Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

Social impairment is a long-recognized core feature of schizophrenia and is common in other psychotic disorders. Still, to date the long-term trajectories of social impairment in psychotic disorders have rarely been studied systematically.

Methods:

Data came from the Suffolk County Mental Health Project, a 20-year prospective study of first-admission patients with psychotic disorders. A never-psychotic comparison group was also assessed. Latent class growth analysis was applied to longitudinal data on social functioning from 485 respondents with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and psychotic mood disorders, and associations of the empirically derived trajectories with premorbid social adjustment, diagnosis, and 20-year outcomes were examined.

Results:

Four mostly stable trajectories of preserved (N=82; 59th percentile of comparison group sample distribution), moderately impaired (N=148; 17th percentile), severely impaired (N=181; 3rd percentile), and profoundly impaired (N=74; 1st percentile) functioning best described the 20-year course of social functioning across diagnoses. The outcome in the group with preserved functioning did not differ from that of never-psychotic individuals at 20 years, but the other groups functioned significantly worse. Differences among trajectories were already evident in childhood. The two most impaired trajectories started to diverge in early adolescence. Poorer social functioning trajectories were strongly associated with other real-world outcomes at 20 years. Multiple trajectories were represented within each disorder. However, more participants with schizophrenia spectrum disorders had impaired trajectories, and more with mood disorders had better functioning trajectories.

Conclusions:

The results highlight substantial variability of social outcomes within diagnoses—albeit overall worse social outcomes in schizophrenia spectrum disorders—and show remarkably stable long-term impairments in social functioning after illness onset across all diagnoses.
Impairment in social functioning is a core feature of schizophrenia. It is characterized by difficulties in achieving social milestones and establishing relationships, such as social network involvement and marriage or family life (14). Real-world indices of functioning have gained increasing importance in investigations into recovery (5, 6), and social functioning, defined as involvement in social interactions and social activities, has been recognized as a key outcome measure for determining treatment success (7, 8).
In contrast to the growing awareness about the importance of social functioning for tracking outcome, previous reports have left several issues unresolved. First, it has been shown that social outcomes in schizophrenia are poor (9), but prospective evaluations reported mixed findings, with improving (1012), stable (13, 14), and declining (15) social functioning after illness onset. In addition, studies generally examined group averages without taking differences between individuals within psychotic disorders into account. Averages can mask functional recovery or deterioration present in subgroups of patients. It is important to explicate the different long-term trajectories of social functioning in order to identify critical periods and specific trajectories that warrant intervention.
While considerable research has been done in schizophrenia, social outcomes in other psychotic illnesses have been less studied (1517). It is generally assumed that schizophrenia is associated with worse social functional outcomes compared with other psychotic disorders, but the few studies that directly tested this assumption by comparing the longitudinal courses of social functioning in affective and nonaffective psychoses have yielded conflicting findings. The pioneering work of Harrow and colleagues found evidence that social impairment was more severe in schizophrenia than other psychotic disorders at 7.5- and 15-year follow-up (10, 18). However, two other studies reported comparable levels of social functioning in schizophrenia and affective psychosis. The first, a cross-sectional study, compared individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (19), and the second study compared affective disorders and schizophrenia 6 months after hospitalization (17). Thus, the evidence for diagnosis-specific differences in psychosocial functioning is inconsistent.
Moreover, while a wealth of research has shown that poor premorbid functioning is associated with poorer outcomes after illness onset at cross-sectional time points, it remains unclear whether poor premorbid functioning is associated with continuously poor social trajectories. Finally, the findings across studies have been mixed in terms of how strongly social functioning is related to other daily life outcomes, with results ranging from fairly weak to strong associations (20).
The current study aimed to address these questions by examining differences in the trajectories of social functioning over 20 years across and within diagnostic groups in a large, countywide sample of first-admission individuals with affective and nonaffective psychosis (21). We also sought to 1) examine associations of these trajectories with premorbid social functioning and 2) evaluate their associations with other areas of functioning at 20-year follow-up. Finally, we examined the severity of impairment of social functioning 20 years postadmission by comparing the trajectory groups to a never-psychotic comparison group that was matched on demographic characteristics and neighborhood.

