinical symptoms (phenotype) of a complex disorder. According to the criteria outlined by Gottesman and Gould (10), endophenotypes are quantifiable traits that are 1) associated with the disorder; 2) genetically determined; 3) largely state independent (i.e., they should manifest in periods of health and during acute illness); 4) cosegregate with the disorder within families; and 5) overrepresented in nonaffected family members relative to the general population. The identification of familial vulnerability markers for drug dependence may provide a scientific basis for the development of effective preventive and therapeutic strategies for individuals at risk.
In our study, we used an endophenotype strategy to identify putative cognitive, emotional, and personality markers of stimulant dependence vulnerability. Addiction is largely subserved by brain circuits that have also been associated with executive control (11, 12), specifically response inhibition (13, 14), mental planning (15, 16), working memory (1719), and attentional control (2022). These neural networks were possibly dysfunctional before the stimulant abuse, rendering individuals vulnerable for addiction. Alternatively, cognitive function may also deteriorate in response to chronic stimulant abuse, a proposal supported by preclinical studies showing that prolonged stimulant abuse leads to deficits in neuropsychological function (23, 24). Moreover, interactions between both predisposing and neurotoxic effects on cognitive function are equally conceivable.
Stimulant dependence often co-occurs with anxiety and affective disorders (25, 26), and affect-related psychopathologies are also common in families with substance abuse problems (27). Depressive symptoms have been associated with both intoxication and withdrawal from various addictive drugs (28, 29), possibly resulting from drug-induced changes in monoamine neurotransmission (29, 30). Growing evidence also points to the role of personality traits, attitudes, and demographics in elevating the risk of drug dependence (31, 32). Both impulsivity (33, 34) and sensation-seeking (35) traits have been prospectively associated with a higher risk of drug abuse and addiction. Psychological constructs such as self-efficacy (36, 37), which describes the confidence in being able to achieve a certain outcome or the perceived control that a person has over life events (3840), may mediate the risk of problem drug abuse.
We compared the cognitive and emotional functioning and the personality traits of stimulant-dependent individuals, their non-drug-dependent siblings, and healthy unrelated comparison volunteers. The rationale for the focus on stimulants is based on relatively high heritability estimates for stimulant dependence (41). The recruitment of sibling pairs was pragmatically advantageous, but the design has limited power for making inferences about the etiology of personality and cognitive abnormalities evident in both the dependent individuals and their siblings when compared with a twin design. We can say that these represent abnormal familial vulnerability factors (i.e., they are shared by members of the same family), but we cannot discriminate genetic from common environmental causes for their emergence. Nevertheless, the distinction this affords between predisposing factors and drug-induced changes is still valuable. We hypothesized that deficits in executive control function and impulsive personality traits represent an endophenotype for drug dependence and would therefore be identified not only in the drug-dependent individuals but also in their siblings.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited from treatment services and media advertisements; recruitment figures and participation details are shown in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article. Sibling pairs were included if three conditions were met: 1) they had the same biological parents; 2) one sibling satisfied DSM-IV-TR criteria for cocaine or amphetamine dependence; and 3) the other sibling had no personal history of substance dependence (other than nicotine). All participants who enrolled in the study underwent a screening that included semistructured interviews to ascertain history of drug use, physical health (including signs of acute intoxication and withdrawal), mental health as assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders (42), and demographic characteristics.
Exclusionary criteria, applied for all groups, were a lifetime history of a psychotic disorder or a history of a neurological illness, neurodevelopmental disorder, or traumatic head injury. Healthy comparison volunteers did not have a personal or family history of drug or alcohol dependence. Participants had to be 18–55 years old and able to read and write in English. The study protocol was approved by the Cambridge Research Ethics Committee and written informed consent was obtained from all volunteers before study enrollment.
All drug-dependent individuals (N=50) met DSM-IV-TR criteria for stimulant dependence (94% for cocaine and 6% for amphetamines). The majority (76%) were currently enrolled in a drug treatment program, and all but five were actively using stimulant drugs as verified by urine screens. On average, they had been using stimulants for 16 years (SD=6.4) starting at age 16 (SD=2.8), and they last used them 3 days before testing began (SD=4.7; range=0.5–28 days); without the five drug users who were abstinent, stimulants were last used 1.7 days before testing (SD=2.0), which is consistent with the 72-hour detection window for stimulants. On the Obsessive-Compulsive Drug Use Scale (43), participants indicated moderate levels of stimulant-related compulsivity (mean score=23.7; SD=9.5). Half of the drug-dependent sample also met DSM-IV-TR criteria for dependence on other substances (54% for opiates, 24% for alcohol, and 8% for cannabis). The drug-taking experiences and the use of alcohol were notably low in the sibling and comparison groups, as reflected by low scores on the Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST-20; 44) and the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; 45); these results are summarized in Table 1.
TABLE 1. Demographic, Clinical, and Familial Data for Drug-Dependent Individuals, Their Siblings, and Unrelated Healthy Comparison Volunteersa
         Post Hoc Comparisons
CharacteristicHealthy Volunteers (N=50)Unaffected Siblings (N=50)Drug-Dependent Individuals (N=50)Group ComparisonHealthy Volunteers and SiblingsHealthy Volunteers and Drug-DependentDrug-Dependent and Siblings
Demographics (present)N%N%N%Fp   
Male gender32642550448816.8<0.0010.1570.005<0.001
Unemployed2412242958Fisher’s p<0.0010.012<0.0010.002
Marital status      Fisher’s p0.0120.5400.0020.037
 Single275432643774     
 Married1836132648     
 Divorced510510918     
 MeanSDMeanSDMeanSDFp   
Age (years)32.88.932.98.434.37.20.30.775   
Verbal intelligence (National Adult Reading Test score)112.38.2109.29.1110.67.51.50.230   
Education (years)12.61.912.32.311.61.72.80.066   
Full-time employment (years)7.27.16.56.15.05.12.20.115   
Household size1.60.51.50.62.11.37.60.0051.0000.0170.009
Average household income (£ / month)2,5162,2082,0031,5792,2695,8330.260.806   
Childhood (past)N%N%N%χ2p   
Parent with drug/alcohol addiction002550255037.5<0.001<0.001<0.001 
Parents divorced10202448244811.00.0040.0030.0031.000
Brought up together until age 10  44884692Fisher’s p0.741   
Brought up together until age 15  37744590Fisher’s p0.040   
Birth rank      Fisher’s p0.378   
 Only240000     
 Youngest193820401530     
 Middle153019381632     
 Eldest142811221938     
 MeanSDMeanSDMeanSDFp   
Household size4.61.05.41.75.51.74.90.0050.0300.0221.000
Age when parents divorced (years)10.64.97.74.88.24.91.50.227   
Childhood Trauma Questionnaire score           
 Emotional abuse6.83.09.95.011.46.212.2<0.0010.010<0.0010.194
 Physical abuse5.82.97.63.59.45.68.5<0.0010.098<0.0010.169
 Sexual abuse5.11.06.84.57.85.46.40.0020.2110.0010.267
Drug exposure
 Alcohol consumption (AUDIT score)3.32.33.84.511.111.114.0<0.0011.000<0.001<0.001
 Drug-taking experiences (DAST-20 score)0.00.00.51.1  10.50.002   
 Cigarette consumption (number per day)5.55.85.07.815.712.512.3<0.0011.0000.026<0.001
 Age at onset of tobacco smoking (years)b15.82.814.52.012.53.315.00.0080.163<0.0010.002
 Age of onset cannabis smoking (years)c17.64.017.74.214.53.211.0<0.0011.0000.033<0.001
a
Analyses are controlled for gender, p<0.05; the Bonferroni correction is applied for the post hoc comparisons. AUDIT=Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; DAST-20=Drug Abuse Screening Test.
b
23 healthy volunteers, 46 siblings, and 49 drug-dependent individuals reported tobacco smoking.
c
10 healthy volunteers, 35 siblings, and 50 drug-dependent individuals reported cannabis smoking.
Details about the comprehensive assessment of cognitive function and personality traits are summarized in the online data supplement. The neurocognitive tests were selected on the basis of their known cortico-striatal and medio-temporal neural substrates, which are brain systems that have been associated with the pathophysiology of drug dependence. Personality measures were included because of their hypothesized associations with drug dependence. Measures of trait-anxiety, stress-sensitivity, and trauma history were used as markers of psychological stress or vulnerability. All participants were assessed and tested in a fixed order with the same cognitive battery at a clinical research facility, and they were allowed breaks as needed.

