Skip to main content
Full access
Reviews and Overviews
Published Online: 14 November 2016

Fetal Origins of Mental Health: The Developmental Origins of Health and Disease Hypothesis

Abstract

The quality of fetal growth and development predicts the risk for a range of noncommunicable, chronic illnesses. These observations form the basis of the “developmental origins of health and disease” hypothesis, which suggests that the intrauterine signals that compromise fetal growth also act to “program” tissue differentiation in a manner that predisposes to later illness. Fetal growth also predicts the risk for later psychopathology. These findings parallel studies showing that antenatal maternal emotional well-being likewise predicts the risk for later psychopathology. Taken together, these findings form the basis for integrative models of fetal neurodevelopment, which propose that antenatal maternal adversity operates through the biological pathways associated with fetal growth to program neurodevelopment. The authors review the literature and find little support for such integrated models. Maternal anxiety, depression, and stress all influence neurodevelopment but show modest, weak, or no associations with known stress mediators (e.g., glucocorticoids) or with fetal growth. Rather, compromised fetal development appears to establish a “meta-plastic” state that increases sensitivity to postnatal influences. There also remain serious concerns that observational studies associating either fetal growth or maternal mental health with neurodevelopmental outcomes fail to account for underlying genetic factors. Finally, while the observed relation between fetal growth and adult health has garnered considerable attention, the clinical relevance of these associations remains to be determined. There are both considerable promise and important challenges for future studies of the fetal origins of mental health.
The association between birth weight and cardiometabolic health catalyzed interest in the fetal origins of noncommunicable disease (1, 2). Importantly, the relation between birth weight and metabolic health is broadly continuous: the risk of metabolic disease declines with increasing birth weight up to the point of macrosomia, where it once again increases. More successful pregnancies (i.e., optimal fetal growth) are associated with better metabolic health. These studies (1, 2) contributed to the emphasis on maternal health as a global priority for the World Health Organization (http://www.who.int/pmnch) and the International Monetary Fund (http://www.imf.org/external/np/exr/facts/mdg.htm).
Studies of fetal growth and adult health outcomes spawned the idea that intrauterine signals “program” the development of tissue function in a manner that predisposes to specific health outcomes. This idea is framed as the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) hypothesis, which suggests that the quality of fetal development shapes individual differences in the risk for chronic illness over the lifespan. The DOHaD theme is familiar in psychiatry/psychology, which emphasizes the importance of early developmental influences as determinants of mental health and human capacity. DOHaD studies reflect the importance of antenatal maternal well-being and fetal growth for individual differences in vulnerability to adverse mental health outcomes, which could inform precision medicine and intervention programs in psychiatry (3). We briefly review the evidence for the importance of the antenatal period for neurodevelopment outcomes and suggest challenges for future research, including studies that enable the integration of findings from DOHaD studies into clinical practice and public policy.

Fetal Origins of Brain Health

Fetal Growth and Psychopathology

Detailed information on fetal growth, such as that derived from serial ultrasound scans, is difficult to obtain. Epidemiological studies instead use proxy measures, such as birth weight, gestational age, and, less often, birth length, head circumference, placental weight, or ponderal index (4). We focus here on birth weight, which is the most commonly studied measure of fetal development, but it is important to note that birth weight, gestational age, and birth length may reflect different underlying mechanisms with independent effects on specific mental health outcomes.
Fetal growth, measured as birth weight corrected for gestational age, predicts the risk for later psychopathology (49). The evidence is particularly strong for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (69). The risk for ADHD is significantly increased among children whose birth weight is <2.5 kg (6, 7). The relation cuts across the normal range for birth weight. A one-standard-deviation increase in fetal growth is associated with a 14% decrease in the risk for childhood behavioral problems, including ADHD symptoms (8). And like metabolic dysfunction, the risk for ADHD increases with macrosomia (9). More precise phenotyping reveals that low birth weight predicts impairments of executive function, including attention and working memory (10). Birth weight is associated with IQ as well as educational achievement (10). Birth weight is also linked to the frequency and severity of depressive symptoms (e.g., 11) and measures of negative emotional reactivity in childhood that presages depression (fearful, distress, sadness, anger) (4, 5). And there is evidence for an association between birth weight and psychosis-like symptoms (12), consistent with reports associating obstetric outcomes with the risk for schizophrenia.
In sum, epidemiological studies with clinical and community samples reveal associations between fetal growth and a range of mental health outcomes, with little evidence for specificity. Indeed, analysis of Scandinavian registries shows that low birth weight (<2.5 kg) predicts an increased risk for schizophrenia, alcohol and drug use disorders, anxiety disorders, and somatoform disorders (13), even after controlling for socioeconomic status.