Method

Sample

Participants came from the Suffolk County Mental Health Project, a longitudinal countywide study of first-admission patients with a psychotic disorder (21, 22). They were recruited from the 12 psychiatric inpatient units in Suffolk County, N.Y., between September 1989 and December 1995. Patients first hospitalized outside of Suffolk County or in nonpsychiatric units were not sampled unless they were rehospitalized within 6 months in one of the 12 study sites. Inclusion criteria were age 15–60, first admission either current or within 6 months, clinical evidence of psychosis, the ability to understand assessment procedures in English, IQ higher than 70, and the capacity to provide written informed consent. The study was approved annually by the Stony Brook institutional review board and the institutional review boards of participating hospitals. Written informed consent was obtained. For participants aged 15–17, written consent was obtained from parents and verbal consent was obtained from participants. The response rate for individuals approached for baseline assessment during the recruitment period was 72%.
Initially, the Suffolk County Mental Health Project interviewed 675 individuals. Of these, 628 met the eligibility criteria (22). Figure 1 provides a flow chart of the analysis sample. Among the 628 eligible participants, 511 had one of the three target diagnoses included in this article: schizophrenia spectrum disorder (schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, schizophreniform disorder), major depressive disorder with psychosis, and bipolar disorder with psychosis. Seventy-one patients with psychosis not otherwise specified and 46 individuals with drug-related psychoses were excluded from the current study. Further, 66 individuals did not complete any social functioning assessment, resulting in a final analysis sample of 485 individuals with at least one data point. The 66 dropouts did not differ from the analysis sample in terms of sex, age, or diagnosis (all p>0.05). At the 20-year point, of the 485 included participants, 262 were assessed and 56 had died. Nonresponse was primarily accounted for by refusal to participate and loss to follow-up. Overall, 40.6% of the 485 participants who took part in our study completed all five assessments, 21.2% four, 21.7% three, 10.5% two, and 6.0% one assessment. Attrition within the analysis sample seemed random, that is, the number of assessments was not associated with age, sex, negative symptoms, positive symptoms, employment, public assistance, independent living, homelessness, or baseline diagnosis.
FIGURE 1. Flow Chart of Participants in 20-Year Study of Social Functioning in Patients With Psychotic Disordersa
a Diagnoses were made at the 10-year follow-up point or the last available assessment. The total number of participants with at least one social functioning assessment was 485.
Respondents completed face-to-face interviews at baseline, 6 months, 2 years, 4 years, 10 years, and 20 years. The initial social functioning assessment was taken at 6 months, when the participants were no longer in the hospital. Thus, the 6-month assessment was used as the starting point for the functional trajectories.
To obtain a benchmark for social functioning, a never-psychotic comparison group was recruited at the 20-year time point for respondents living within a 50-mile radius of Stony Brook University. We used a two-step procedure approved by the Stony Brook institutional review board. Step 1, performed by the Stony Brook University Center for Survey Research, involved random digit dialing within zip codes selected in proportion to cases residing there. The goal was to obtain a sample with a similar age and sex distribution and no history of psychosis. The initial number of randomly generated telephone numbers was 12,388. Of these, 2,594 numbers were inactive, 4,321 calls went unanswered, and 4,291 people were ineligible (outside the age/sex target for the zip code or had a psychosis diagnosis or psychiatric hospitalization). Of the eligible households (N=1,182), 750 refused participation and 432 agreed to consider participating in the study and provided a time when they could best be recontacted by study staff.
Step 2, conducted by trained study staff, involved telephone rescreening of the 432 potentially eligible participants. The rescreen included an adaptation of the six-item psychosis screening questionnaire (23) covering visual and auditory hallucinations, thought insertion, paranoia, strange experiences, and diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Twenty individuals could not be reached or were unavailable for rescreening. Of the remaining 412, 58 refused participation, 49 could not be scheduled, and 35 disclosed psychotic symptoms. Of the remaining 270 who participated in the study, eight endorsed psychotic symptoms on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV and were removed from the sample. The final comparison group was composed of 262 participants and was closely matched to the cases on sex (55.9% versus 56.7% male) and age (mean: 50.46 years [SD=9.02] versus 48.14 years [SD=9.14]).

Measures of Social Functioning

The social functioning index was based on a composite of three items relating to relationships and activities with other people from the Heinrichs-Carpenter Quality of Life Scale (24). The scores ranged from 0 (extremely poor) to 6 (satisfactory) for social activity and social sexual relationships and from 1 to 5 for relationships with friends. The Quality of Life Scale is a semistructured interview with multiple probes providing information for each interviewer rating. For example, questions in the “relationships with friends” domain include “Do you have friends with whom you are especially close other than your immediate family or the people you live with?”, “How many friends do you have?”, and “How often have you spoken with them recently, in person or by phone?” Ratings were based on information from the participant, as well as significant others and medical records when available. Information from significant others was available for 66.8% of participants who completed the 6-month assessment and 48.1% of participants who completed the assessment at 20 years. The availability of this information did not differ between classes at any of the time points. Medical records were available for 82.6% of participants at 6 months and 55.3% of participants at 20-year follow-up. At baseline, significantly more records were available for lower-functioning individuals (class 1=92.0%, class 2=84.5%, class 3=83.1%, and class 4=73.1%). There was no difference between classes at 20-year follow-up. The composite score ranged from 1 to 17 and showed acceptable internal reliability at each assessment (α ranged from 0.79 to 0.88).

Premorbid social functioning.

The Premorbid Adjustment Scale (25) was administered at the 6-month follow-up. Ratings were based on a semistructured interview developed to match the Premorbid Adjustment Scale criteria, as well as information obtained from significant others, which was available for 79.6% of participants, and school records, which were available for 63.0% of participants. Overall, 88.5% had additional information to complement scores on the Premorbid Adjustment Scale. Items were rated on a 7-point scale, with 6 reflecting lowest and 0 reflecting highest social functioning. To compare scores on the Premorbid Adjustment Scale and the Quality of Life Scale, items were rescaled so that higher scores indicated better functioning. Three subscales relevant to social contact were included: sociability and social withdrawal (frequency of and interest in social contact), peer relationships (quality of relationships with people of own age), and sociosexual relationships (sexual interest). Here we report the Premorbid Adjustment Scale social functioning scores in childhood (up to age 11), early adolescence (age 12 to 15), and late adolescence (age 15 to 18). Childhood ratings did not include sociosexual relationships. For comparability, we multiplied the childhood score by 1.5.
To equate the metrics of pre- and postadmission functioning, we compared distributions of the late adolescent Premorbid Adjustment Scale scores (ages 15–18) with the Quality of Life Scale scores of participants first assessed before age 19 (N=29), where the scores should be identical if they indeed reflected the same outcome. Distributions of the two composites were largely parallel, but the Premorbid Adjustment Scale scores (mean=13.38; SD=3.35; median=14; 10th percentile=8; 25th percentile=11.5; 75th percentile=16; 90th percentile=18) were around three points higher than the Quality of Life Scale scores (mean=10.78; SD=3.70; median=11; 10th percentile=5; 25th percentile=9; 75th percentile=13; 90th percentile=15). To make the scores on the two scales comparable, we therefore applied a transformation whereby we adjusted the Premorbid Adjustment Scale scores by subtracting 3 points. To avoid confounding of premorbid and postadmission social functioning at 6 months, the Premorbid Adjustment Scale data for those whose age of first admission was <19 years (N=29) were not included in the analyses.