Data Analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS, version 13, using a five-step strategy:
1. 
We performed data reduction by Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) domains to control for the number of tests by calculating summary mean scores from z-transformed test variables within the cognitive domains.
2. 
We tested the specificity of cognitive effects by performing repeated-measures analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) with group as the between-subject factor and domain as the within-subject factor. Where Mauchly tests showed violation of the sphericity assumption, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were used. Nonsignificant group-by-domain interactions were compatible with the null hypothesis that all cognitive impairments are attributable to deficits in general intelligence.
3. 
We used ANCOVA models for group comparisons for each cognitive domain, which were controlled for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni correction.
4. 
We made post hoc comparisons for each significant domain using paired t tests to control for relatedness of siblings and drug-dependent individuals and independent t tests for comparisons between unrelated pairs.
5. 
We used permutation statistics for familiality testing for each domain impaired in the sibling pairs relative to healthy comparison volunteers. The within-pair variance between the biological sibling pairs was compared with a permuted distribution of random pairs of drug-dependent-individuals and unrelated siblings (46).
Additionally, we conducted exploratory analyses at the subdomain level of individual tests using ANCOVA models, followed by post hoc comparisons using the Bonferroni correction. Gender was included as a covariate in all analyses to control for the significant group differences of gender. To obviate the confounding effects of dysphoric mood on cognitive performance (47), a participant score on the Beck Depression Inventory-II was also included as a covariate in the analysis of the cognitive data. ANCOVA models were applied to compare the results for individuals with and without parental drug or alcohol abuse separately within the drug-dependent and sibling groups; verbal IQ was included as a covariate to control for IQ differences between these subgroups. The affective and personality domains were defined according to the questionnaire constructs, and we followed the same analysis methods as outlined above. Independent t tests were used to compare cognitive and personality profiles of drug-dependent individuals with and without comorbid opioid dependence. The Padua Inventory–Washington State University Revision and the Perceived Stress Scale (version 14) total scores were square-root transformed to reduce skew, as described by Howell (48). For the group differences in demographic characteristics, ANCOVA models, chi-square tests, or Fisher’s exact tests were applied as appropriate for the analysis of categorical data. Pearson correlations were estimated where appropriate using the Bonferroni correction. All tests were two-tailed, and we set a significance level of 0.05. Because of technical problems, Rapid Visual Information Processing, One-Touch Stockings of Cambridge, and stop-signal data for one drug-dependent participant were unavailable.