Diagnosis

Face-to-face assessments were conducted by master’s-level mental health professionals at each time point, including the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (26). The assessors were blind to participants’ research diagnoses. However, out of respect to the sample and to maximize the accuracy of information gathered in the interview, raters were asked to review past interview material. Thus, they were aware of the SCID diagnoses (which did not always correspond with the research diagnosis). Primary DSM-IV diagnosis was formulated by consensus of four or more psychiatrists using all available longitudinal information, including SCID interviews, medical records, and information from significant others. We used the last available diagnosis to select the study sample. For the majority of individuals, this was the 10-year follow-up consensus diagnosis. For 91 individuals without a 10-year diagnosis, we substituted the temporally most proximal prior diagnosis.

Symptom Measures

At each time point, symptoms were assessed with the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) (27) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (28), which rate the presence of symptoms on a 6-point scale from absent (score=0) to severe (score=5). The SAPS assesses hallucinations, delusions, bizarre behavior, and thought disorder. We were interested in the SAPS psychosis subscale, a composite of 16 ratings measuring hallucinations and delusions (range=0–80; α internal consistency ranged from 0.81 to 0.89). Factor analysis identified two dimensions within the SANS: inexpressivity and avolition/asociality, which parallel prior findings (29). We were particularly interested in the SANS inexpressivity subscale, a composite of nine items measuring blunted affect and alogia (range=0–45; α ranged from 0.89 to 0.91), because avolition/asociality is conceptually overlapping with social functioning.

Other Functional Outcomes

Other functional outcomes that were assessed in the 20-year follow-up interview were having a high school diploma (yes/no), employment status (being employed, yes/no), homelessness in past 10 years (yes/no), financial independence (receiving public assistance, yes/no), and living independently (own household or not).

Data Analyses

Analyses were conducted in STATA 13 (30) and MPlus version 7.2 (31). Demographic characteristics were compared using regression analyses or chi-square tests.

Functional trajectories.

To examine the trajectories of participant functioning, we conducted latent class growth analyses, a method used to discover subgroups (classes) of individuals following distinct patterns of change over time. In our case, individual class membership was assigned on the basis of social functioning scores from 6 months to 20 years, making use of all available data with maximum likelihood estimation and robust standard errors to account for missing data (i.e., full information maximum likelihood) (31, 32). To determine the appropriate number of latent classes, the analysis is run from a one-class model to increasing numbers of classes. To compare models with the different numbers of classes and determine the optimum model fit, we examined the recommended fit indices: entropy, Akaike’s information criterion, and Bayesian information criterion. Highest entropy and lowest Akaike’s information criterion and Bayesian information criterion suggest the best fit and parsimony of the model (31). Values of 0.4, 0.6, and 0.8 represent low, medium, and high entropy (33). To assess model fit we also consulted the Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin test (in which a significant p value indicates that this model fits significantly better than a model with a lower number of classes [34, 35]). Two piecewise multilevel regression analyses accounting for multiple observations within individuals were conducted to compare the slopes of the four different trajectories from 6 months to 4 years and from 10 to 20 years between classes.

Functional trajectories and diagnosis.

To determine how functional trajectories map onto the current diagnostic classification, we calculated the distribution of schizophrenia spectrum disorder, major depressive disorder with psychosis, and bipolar disorder with psychosis diagnoses across the resulting latent class growth analysis trajectories.

Functional trajectories and premorbid functioning.

Regression analyses and paired t tests were used to examine how the latent class growth analysis trajectories were associated with premorbid functioning (childhood, early and late adolescence), with other 20-year functional outcomes, and with change from premorbid functioning in late adolescence to functioning after illness onset. Overall differences in social functioning at 20-year follow-up between the latent trajectory groups and the comparison group were evaluated with regression and chi-square analyses.

Results

The sample contained 269 participants diagnosed with a schizophrenia spectrum disorder; 76.6% with schizophrenia, 21.9% with schizoaffective disorder, and 1.5% with schizophreniform disorder. Two-thirds (65.8%) of these patients were male, and their mean age at baseline was 29.01 years (SD=8.92, median=28.0). Major depressive disorder with psychosis was the diagnosis for 77 participants, 41.6% of whom were male; their mean age at baseline was 30.81 years (SD=10.84, median=30.0). Bipolar disorder with psychosis was diagnosed in 139 participants, 47.5% of whom were male; their mean age at baseline was 29.18 years (SD=9.81, median=27.0).

Trajectories of Social Functioning in Psychotic Disorders

We selected the four-class model as it performed best on most fit indices (see Table S1 in the data supplement accompanying the online version of this article). The four-class model fit was best on the Akaike’s information criterion and Bayesian information criterion. The Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin test indicated that the fit was significantly better for the four-class than three-class model (p=0.035), but the five-class model did not significantly improve fit. Entropy was medium (0.65) for the four-class model, and mean class probabilities were moderate to high (0.76−0.81), suggesting that with the four-class model individuals were likely to be correctly assigned to a latent class. Information on clinical symptoms and antipsychotic treatment by trajectory class is presented in Table 1.
TABLE 1. Relation of Symptoms and Medication to 20-Year Social Functioning Trajectory in Patients With Psychotic Disorders
VariableaSocial Functioning ClassScore or PercentTukey GroupingbAnalysis
  MeanSD  
SANS inexpressivity subscale, 6 monthsc(1) Profoundly impaired12.269.50A   t=–6.90, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired9.758.95 B  
(3) Moderately impaired5.536.39  C 
(4) Preserved1.443.24   D
SANS inexpressivity subscale, 2 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired12.429.01A   t=–8.02, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired8.318.23 B  
(3) Moderately impaired3.805.32  C 
(4) Preserved1.001.69   D
SANS inexpressivity subscale, 4 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired12.129.86A   t=–6.30, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired7.938.33 B  
(3) Moderately impaired3.766.17  C 
(4) Preserved0.722.24  C 
SANS inexpressivity subscale, 10 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired10.269.56A   t=–4.02, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired7.177.95A   
(3) Moderately impaired3.415.61 B  
(4) Preserved1.293.22 B  
SANS inexpressivity subscale, 20 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired14.7010.59A   t=–4.70, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired9.249.93 B  
(3) Moderately impaired6.198.42 BC 
(4) Preserved2.554.02  C 
SAPS, 6 monthsc(1) Profoundly impaired5.738.90A   t=–4.66, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired4.467.47A   
(3) Moderately impaired1.513.20 B  
(4) Preserved0.632.45 B  
SAPS, 2 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired4.566.62A   t=–2.33, p=0.020
(2) Severely impaired4.136.34 B  
(3) Moderately impaired2.415.19A   
(4) Preserved0.893.91A   
SAPS, 4 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired4.206.90A   t=–1.99, p=0.048
(2) Severely impaired3.806.97A   
(3) Moderately impaired1.984.35A   
(4) Preserved0.822.53A   
SAPS, 10 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired6.288.43A   t=–2.02, p=0.044
(2) Severely impaired6.369.93 B  
(3) Moderately impaired3.256.76A   
(4) Preserved0.421.43A   
SAPS, 20 yearsc(1) Profoundly impaired8.588.99A   t=–3.48, p=0.001
(2) Severely impaired6.5210.16A   
(3) Moderately impaired2.804.99 B  
(4) Preserved0.311.00 B  
  % TimeN  
Antipsychotic use, baseline to 6 monthsd(1) Profoundly impaired85.163A   t=–3.75, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired78.3114A   
(3) Moderately impaired75.0111A   
(4) Preserved58.548 B  
Antipsychotic use, 6 months to 2 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired79.558A   t=–6.25, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired65.2116 B  
(3) Moderately impaired52.777  C 
(4) Preserved36.630   D
Antipsychotic use, 2–4 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired74.054A   t=–7.35, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired59.3105 B  
(3) Moderately impaired44.265  C 
(4) Preserved24.420   D
Antipsychotic use, 10 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired87.750A   t=–7.36, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired72.0103 B  
(3) Moderately impaired58.963  C 
(4) Preserved31.219   D
Antipsychotic use, 20 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired77.828A   t=–6.46, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired73.965A   
(3) Moderately impaired56.250 B  
(4) Preserved26.414  C 
a
SANS, Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. SAPS, Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms.
b
Means with the same letter are not significantly different.
c
All SANS inexpressivity and SAPS comparisons are controlled for diagnosis, age, and sex.
d
All analyses of antipsychotic use are controlled for gender and age. Antipsychotic use for baseline to 6 months, 6 months to 2 years, and 2–4 years reflects the percentage time of use between the two time points first (25% cutoff). Antipsychotic use at 10 years and 20 years reflects use at time of assessment (25% cutoff).
Figure 2 and Table 2 present the social functioning trajectories from 6 months to 20 years. The classes represented groups whose social functioning was categorized as profoundly impaired (class 1; N=74; 1st percentile of comparison group sample distribution), severely impaired (class 2; N=181; 3rd percentile), moderately impaired (N=148; 17th percentile), and preserved (N=82; 59th percentile). Piecewise multilevel regression analyses were conducted to compare the slopes of the trajectories from 6 months to 4 years and from 10 to 20 years among classes. The results of the first analysis showed a significant effect of class (B=3.55, SE=0.12, p<0.001) and time point (B=0.54, SE=0.23, p<0.05) but no significant interaction. The second analysis from 10 to 20 years only revealed a significant class effect (B=3.49, SE=0.89, p<0.001). The trajectories of the four classes were largely parallel, differing in degree of severity but not in shape. At the 20-year time point, the trajectories of the profoundly (B=−8.61, SE=0.55, p<0.001), severely (B=−6.54, SE=0.38, p<0.001), and moderately (B=−3.02, SE=0.37, p<0.001) impaired groups showed significantly worse social functioning than the comparison group. There was no significant difference in 20-year social functioning between those in the preserved functioning class (B=0.81, SE=0.45, p=0.07) and individuals in the comparison group (mean=14.17, SD=2.74).
FIGURE 2. Social Functioning Trajectories Across Psychotic Disorders, Derived From Latent Class Growth Analysesa
a See text for description of social functioning index. Error bars represent standard deviations.
TABLE 2. Social Functioning at Individual Time Points in Relation to 20-Year Trajectory in Individuals With Psychotic Disorders
VariableSocial Functioning ClassaMeanSDTukey GroupingbAnalysis
Premorbid Adjustment Scale, childhoodc(1) Profoundly impaired8.123.42A   t=5.62, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired8.773.78AB  
(3) Moderately impaired9.414.10 B  
(4) Preserved12.003.13  C 
Premorbid Adjustment Scale, adolescencec(1) Profoundly impaired8.253.93A   t=6.38, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired9.073.54A   
(3) Moderately impaired10.283.39 B  
(4) Preserved12.362.69  C 
Premorbid Adjustment Scale, late adolescencec(1) Profoundly impaired7.754.25A   t=5.67, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired9.233.88 B  
(3) Moderately impaired10.443.52 B  
(4) Preserved13.032.54  C 
Social functioning, 6 monthsd(1) Profoundly impaired4.372.60A   t=21.81, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired7.602.79 B  
(3) Moderately impaired10.902.96  C 
(4) Preserved15.111.93   D
Social functioning, 2 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired4.052.03A   t=27.65, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired8.622.33 B  
(3) Moderately impaired11.852.33  C 
(4) Preserved15.431.86   D
Social functioning, 4 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired5.032.08A   t=24.81, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired8.522.58 B  
(3) Moderately impaired12.352.39  C 
(4) Preserved15.281.76   D
Social functioning, 10 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired5.352.71A   t=14.03, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired7.603.65 B  
(3) Moderately impaired11.783.47  C 
(4) Preserved15.052.38   D
Social functioning, 20 yearsd(1) Profoundly impaired5.563.51A   t=13.18, p<0.001
(2) Severely impaired7.643.32 B  
(3) Moderately impaired11.163.66  C 
(4) Preserved14.982.24   D
a
Number of participants per class: class 1=74, class 2=181, class 3=148, class 4=82.
b
Means with the same letter are not significantly different.
c
All analyses of Premorbid Adjustment Scale scores are controlled for diagnosis and sex.
d
See text for description of social functioning index. All analyses of social functioning are controlled for diagnosis, sex, and age.