Results

Demographic, Familial, Social, and Mood Data

The groups were matched for age, verbal intelligence, and duration of formal education (Table 1), but they differed with regard to gender, as the majority of drug-dependent individuals were men. The shared familial environment in the sibling pairs, which lasted in most cases into adolescence, was notably different from the childhood familial environment reported by healthy volunteers; the sibling pairs had larger household sizes, a higher incidence of parental divorce and of parents with addiction problems, and more frequent experiences of childhood abuse.

Cognitive Domains

Task performance differed significantly over the four cognitive domains (executive function, visual memory, attention control, and response control) across the three groups, as reflected by a highly significant domain-by-group interaction (F=6.7, df=2.6, 371.7, p<0.001), refuting the null hypothesis that all cognitive impairments were attributable to deficits in general intelligence. No significant main effects of domain were found, but there was a significant main effect of group (F=9.6, df=2, 142, p<0.001). To investigate the nature of the interaction, we compared the groups separately on each domain, revealing significant results for the domains of executive function (F=16.0, df=2, 144, p=0.004) and response control (F=5.9, df=2, 144, p=0.012). Figure 1A shows that the difference between groups in terms of visual memory and attention did not reach significance. Post hoc comparisons revealed that both siblings and drug-dependent individuals differed significantly from healthy volunteers in executive function (tsibs=2.6, p=0.012; tdrug=8.8, p<0.001) and from each other (t=5.9, p<0.001). In response control, the sibling pairs differed significantly from healthy volunteers (tsibs=4.0, p<0.001; tdrug=3.6, p<0.001) but not from each other. Parental drug or alcohol abuse was associated with aggravated performance in executive function and visual memory only in drug-dependent individuals but not in their siblings.
FIGURE 1. Cognitive and Emotional Profiles of Participants in a Study of Endophenotypes for Drug Dependencea
aPanel A shows performance profiles in four cognitive domains in healthy comparison volunteers, unaffected siblings, and drug-dependent individuals. Panel B shows characteristic profiles in affective, psychosocial, and personality domains.

Affective and Personality Domains

We observed a significant domain-by-group interaction for affective and personality domains (F=23.0, df=2, 45, 356, p<0.001), providing support for the hypothesized specificity of the domains. We observed a significant main effect of group (F=10.2, df=2, 45, 356, p<0.001) but not a significant main effect of domain. Group comparisons conducted separately for each domain revealed significant group differences in all four domains: emotional (F=37.3, df=2, 145, p<0.001), psychosocial (F=8.7, df=2, 146, p<0.001), impulsivity-compulsivity (F=36.7, df=2, 146, p<0.001), and self-evaluation (F=11.8, df=2, 146, p<0.001). As shown in Figure 1B, both siblings and drug-dependent individuals differed significantly from healthy comparison volunteers in emotional functioning (tsibs=2.8, p=0.007; tdrug=8.4, p<0.001), impulsivity-compulsivity (tsibs=−2.6, p=0.012; tdrug=−8.9, p<0.001), and self-evaluation traits (tsibs=2.0, p=0.044; tdrug=5.3, p<0.001). On psychosocial functioning, the siblings were not different from healthy volunteers, but data from drug-dependent individuals revealed significant psychosocial impairment (t=4.4, p<0.001). Figure 1B further illustrates that the impairments in all four domains were exacerbated in the drug-dependent individuals compared with their siblings (emotional: t=−5.5; psychosocial: t=−3.4; impulsivity-compulsivity: t=6.5; self-evaluation: t=3.2; p<0.005 in all cases). Parental drug or alcohol abuse did not affect emotional function and personality traits in the sibling pairs.

Familial Relatedness and Traumatic Childhood

The difference between sibling pairs and healthy volunteers reached significance in executive function and response control. We compared the within-pair variance between the biological sibling pairs in these two measures with the variance between random sibling pairs (46). The observed variance on response control was significantly smaller within the biological pairs compared with the randomly permutated distribution (p=0.006), indicating that impairment in response control is a shared trait between family members. For executive function, variances in biological pairs did not significantly differ from those in random sibling pairs, suggesting that impairments in executive function are not familial. Biological sibling pairs also differed significantly from unrelated healthy volunteers in emotional function, impulsivity-compulsivity traits, and self-evaluation, but the variances within these biological pairs did not significantly differ in any of these domains from variances within randomly permutated pairs. Comorbid dependence on opioids was not related to a different cognitive or personality profile in the current sample.
The sibling pairs reported significantly higher levels of childhood trauma compared with unrelated healthy volunteers (Table 1). We used a composite variable across all of the three types of abuse (49) to explore the relationship within the sibling pairs’ traumatic childhood and the cognitive and personal domain measures. Executive function (r=−0.3, p<0.05) and impulsive-compulsive traits (r=0.3, p<0.05) were significantly associated with the degree of childhood abuse in the sibling pairs. The significant relationship between childhood abuse and emotional function, however, did not survive correction for multiple comparisons.

Cognitive Profiles and Indexes of Drug Abuse

Correlational analyses between last stimulant use, duration of stimulant abuse, and cognitive or personality domain measures did not reach significance. Compulsive pattern of stimulant use was significantly associated with emotional functioning (r=−0.5, p<0.05) and self-evaluation (r=0.5, p<0.05). Relationships between alcohol use (as reflected by the AUDIT score) and cognitive/personality measures did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. No statistically significant relationships were observed between cognitive performance and the duration of cannabis use or the age at onset. Trait impulsivity and anxiety traits were associated with each other in all volunteers (r=0.5, p<0.05). Cognitive and personality domains correlated with each other; a full correlation matrix can be found in the online data supplement.