Characteristics of Social Functioning Trajectory Groups

Trajectories and diagnosis.

The distribution of the three diagnostic groups varied widely across the trajectory classes (χ2=171.26, df=6, p<0.001; see Figure 2), showing that there is substantial individual variation in social functioning within each of the three disorders.

Trajectories and premorbid functioning.

Figure 2 also demonstrates the association of the social functioning trajectories with premorbid social development. The two main findings are that, at the group level, differences in social functioning among the four classes are already evident in childhood and that those with worse social functioning in childhood experience a larger decline in social functioning from the Premorbid Adjustment Scale scores in adolescence to the Quality of Life Scale scores 6 months after first admission. This decline from premorbid to postmorbid functioning was significant in the two lowest classes, profoundly and severely impaired functioning (class 1: mean difference=–4.18, SD=3.96, t=7.40, df=48, p<0.001; class 2: mean difference=−1.87, SD=3.95, t=5.48, df=133, p<0.001). Functioning in the moderately impaired class, class 3, remained stable (mean difference=0.34, SD=3.90, t=–0.86, df=100, p=0.39). In line with normal developmental changes, there was a significant improvement in the level of social functioning from premorbid to postmorbid functioning in class 4 (mean difference=2.20, SD=3.04, t=–5.56, df=58, p<0.001).

Trajectories and 20-year functional outcomes.

Table 3 presents the associations of the social functioning trajectories with demographic variables and outcomes at year 20. The trajectories of profoundly (class 1) and severely (class 2) impaired social functioning were associated with worse 20-year real-life functional outcomes in a variety of domains, such as not having obtained a high school diploma, unemployment, not living independently, and the use of public assistance. The trajectories for the moderately impaired (class 3) and preserved (class 4) functional groups differed from each other only in independent living and public assistance.
TABLE 3. Relation of Characteristics and Outcomes to 20-Year Social Functioning Trajectory for Individuals With Psychotic Disorders
VariableSocial Functioning ClassMean or ProportionTukey GroupingaAnalysis
  MeanSD  
Age at onset (years)bProfoundly impaired30.1516.61A   t=–1.65, p=0.10
Severely impaired29.7214.29A   
Moderately impaired28.6413.11A   
Preserved29.8011.16A   
Age at baseline (years)bProfoundly impaired30.329.43A   t=–1.79, p=0.074
Severely impaired29.519.22A   
Moderately impaired28.118.95A   
Preserved30.3010.99A   
  N%  
Baseline or 6-month characteristics        
MaleProfoundly impaired5371.6A   χ2=27.06, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired11764.7A   
Moderately impaired7550.7 B  
Preserved3036.6  C 
White/CaucasianProfoundly impaired4864.8AB  χ2=23.74, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired12669.9A   
Moderately impaired11980.4 BC 
Preserved7692.7  C 
Unemployed, 6 monthsProfoundly impaired4876.1A   χ2=88.03, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired10664.2A   
Moderately impaired4935.5 B  
Preserved810.7  C 
Public assistance, 6 monthsProfoundly impaired3047.6A   χ2=41.08, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired7646.3A   
Moderately impaired3323.9 B  
Preserved810.7  C 
Independent living, 6 monthsProfoundly impaired1320.6A   χ2=26.04, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired4426.8A   
Moderately impaired6244.6 B  
Preserved4052.6 B  
Homelessness, baselinecProfoundly impaired1425.0A   χ2=7.47, df=3, p=0.06
Severely impaired3626.5A   
Moderately impaired2218.6A   
Preserved710.8A   
20-year outcomes        
No diplomaProfoundly impaired79.5AB  χ2=11.78, df=3, p<0.01
Severely impaired1910.5A   
Moderately impaired53.4 BC 
Preserved11.2  C 
UnemployedProfoundly impaired3597.2A   χ2=46.36, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired7280.0 B  
Moderately impaired4752.8  C 
Preserved2240.0  C 
Public assistanceProfoundly impaired3494.4A   χ2=62.83, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired7785.6A   
Moderately impaired4955.1 B  
Preserved1730.9  C 
Independent livingProfoundly impaired1443.8A   χ2=30.77, df=3, p<0.001
Severely impaired4648.9A   
Moderately impaired6368.5 B  
Preserved4790.4  C 
HomelessnesscProfoundly impaired616.7A   χ2=0.22, df=3, p=0.98
Severely impaired1314.8A   
Moderately impaired1213.5A   
Preserved814.5A   
a
Means with the same letter are not significantly different.
b
Analyses of age are controlled for diagnosis and sex.
c
Homelessness rating is based on any time in lifetime before baseline and any time between 10 and 20 years.