Discussion

By comparing the characteristic phenotypes of drug-dependent individuals with those of their unaffected siblings and unrelated healthy volunteers, our study identified candidate endophenotypes for drug dependence. We found shared abnormalities in the sibling pairs in domains of executive cognitive and response control, emotional function, impulsivity-compulsivity traits, and self-evaluation; the detailed results are summarized in Table 2.
TABLE 2. Descriptive Statistics and Between-Group Comparisons for Cognitive, Emotional, and Social Function as well as Personality Traits Data on Drug-Dependent Individuals, Their Siblings, and Unrelated Healthy Volunteers
MeasurementsHealthy Volunteers (N=50)Unaffected Siblings (N=50)Drug-Dependent Individuals (N=50)Group ComparisonPost Hoc Comparisonsa
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSDFpHealthy Volunteers and SiblingsHealthy Volunteers and Drug-DependentDrug-Dependent and Siblings
Cognitive domains           
Executive function           
 Spatial Working Memory           
  Strategy score29.16.630.95.634.54.63.20.0420.7420.0360.261
  Total number of errors17.014.42115.434.719.26.00.0030.9570.0020.019
  Search reaction time (ms)1,040.0259.21161.9359.11437590.35.10.0070.5250.0050.080
 One-Touch Stockings of Cambridge           
  Attempts, easy solutions1.050.081.070.121.090.130.30.744   
  Attempts, medium solutions1.10.141.160.151.40.3511.6<0.0010.932<0.001<0.001
  Attempts, hard solutions1.480.331.660.452.270.7120.4<0.0010.318<0.001<0.001
Visual memory           
 Pattern Recognition Memory           
  % correct (immediate)97.24.694.87.989.212.32.70.071   
  % correct (delayed by 25 mins.)87.510.784.215.272.816.56.30.0021.0000.0020.011
 Paired Associates Learning           
  First trial memory score16.92.515.33.612.53.312.2<0.0010.038<0.0010.006
  Total number of trials7.62.19.13.012.53.716.5<0.0010.081<0.001<0.001
  Total number of errors7.47.112.613.627.216.712.7<0.0010.207<0.0010.001
Attention           
 Reaction Time Task           
  Accuracy score14.80.314.60.413.81.36.30.0021.0000.0020.013
  Premature responses0.020.100.050.150.090.192.40.094   
  Reaction time (ms)290.431.3294.433.8297.0108.62.20.116   
 Rapid visual information processing           
  A’ (detection accuracy)0.930.050.900.050.890.055.90.0030.0440.0050.528
  B” (response bias)0.900.280.920.090.900.080.20.851   
  Reaction time, correct responses (ms)386.884.0406.787.5441.7111.23.70.0270.8640.0220.155
  Total number of commission errors1.42.02.12.92.62.72.50.084   
  Total number of  omission errors7.64.79.95.310.65.35.40.0060.0470.0090.729
Response control           
 Stop-signal percentage successful stops55.03.456.13.156.85.01.20.312   
 Stop-signal mean reaction time           
  Successful go trials473.859.6501.161.7504.189.82.30.108   
  Unsuccessful stop trials446.646.0477.447.3461.959.03.80.0240.0210.3741.000
  Post-stop go trials463.6103.3498.687.4607.6874.00.10.875   
  Stop-signal reaction  time238.945.0276.955.0280.360.67.40.0010.0040.0041.000
Emotion-personality domains           
Emotional functioning           
 Depressive mood (Beck Depression Inventory-II)2.22.65.16.218.111.955.3<0.0010.177<0.001<0.001
 Anhedonia (Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale)0.20.60.61.41.41.96.90.0010.4320.0010.087
 Anxiety (Spielberger Anxiety Inventory-trait)30.05.934.810.046.411.237.6<0.0010.035<0.001<0.001
 Anxious avoidance (Behavioral-Approach/Behavioral Inhibition Scale)19.13.519.13.819.73.31.80.162   
 Perceived stress (Perceived Stress Scale)23.97.127.57.535.38.225.0<0.0010.037<0.001<0.001
Psychosocial functioning           
 Community Integration Questionnaire           
  Home and family life5.82.16.32.46.32.91.60.205   
  Social activities8.11.881.86.51.810.1<0.0011.000<0.0010.001
  Productive activities3.80.93.21.61.91.823.9<0.0010.195<0.001<0.001
Impulsive-compulsive traits           
 Impulsivity (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, Version 11)59.47.567.010.676.310.236.4<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
 Sensation-seeking (Sensation-Seeking Scale, Form V)18.66.118.35.223.65.88.00.0011.0000.0010.003
 Reward approach (Behavioral-Approach/Behavioral Inhibition Scale)40.15.040.75.642.85.13.80.0251.0000.0290.106
 Ritualistic behaviors (Padua Inventory for Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms)7.56.912.311.021.815.321.5<0.0010.020<0.0010.001
Self-evaluation traits           
 Self-efficacy (General Self-Efficacy Scale)33.03.432.24.129.34.59.7<0.0010.868<0.0010.006
 Social Comparison Rating (Social Comparison Rating Scale)75.716.669.813.264.017.96.20.0030.2200.0020.279
 Locus of control (Internal-External Scale)b10.72.810.02.99.03.23.00.055   
a
Analyses are controlled for gender and dysphoric mood, p<0.05; the Bonferroni correction is applied for the post hoc comparisons.
b
The three items of the Internal-External scale referring to situations at school have previously been found inappropriate for adults and were therefore removed from the scale.

Cognitive Endophenotypes for Drug Dependence

The siblings’ cognitive profile was characterized by deficits in executive function such as working memory and mental planning. The impairments became especially apparent when the rapid suppression of an ongoing, well-established response was required. The siblings showed a general slowing in response speed, and this was even slower in drug-dependent individuals (Table 2). Similar profiles of impaired response regulation have been reported in adults with a family history of alcohol dependence (50) and in children with drug-dependent parents (51, 52). We believe that the cognitive deficits identified in both drug-dependent individuals and their siblings represent a shared trait in family members that predates the exposure to stimulant drugs and may be a predisposing risk factor for the development of drug dependence. This proposal is supported by a longitudinal study in boys with and without a family history of alcoholism: poor inhibitory control, as indicated by prolonged stop-signal response time, predicted the onset of substance abuse in all children, but most strongly in those with alcohol-dependent parents (53).
Contemporary models of the pathogenesis of drug dependence suggest that immaturity of the regulatory control systems in the prefrontal cortex renders adolescents vulnerable to the initiation of drug abuse (54). The progressive breakdown of inhibitory control implemented by this circuitry, however, has been attributed to the repeated abuse of drugs, possibly paving the way for the development of drug dependence (55). Impaired regulatory abilities in those siblings without a history of chronic drug abuse might indicate a developmental dysfunction of prefrontal control. We recently showed that impaired stop-signal performance in the nondependent siblings of drug-dependent individuals was associated with reduced fractional anisotropy in diffusion tensor imaging scans of frontal white matter, which is compatible with disrupted anatomical connectivity of the inferior prefrontal cortex (56) (see Figure 2). The low within-pair variation in response control performance between the sibling pairs further supports the familial risk for impaired inhibitory control in people with stimulant dependence and their first-degree relatives. Yet, despite this higher familial risk of addiction, some of the siblings have experimented with drugs but never made the transition to addiction. Thus, occasional exposure to drugs, such as the recreational use of cannabis and social drinking, did not produce addiction in these high-risk individuals. The potentially protective factors against addiction in at-risk individuals are incompletely characterized and merit further study.
FIGURE 2. Deficits of Motor Inhibitory Control and White Matter Organization in Drug-Dependent Individuals and Their Unaffected Siblingsa
a Panel A shows stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), which was significantly prolonged in both the stimulant-dependent individuals and their nondependent siblings compared with unrelated healthy volunteers (p≤0.005 for both comparisons with the Bonferroni correction). Panel B shows the skeleton of group differences in mean fractional anisotropy (FA) is colored in blue; stimulant-dependent individuals and their first-degree relatives both had significantly reduced fractional anisotropy compared with unrelated healthy comparison volunteers. On the basis of prior literature, regions of interest were selected within the blue skeleton that included the inferior frontal gyrus and the presupplementary motor area (in orange). Panel C is a scatter plot showing that participants with greater fractional anisotropy in the right inferior frontal gyrus had better inhibitory performance (i.e., shorter stop-signal reaction time) on the stop-signal task (r=0.24, df=142, p<0.005). From Ersche et al. (56). Reprinted with permission from American Association for the Advancement of Science.

Personality Endophenotypes for Drug Dependence

As shown by our preliminary data, the sibling pairs reported significantly higher levels of trait impulsivity than did the healthy comparison volunteers (57). Our data further indicate reduced emotional functioning in the sibling pairs, as reflected by high levels of trait anxiety and stress sensitivity (Table 2). Anxious-impulsivity trait has previously been described by Newman and Wallace (58) as a breakdown of inhibitory control in situations when the escape from aversive consequences appears impossible. More recently, a similar concept of negative urgency has been proposed that describes a personality trait of impulsive actions in response to intense negative affect (59). The higher levels of anxious-impulsivity in the sibling pairs suggest underlying deficits in emotion regulation, an ability that develops through the maturation of the fronto-limbic brain systems (60, 61). Developmental imbalances between these systems generally emerge during adolescence. Emotion systems in limbic structures mature before control systems in the prefrontal cortex (60), possibly laying the foundation for individual differences in emotion regulation or psychopathologies (62, 63). The concept of self-evaluation includes individuals’ beliefs and perceptions about themselves that influence their mental and social well-being (64). Low levels of self-efficacy and self-perception in relation to other people, as shown in the present sample (Table 2), have been associated with addictive behaviors (65).
Relationships between childhood trauma and executive function as well as impulsivity have been reported in stimulant-dependent individuals (66). These relationships are not just restricted to dependent individuals but also affect their nondependent siblings. Our results are therefore consistent with the contemporary literature, suggesting that abuse experiences during childhood have long-lasting effects on cognitive function and behavior in adulthood (6769).

Dysfunction Associated With Chronic Drug Abuse

As hypothesized, and in keeping with previous studies (15, 18, 70), cognitive function in drug-dependent individuals was significantly impaired on all tests. Impairments in executive function were significantly exacerbated in drug-dependent individuals compared with their siblings, suggesting that neurotoxic or other ancillary effects of chronic drug abuse may account for the scale of the impairments. However, we did not find significant correlations between the duration of stimulant abuse and cognitive performance, indicating that the relationship is more complex than for a simple exposure hypothesis. The fact that almost all drug-dependent individuals tested positive for stimulants at the time of testing may also have affected performance to some degree (71, 72). We did not find any relationships between the time of last stimulant use and performance in the measured cognitive domains. Although chronic drug abuse has a negative influence on cognitive performance, some cognitive deficits also exist in first-degree relatives in the absence of drug abuse. Cognitive dysfunction may thus be considered as a stable trait predating drug-taking that is independent of the states of intoxication or withdrawal.
Consistent with our preliminary data (57), higher levels of sensation- and reward-seeking personality were observed only in drug-dependent individuals but not in their siblings (Table 2), suggesting that the need for excitement is specific for individuals with chronic stimulant drug exposure. Levels of anxious-impulsivity were also higher in drug-dependent individuals than in their siblings, indicating that chronic drug abuse further increases both anxiety and impulsive traits. The significantly higher levels of stress-sensitivity in drug-dependent individuals are consistent with the notion that the system implicated in stress overlaps with brain circuitries associated with addiction (73, 74).