Discussion

Psychotic disorders are associated with profound social impairments (32, 33). It is often implicitly assumed that these impairments fluctuate and that the course of social functioning is worse in schizophrenia compared with affective psychotic disorders (34). However, only limited research had directly addressed cross-diagnostic and individual variation in patients’ social outcomes over time.
Our study went beyond investigations that considered individual disorders by examining latent trajectories in the 20-year course of social functioning across three broad psychotic disorder groups. Using latent growth curve modeling, we detected four remarkably stable trajectories of preserved, moderately, severely, and profoundly impaired social functioning. Interestingly, our findings reveal that more than one of these classes were found in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, psychotic bipolar disorder, and psychotic depression.
In addition, our findings suggest that differences in the level of social functioning among these 20-year trajectories are already evident in childhood. The years between early adolescence and first hospitalization appear to be a period in which a substantial number of individuals who later develop a psychotic disorder display a steep decline in social functioning. This extends the findings of earlier research that investigated social functioning within diagnostic categories by showing not only that premorbid adjustment is a strong predictor of social functioning over the 3 years following illness onset in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (35), but that premorbid adjustment also predicts social outcome for patients with bipolar disorder with psychosis and major depressive disorder with psychosis. Besides, the level of social functioning after the acute illness phase in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, bipolar disorder with psychosis, and major depressive disorder with psychosis turned out to be relatively stable (12, 15, 36, 37).
Particularly the two lower social functioning trajectories were associated with other unfavorable psychosocial outcomes at 20-year follow-up. This suggests that social functioning is a valuable indicator of long-term outcome and that it may be an important treatment target in psychotic disorders that could lead to improvements in other areas of functioning. It also shows the value of a recovery-oriented perspective of mental health services, in the sense of helping patients to formulate adjusted but meaningful (social) goals (38).
In sum, the current findings expand existing knowledge on social functioning in psychotic disorders by showing that severe and persistent social impairment preceded by a drop in social functioning in adolescence is common in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (75%), but it is not limited to the schizophrenia spectrum, because it is also present in about 35% of participants with major depressive disorder with psychosis and about 18% of those having bipolar disorder with psychosis. On the other hand, a substantial number of individuals with bipolar disorder with psychosis (42%) and major depressive disorder with psychosis (26%), but hardly any individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (1.5%), achieved levels of social functioning after illness onset that were similar to that of the comparison group. Our results suggest that, at the group level, the trajectories of social functioning do not exhibit marked changes after illness onset (e.g., showing improvement or deterioration), as previously suggested (39, 40). Whereas small improvements in social functioning are visible in all classes in the first years after onset, the overall trajectories follow comparable, rather stable courses, which are mostly characterized by differences in severity. These differences are also reflected by differences in medication intake: the more severe the social impairment, the higher the antipsychotic medication intake. This finding, of course, does not imply causality (arguably, it may be that both antipsychotic use and social impairment are the direct consequences of symptom severity), yet it would be interesting to investigate the effect of prolonged medication on real-life outcomes.
Our findings are in line with those of the FUNCAP study, wherein real-world outcomes and their determinants were examined in individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Also, social impairment was found to be more prominent but not limited to schizophrenia (41, 42). Those results also provided important etiological clues, suggesting that social functioning in both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder seems largely driven by performance on functional capacity measures (measuring the capacity to perform everyday tasks, such as communication skills needed in daily interactions). Although this hypothesis needs further testing, it may explain at least part of our findings and suggests that similar pathways to poor social functioning apply across mental disorders.
Of interest is our finding that, in contrast to research that compared patients diagnosed with major depression versus bipolar disorder without psychosis (43), Suffolk County participants with bipolar disorder had consistently better outcomes than individuals with psychotic depression. A potential explanation is that psychotic depression is a more severe illness than major depressive disorder without psychosis, which is the majority of what was examined in prior comparisons.
Our results should be interpreted in light of the following limitations. First, the Suffolk County project provided a unique opportunity to prospectively follow up a large sample for two decades; however, the gaps between the later follow-up assessments were large (6 and 10 years, respectively) and may have overlooked short-term changes in social functioning. Second, premorbid functioning was assessed retrospectively, which may limit the reliability of these data. We sought to mitigate this issue by integrating participant data with information from family members and school records. Third, critical data on factors that might more directly influence unfavorable social outcomes in people with psychosis, such as social-cognitive ability; effects of early social modeling from parents, relatives, and friends; and idiographic experiences (early social reinforcement and social rejection), was not available, and we were therefore not able to perform analyses of the potential determinants of poor functional outcome. Fourth, raters were aware of previous SCID diagnoses, which might be a source of bias. However, raters were unaware of both the study diagnosis (decided by study psychiatrists) and hypotheses of the current study, and social functioning was not a primary target of the study. Fifth, our focus was to investigate associations of social functioning trajectories with other 20-year outcomes; however, in order to assess the value of social functioning in relation to other real-world outcomes, it will be important to establish experimentally whether improvement in social functioning (e.g., with treatment) can indeed lead to other favorable outcomes and to determine whether trajectories of functioning in other domains (e.g., employment, life satisfaction) are parallel to the social functioning trajectories. The current sample had no systematic treatment aimed at social functioning, and future studies should examine how specific treatment might influence social functioning in the long run. Finally, latent class growth curve analysis offers a powerful method for studying between-person differences in longitudinal change. However, because it models a single trajectory for all members of a class (35), we may have missed patterns where a few individuals show greater change than the rest of the class. Importantly, our results show large individual variation within groups (as indicated by the error bars in Figure 2) and do not allow for conclusions about individual outcomes.