Strength and Weaknesses

The strengths of the study include the relatively large sample size and well-characterized participants in terms of personality and demographic variables. The neuropsychological tests we used have been validated in terms of their neural substrates (75). The test battery was theoretically focused on cognitive mechanisms of special relevance to drug dependence and demonstrated excellent sensitivity for the detection of cognitive endophenotypes in sibling pairs. The drug-dependent individuals were recruited from a variety of sources, enhancing the generalizability of the results. From its relatively novel perspective, our study confirmed and extended several findings using other approaches, suggesting that male gender, stressful life events, dysfunctional family structure, anxious personality, and impulsivity are associated with a heightened risk for drug dependence (32). Although the siblings shared many of these risk factors with their drug-dependent brothers and sisters, they did not engage in drug abuse. Their average age of 33 years suggests that they may have had many opportunities to initiate drug abuse and develop dependence, which makes them ideal candidates for the study of endophenotypes.
Potential limitations of the study include the use of a sibling design instead of a twin design; the latter would have allowed the disentangling of genetic from environmental influences, but it would have been prohibitively difficult to complete. However, we used permutation statistics to test the familiality of salient markers as well as correlational analyses to explore the relationships with childhood trauma and the candidate endophenotype markers. Longitudinal studies are now warranted in order to clarify whether abnormalities that are not shared between the sibling pairs reflect a more selective higher “dose” of genetic or environmental risk factors in those individuals who become dependent. In contrast to previous research suggesting that the genetic influence on opioid abuse is specific and not shared with other drugs (8), we did not find differences in the cognitive and personality profiles of drug-dependent individuals with and without opiate dependence or between their siblings. However, further research is also needed to clarify whether these findings reflect a specific vulnerability to stimulants or to drug dependence more generally. Finally, our study does not directly address the important question of why some individuals with familial risks do not become dependent on drugs. This shortcoming lies in the endophenotype concept itself, which defines “intermediate markers of genetic risk,” not of resilience. Presumably, the nondependent siblings have benefited from some resilience or protective factors to offset the risks they shared with their dependent siblings, but it is not clear what these protective factors might be.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the volunteers, the staff at the Mental Health Research Network, the staff at the Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility at Addenbrooke’s Hospital, and Daisy Appanah for her invaluable help in making room at the Research Facility available whenever needed.

Footnotes

Dr. Chamberlain consults for Cambridge Cognition, P1Vital, Shire, Eli Lilly, and Lucinda Ellery. Mrs. Turton and Dr. Robbins consult for Cambridge Cognition; Dr. Robbins also consults for Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, and GlaxoSmithKline and holds research grants from those companies. Dr. Bullmore is employed part time by GlaxoSmithKline and part time by the University of Cambridge.
Preliminary data on the cognitive performance and personality traits of 30 sibling pairs and 30 comparison volunteers were presented at the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology Annual Meeting in Amsterdam, August 2010, and parts of it were subsequently published in Biological Psychiatry.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material (appi.ajp.2012.11091421_ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Nutt DJ, Robbins TW, Stimson GV, Ince M, Jackson ARW: Drugs and the Future: Brain Science, Addiction and Society. Oxford, UK, Elsevier, 2006
2.
Nutt D, King LA, Saulsbury W, Blakemore C: Development of a rational scale to assess the harm of drugs of potential misuse. Lancet 2007; 369:1047–1053
3.
Merikangas KR, Stolar M, Stevens DE, Goulet J, Preisig MA, Fenton B, Zhang HP, O’Malley SS, Rounsaville BJ: Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:973–979
4.
Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Neale MC: Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:687–695
5.
Tyrfingsson T, Thorgeirsson TE, Geller F, Runarsdottir V, Hansdottir I, Bjornsdottir G, Wiste AK, Jonsdottir GA, Stefansson H, Gulcher JR, Oskarsson H, Gudbjartsson D, Stefansson K: Addictions and their familiality in Iceland. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2010; 1187: 208–217
6.
Zucker RA: The developmental behavior genetics of drug involvement: overview and comments. Behav Genet 2006; 36:616–625
7.
Vanyukov MM, Kirisci L, Moss L, Tarter RE, Reynolds MD, Maher BS, Kirillova GP, Ridenour T, Clark DB: Measurement of the risk for substance use disorders: phenotypic and genetic analysis of an index of common liability. Behav Genet 2009; 39:233–244
8.
Tsuang MT, Lyons MJ, Meyer JM, Doyle T, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, True W, Lin N, Toomey R, Eaves L: Co-occurrence of abuse of different drugs in men: the role of drug-specific and shared vulnerabilities. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:967–972
9.
Bierut LJ: Genetic vulnerability and susceptibility to substance dependence. Neuron 2011; 69:618–627
10.
Gottesman II, Gould TD: The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:636–645
11.
Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW: Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004; 28:771–784
12.
Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND: Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1642–1652
13.
Fillmore MT, Rush CR: Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend 2002; 66:265–273
14.
Monterosso JR, Aron AR, Cordova X, Xu J, London ED: Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend 2005; 79:273–277
15.
Ersche KD, Clark L, London M, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ: Profile of executive and memory function associated with amphetamine and opiate dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006; 31:1036–1047
16.
Ornstein TJ, Iddon JL, Baldacchino AM, Sahakian BJ, London M, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW: Profiles of cognitive dysfunction in chronic amphetamine and heroin abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 23:113–126
17.
Tomasi D, Goldstein RZ, Telang F, Maloney T, Alia-Klein N, Caparelli EC, Volkow ND: Widespread disruption in brain activation patterns to a working memory task during cocaine abstinence. Brain Res 2007; 1171:83–92
18.
Ersche KD, Roiser JP, Lucas M, Domenici E, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET: Peripheral biomarkers of cognitive response to dopamine receptor agonist treatment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011; 214:779–789
19.
Fernández-Serrano MJ, Pérez-García M, Schmidt Río-Valle J, Verdejo-García A: Neuropsychological consequences of alcohol and drug abuse on different components of executive functions. J Psychopharmacol 2010; 24:1317–1332
20.
Gooding DC, Burroughs S, Boutros NN: Attentional deficits in cocaine-dependent patients: converging behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Psychiatry Res 2008; 160:145–154
21.
Ersche KD, Barnes A, Jones PS, Morein-Zamir S, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET: Abnormal structure of frontostriatal brain systems is associated with aspects of impulsivity and compulsivity in cocaine dependence. Brain 2011; 134:2013–2024
22.
London ED, Berman SM, Voytek B, Simon SL, Mandelkern MA, Monterosso J, Thompson PM, Brody AL, Geaga JA, Hong MS, Hayashi KM, Rawson RA, Ling W: Cerebral metabolic dysfunction and impaired vigilance in recently abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:770–778
23.
Beveridge TJR, Smith HR, Daunais JB, Nader MA, Porrino LJ: Chronic cocaine self-administration is associated with altered functional activity in the temporal lobes of non human primates. Eur J Neurosci 2006; 23:3109–3118
24.
Beveridge TJR, Gill KE, Hanlon CA, Porrino LJ: Parallel studies of cocaine-related neural and cognitive impairment in humans and monkeys. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2008; 363:3257–3266
25.
Regier DA, Farmer ME, Rae DS, Locke BZ, Keith SJ, Judd LL, Goodwin FK: Comorbidity of mental disorders with alcohol and other drug abuse: results from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) study. JAMA 1990; 264:2511–2518
26.
Merikangas KR, Mehta RL, Molnar BE, Walters EE, Swendsen JD, Aguilar-Gaziola S, Bijl R, Borges G, Caraveo-Anduaga JJ, Dewit DJ, Kolody B, Vega WA, Wittchen HU, Kessler RC: Comorbidity of substance use disorders with mood and anxiety disorders: results of the International Consortium in Psychiatric Epidemiology. Addict Behav 1998; 23:893–907
27.
Merikangas KR, Dierker LC, Szatmari P: Psychopathology among offspring of parents with substance abuse and/or anxiety disorders: a high-risk study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998; 39:711–720
28.
Volkow ND: The reality of comorbidity: depression and drug abuse. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 56:714–717
29.
Kosten TR, Markou A, Koob GF: Depression and stimulant dependence: neurobiology and pharmacotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:737–745
30.
Markou A, Kosten TR, Koob GF: Neurobiological similarities in depression and drug dependence: a self-medication hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology 1998; 18:135–174
31.
Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Miller JY: Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychol Bull 1992; 112:64–105
32.
Fisher M, Crome I, Macleod J, Bloor R, Hickman M: Predictive factors for illicit drug use among young people: a literature review. Probation Journal 2007; 54:278–279
33.
de Wit H: Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol 2009; 14:22–31
34.
Meda SA, Stevens MC, Potenza MN, Pittman B, Gueorguieva R, Andrews MM, Thomas AD, Muska C, Hylton JL, Pearlson GD: Investigating the behavioral and self-report constructs of impulsivity domains using principal component analysis. Behav Pharmacol 2009; 20:390–399
35.
Stephenson MT, Helme DW: Authoritative parenting and sensation seeking as predictors of adolescent cigarette and marijuana use. J Drug Educ 2006; 36:247–270
36.
Lawrance L, Rubinson L: Self-efficacy as a predictor of smoking behavior in young adolescents. Addict Behav 1986; 11:367–382
37.
Barkin SL, Smith KS, DuRant RH: Social skills and attitudes associated with substance use behaviors among young adolescents. J Adolesc Health 2002; 30:448–454
38.
Newcomb MD, Harlow LL: Life events and substance use among adolescents: mediating effects of perceived loss of control and meaninglessness in life. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986; 51:564–577
39.
Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Croudace T, Stochl J: Who do you think is in control in addiction? a pilot study on drug-related locus of control beliefs. Addict Disord Their Treatment (Epub ahead of print, Jan 15, 2012)
40.
Dielman TE, Campanelli PC, Shope JT, Butchart AT: Susceptibility to peer pressure, self-esteem, and health locus of control as correlates of adolescent substance abuse. Health Educ Q 1987; 14:207–221
41.
Goldman D, Oroszi G, Ducci F: The genetics of addictions: uncovering the genes. Nat Rev Genet 2005; 6:521–532