Clinical Implications

Persistent impairments observed in approximately half of the sample emphasize the need for targeted, long-term care aimed at improving social inclusion for those with low social functioning at illness onset. Our findings indicate that 53% of the patients decline markedly in their social functioning between late adolescence and first hospitalization, a finding that has been supported by two other studies using latent class growth curve analysis (44, 45). This and the high temporal stability of the trajectories extend previous findings suggesting that the level of social functioning may be determined in adolescence. Consequently, our findings are consistent with recent programs of research focused on adolescence as the critical intervention window and support current early intervention strategies for high-risk individuals (46) and those that offer intensive treatment to first-admission patients (47) aimed to prevent social withdrawal in severe psychotic illnesses.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the mental health professionals in Suffolk County, the project psychiatrists and staff, and the study participants and their families and friends. The project psychiatrists and staff received their usual salary support; no others were paid for their services.

Footnote

The National Institutes of Health and the Netherland Organization for Scientific Research had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2016.15111419.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, et al: The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011; 35:573–588
2.
Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, et al: Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:119–136
3.
Harvey PD: Disability in schizophrenia: contributing factors and validated assessments. J Clin Psychiatry 2014; 75(Suppl 1):15–20
4.
Bellack AS, Green MF, Cook JA, et al: Assessment of community functioning in people with schizophrenia and other severe mental illnesses: a white paper based on an NIMH-sponsored workshop. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:805–822
5.
Charzyńska K, Kucharska K, Mortimer A: Does employment promote the process of recovery from schizophrenia? A review of the existing evidence. Int J Occup Med Environ Health 2015; 28:407–418
6.
Liberman RP, Gutkind D, Mintz J, et al: Impact of risperidone versus haloperidol on activities of daily living in the treatment of refractory schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 2002; 43:469–473
7.
Green MF, Hellemann G, Horan WP, et al: From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69:1216–1224
8.
Burns T, Patrick D: Social functioning as an outcome measure in schizophrenia studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2007; 116:403–418
9.
Rabinowitz J, Reichenberg A, Weiser M, et al: Cognitive and behavioural functioning in men with schizophrenia both before and shortly after first admission to hospital. Cross-sectional analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177:26–32
10.
Harrow M, Sands JR, Silverstein ML, et al: Course and outcome for schizophrenia versus other psychotic patients: a longitudinal study. Schizophr Bull 1997; 23:287–303
11.
Robinson DG, Woerner MG, McMeniman M, et al: Symptomatic and functional recovery from a first episode of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:473–479
12.
Wiersma D, Wanderling J, Dragomirecka E, et al: Social disability in schizophrenia: its development and prediction over 15 years in incidence cohorts in six European centres. Psychol Med 2000; 30:1155–1167
13.
Strauss GP, Harrow M, Grossman LS, et al: Periods of recovery in deficit syndrome schizophrenia: a 20-year multi-follow-up longitudinal study. Schizophr Bull 2010; 36:788–799
14.
Häfner H, Nowotny B, Löffler W, et al: When and how does schizophrenia produce social deficits? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995; 246:17–28
15.
Furukawa TA, Azuma H, Takeuchi H, et al: 10-year course of social adjustment in major depression. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2011; 57:501–508
16.
Judd LL, Schettler PJ, Solomon DA, et al: Psychosocial disability and work role function compared across the long-term course of bipolar I, bipolar II and unipolar major depressive disorders. J Affect Disord 2008; 108:49–58
17.
Tohen M, Strakowski SM, Zarate C Jr, et al: The McLean-Harvard first-episode project: 6-month symptomatic and functional outcome in affective and nonaffective psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:467–476
18.
Harrow M, Grossman LS, Jobe TH, et al: Do patients with schizophrenia ever show periods of recovery? A 15-year multi-follow-up study. Schizophr Bull 2005; 31:723–734
19.
Dickerson FB, Sommerville J, Origoni AE, et al: Outpatients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder: Do they differ in their cognitive and social functioning? Psychiatry Res 2001; 102:21–27
20.
Green MF, Llerena K, Kern RS: The “right stuff” revisited: what have we learned about the determinants of daily functioning in schizophrenia? Schizophr Bull 2015; 41:781–785
21.
Bromet EJ, Kotov R, Fochtmann LJ, et al: Diagnostic shifts during the decade following first admission for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168:1186–1194
22.
Kotov R, Leong SH, Mojtabai R, et al: Boundaries of schizoaffective disorder: revisiting Kraepelin. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:1276–1286
23.
Bebbington PNT: The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 1995; 5:11–19
24.
Heinrichs DW, Hanlon TE, Carpenter WT Jr: The Quality of Life Scale: an instrument for rating the schizophrenic deficit syndrome. Schizophr Bull 1984; 10:388–398
25.
Cannon-Spoor HE, Potkin SG, Wyatt RJ: Measurement of premorbid adjustment in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1982; 8:470–484
26.
First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, et al: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID), Clinician Version. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1997
27.
Andreasen NC: The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984
28.
Andreasen NC: The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1983
29.
Blanchard JJ, Cohen AS: The structure of negative symptoms within schizophrenia: implications for assessment. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:238–245
30.
StataCorp: Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College Station, Tex, StataCorp, 2013
31.
Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 7th ed. Los Angeles, Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2012 (https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/MplusUserGuideVer_7.pdf)
32.
Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J: Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015; 16:620–631
33.
Harvey PD, Sabbag S, Prestia D, et al: Functional milestones and clinician ratings of everyday functioning in people with schizophrenia: overlap between milestones and specificity of ratings. J Psychiatr Res 2012; 46:1546–1552
34.
Jobe TH, Harrow M: Long-term outcome of patients with schizophrenia: a review. Can J Psychiatry 2005; 50:892–900
35.
Bailer J, Bräuer W, Rey ER: Premorbid adjustment as predictor of outcome in schizophrenia: results of a prospective study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1996; 93:368–377
36.
Harrow M, Jobe TH: Factors involved in outcome and recovery in schizophrenia patients not on antipsychotic medications: a 15-year multifollow-up study. J Nerv Ment Dis 2007; 195:406–414
37.
MacQueen GM, Young LT, Joffe RT: A review of psychosocial outcome in patients with bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2001; 103:163–170
38.
Yarborough BJ, Yarborough MT, Janoff SL, et al: Getting by, getting back, and getting on: matching mental health services to consumers’ recovery goals. Psychiatr Rehabil J 2016; 39:97–104
39.
Rietschel M, Georgi A, Schmael C, et al: Premorbid adjustment: a phenotype highlighting a distinction rather than an overlap between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res 2009; 110:33–39
40.
Uzelac S, Jaeger J, Berns S, et al: Premorbid adjustment in bipolar disorder: comparison with schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 2006; 194:654–658
41.
Mausbach BT, Harvey PD, Pulver AE, et al: Relationship of the Brief UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment (UPSA-B) to multiple indicators of functioning in people with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 2010; 12:45–55
42.
Bowie CR, Depp C, McGrath JA, et al: Prediction of real-world functional disability in chronic mental disorders: a comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:1116–1124
43.
Mehta S, Mittal PK, Swami MK: Psychosocial functioning in depressive patients: a comparative study between major depressive disorder and bipolar affective disorder. Depress Res Treat 2014; article 302741 (https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/302741)
44.
Cole VT, Apud JA, Weinberger DR, et al: Using latent class growth analysis to form trajectories of premorbid adjustment in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 2012; 121:388–395
45.
Hodgekins J, Birchwood M, Christopher R, et al: Investigating trajectories of social recovery in individuals with first-episode psychosis: a latent class growth analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2015; 207:536–543
46.
Fusar-Poli P, Yung AR, McGorry P, et al: Lessons learned from the psychosis high-risk state: towards a general staging model of prodromal intervention. Psychol Med 2014; 44:17–24
47.
Kane JM, Schooler NR, Marcy P, et al: The RAISE early treatment program for first-episode psychosis: background, rationale, and study design. J Clin Psychiatry 2015; 76:240–246