42.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-Patient Edition (SCID-I/NP). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 2002
43.
Franken IHA, Hendriksa VM, van den Brink W: Initial validation of two opiate craving questionnaires: the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale and the Desires for Drug Questionnaire. Addict Behav 2002; 27:675–685
44.
Gavin DR, Ross HE, Skinner HA: Diagnostic validity of the Drug Abuse Screening Test in the assessment of DSM-III drug disorders. Br J Addict 1989; 84:301–307
45.
Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M: Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption—II. Addiction 1993; 88:791–804
46.
Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N, Chen CH, del Campo N, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Bullmore E: Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 2007; 130:3223–3236
47.
Hartlage S, Alloy LB, Vázquez C, Dykman B: Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychol Bull 1993; 113:247–278
48.
Howell DC: Statistical Methods for Psychology. London, Duxbury Press, 1997
49.
Bradley RG, Binder EB, Epstein MP, Tang Y, Nair HP, Liu W, Gillespie CF, Berg T, Evces M, Newport DJ, Stowe ZN, Heim CM, Nemeroff CB, Schwartz A, Cubells JF, Ressler KJ: Influence of child abuse on adult depression: moderation by the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor gene. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:190–200
50.
Acheson A, Richard DM, Mathias CW, Dougherty DM: Adults with a family history of alcohol-related problems are more impulsive on measures of response initiation and response inhibition. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011; 117:198–203
51.
Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, Cornelius JR, Pajer K, Vanyukov M, Gardner W, Blackson T, Clark D: Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1078–1085
52.
Dawes MA, Tarter RE, Kirisci L: Behavioral self-regulation: correlates and 2-year follow-ups for boys at risk for substance abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend 1997; 45:165–176
53.
Nigg JT, Wong MM, Martel MM, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass JM, Adams KM, Fitzgerald HE, Zucker RA: Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2006; 45:468–475
54.
Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN: Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1041–1052
55.
Kalivas PW, Volkow ND: The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1403–1413
56.
Ersche KD, Jones PS, Williams GB, Turton AJ, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET: Abnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science 2012; 335:601–604
57.
Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Pradhan S, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW: Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus sensation-seeking personality traits. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 68:770–773
58.
Newman JP, Wallace JF: Diverse pathways to deficient self-regulation: implications for disinhibitory psychopathology in children. Clin Psychol Rev 1993; 13:699–720
59.
Whiteside SP, Lynam DR: The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers Individ Dif 2001; 30:669–689
60.
Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA: The adolescent brain. Ann NY Acad Sci 2008; 1124:111–126
61.
Steinberg L: Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9:69–74
62.
Lewis MD, Todd RM: The self-regulating brain: cortical-subcortical feedback and the development of intelligent action. Cogn Dev 2007; 22:406–430
63.
Ernst M, Romeo RD, Andersen SL: Neurobiology of the development of motivated behaviors in adolescence: a window into a neural systems model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2009; 93:199–211
64.
Mann M, Hosman CMH, Schaalma HP, de Vries NK: Self-esteem in a broad-spectrum approach for mental health promotion. Health Educ Res 2004; 19:357–372
65.
DiClimente CC: Self-efficacy and the addictive behaviours. J Soc Clin Psychol 1986; 4:302–315
66.
Narvaez JCM, Magalhaes PVS, Trindade EK, Vieira DC, Kauer-Sant’Anna M, Gama CS, von Diemen L, Kapczinski NS, Kapczinski F: Childhood trauma, impulsivity, and executive functioning in crack cocaine users. Compr Psychiatry 2012; 53:238–244
67.
Brodsky BS, Oquendo M, Ellis SP, Haas GL, Malone KM, Mann JJ: The relationship of childhood abuse to impulsivity and suicidal behavior in adults with major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1871–1877
68.
Roy A: Childhood trauma and impulsivity: possible relevance to suicidal behavior. Arch Suicide Res 2005; 9:147–151
69.
Pechtel P, Pizzagalli DA: Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011; 214:55–70
70.
Verdejo-García A, Pérez-García M: Profile of executive deficits in cocaine and heroin polysubstance users: common and differential effects on separate executive components. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007; 190:517–530
71.
Pace-Schott EF, Morgan PT, Malison RT, Hart CL, Edgar C, Walker M, Stickgold R: Cocaine users differ from normals on cognitive tasks which show poorer performance during drug abstinence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2008; 34:109–121
72.
Woicik PA, Moeller SJ, Alia-Klein N, Maloney T, Lukasik TM, Yeliosof O, Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Goldstein RZ: The neuropsychology of cocaine addiction: recent cocaine use masks impairment. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 34:1112–1122
73.
Kreek MJ, Koob GF: Drug dependence: stress and dysregulation of brain reward pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend 1998; 51:23–47
74.
Sinha R: How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 158:343–359
75.
Lee ACH, Owen AM, Rogers RD, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW: Utility of CANTAB in functional neuroimaging, in Functional Neuroimaging in Child Psychology. Edited by, Ernst M, Rumsey J. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 2000, pp 366–377

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 926 - 936
PubMed: 22952072

History

Received: 23 September 2011
Revision received: 12 December 2011
Revision received: 13 March 2012
Accepted: 13 April 2012
Published online: 1 September 2012
Published in print: September 2012

Authors

Details

Karen D. Ersche, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, and the Department of Experimental Psychology at University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Glaxo-SmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge; and Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge.
Abigail J. Turton, B.Sc.
From the Department of Psychiatry, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, and the Department of Experimental Psychology at University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Glaxo-SmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge; and Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge.
Samuel R. Chamberlain, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, and the Department of Experimental Psychology at University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Glaxo-SmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge; and Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge.
Ulrich Müller, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, and the Department of Experimental Psychology at University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Glaxo-SmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge; and Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge.
Edward T. Bullmore, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, and the Department of Experimental Psychology at University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Glaxo-SmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge; and Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge.
Trevor W. Robbins, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, and the Department of Experimental Psychology at University of Cambridge, Cambridge, U.K.; the Glaxo-SmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge; and Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Ersche ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Supported by a Medical Research Council grant to Drs. Ersche, Bullmore, and Robbins and conducted at the Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute at University of Cambridge, U.K., which is supported by a joint award from the Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share