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1075 - 1085
PubMed: 27978770

History

Received: 10 November 2015
Revision received: 17 June 2016
Revision received: 17 August 2016
Revision received: 28 September 2016
Accepted: 6 October 2016
Published online: 16 December 2016
Published in print: November 01, 2017

Keywords

  1. Psychosis
  2. Schizophrenia
  3. Mood Disorders-Bipolar
  4. Mood Disorders-Unipolar
  5. Outcome Studies
  6. Social Functioning

Authors

Details

Eva Velthorst, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Preventive Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York; the Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Educational Neuroscience and Research Institute LEARN! Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, N.Y.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
Anne-Kathrin J. Fett, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Preventive Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York; the Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Educational Neuroscience and Research Institute LEARN! Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, N.Y.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
Avraham Reichenberg, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Preventive Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York; the Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Educational Neuroscience and Research Institute LEARN! Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, N.Y.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
Greg Perlman, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Preventive Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York; the Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Educational Neuroscience and Research Institute LEARN! Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, N.Y.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
Jim van Os, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Preventive Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York; the Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Educational Neuroscience and Research Institute LEARN! Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, N.Y.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
Evelyn J. Bromet, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Preventive Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York; the Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Educational Neuroscience and Research Institute LEARN! Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, N.Y.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
Roman Kotov, Ph.D. [email protected]
From the Department of Psychiatry and the Department of Preventive Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York; the Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Educational Neuroscience and Research Institute LEARN! Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; the Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, U.K.; the Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, N.Y.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, the Netherlands.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Kotov ([email protected]).

Author Contributions

Dr. Velthorst and Dr. Fett contributed equally to the article and are joint first authors.

Competing Interests

All authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

National Institutes of Health10.13039/100000002: MH094398, MH44801
Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek10.13039/501100003246: 451-13-035, 916-15-005
Dr. Kotov and Dr. Bromet received grant support from NIMH (MH094398 and MH44801, respectively). Dr. Velthorst received support from Netherland Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) VENI Grant 916-15-005. Dr. Fett received support from NWO VENI Grant 451-13-035 and a 2015 NARSAD Young Investigator Award from the Brain and Behavior Foundation. Dr. Perlman was supported by a 2013 NARSAD Young Investigator Award from the Brain and Behavior Foundation.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share