Antenatal Maternal Mood and Psychopathology

Antenatal maternal emotional well-being affects neurodevelopmental outcomes in the offspring (1417). Antenatal maternal symptoms of anxiety or depression are associated with more difficult/reactive infant temperament (1719), independent of postnatal maternal mental health (18, 19). This finding suggests that effects are not merely due to genetic transmission. Indeed, the outcomes of maternal mental health are commonly moderated by offspring, but not maternal genotype (2022). Finally, community samples reveal associations between self-reported symptoms of depression or anxiety and neurodevelopment suggesting that effects of antenatal maternal mood on child outcome occur across the spectrum of symptom severity (1416, 23) and are not unique to clinical samples. This point underscores the importance of maternal mental health as a population health issue.
The impact of antenatal maternal stress on neurodevelopmental outcomes is well established in rodent and nonhuman primate models (reviewed in references 24, 25). These studies benefit from controls for postnatal influences, including cross-fostering at birth to nonstressed mothers. The offspring of mothers exposed to prenatal stress show increased fearfulness, heightened hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) responses to stress, and cognitive impairments, most notably in executive functions. There is evidence for comparable effects in humans. Studies of children whose mothers were exposed to extreme forms of stress reveal the impact of prenatal stress on cognitive and socioemotional function (14), including increased levels of fearfulness. While most such studies use global screening measures, there is evidence for effects of maternal stress on executive function as well as on learning and memory (14). There is nevertheless a clear need for studies using “deep” phenotyping to identify the specific functional domains that underlie the association between various forms of maternal adversity and specific mental health outcomes.

Antenatal “Maternal” Influences on Neurodevelopment

Birth Weight and Brain Structure

Only a few studies have examined neural structure in relation to birth weight across the normal range. Birth weight is associated with cortical surface area, but not thickness, in adulthood (e.g., 26) and is modestly associated with adult hippocampal volume, more strongly in women (e.g., 27). Thus, despite the strong evidence for an association between variation in birth weight across the normal range and mental health outcomes, there is a paucity of studies examining potential neural mechanisms.

Antenatal Maternal Influences on Brain Structure

Antenatal maternal mood is associated with structural variation in the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex. After controlling for postnatal maternal mood, infants born to mothers with higher antenatal depressive symptom ratings show greater functional connectivity of the amygdala with the temporal cortex and insula, as well as the left and right anterior cingulate, medial orbitofrontal, and ventromedial prefrontal cortices, which is consistent with patterns of connectivity observed in major depressive disorder (28). Antenatal maternal depressive symptoms are also associated with cortical thinning, primarily in the right frontal lobes in children (29). Cortical thinning is a proposed endophenotype for depression and mediates, in part, the relationship between antenatal maternal mood and externalizing behaviors. Antenatal maternal anxiety is associated with gray matter volume reductions in the prefrontal cortex, the medial temporal lobe, the lateral temporal cortex, and the postcentral gyrus as well as the cerebellum extending to the middle occipital gyrus and the fusiform gyrus (30).
An issue for studies of fetal neurodevelopment is that outcome measures are often collected well after birth. Studies of fetal development using measures of cardiac function or activity reveal clear effects of maternal emotional well-being on fetal physiology (31, 32). While imaging studies of older children suggest prenatal influences on brain structure (e.g., 30), there remains the possibility that these effects are confounded by postnatal experience. Recent studies that overcome this complication by imaging shortly after birth (22, 33) reveal that antenatal maternal anxiety predicts variation in microstructure of regions important for cognitive-emotional function (right insula and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), sensory processing (right middle occipital cortex), and socioemotional function (right angular gyrus, uncinate fasciculus, posterior cingulate, and parahippocampus). These right lateralized clusters predict infant internalizing behavior.
In summary, maternal antenatal mood and variations in birth weight are associated with structure and connectivity in corticolimbic regions associated with affective disorders. However, this conclusion is based on a small number of cross-sectional studies; there is a clear need for longitudinal analysis and studies of mechanism.

Do Maternal Glucocorticoid Levels Link Maternal Adversity, Impaired Fetal Growth, and Altered Neurodevelopment?

Antenatal treatment with the highly catabolic glucocorticoids reduces birth weight (34, 35). This glucocorticoid effect is most pronounced in late pregnancy, when fetal growth is accelerated. Cortisol levels in cord blood are increased in intrauterine growth retardation, implicating endogenous cortisol in fetal growth. Glucocorticoid levels normally rise over pregnancy, and glucocorticoid receptors are highly expressed in the placenta, mediating metabolic and anti-inflammatory effects. While lipophilic steroids easily cross the placenta, fetal glucocorticoid levels are much lower than levels in maternal circulation (36) because of placental 11β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 (11β-HSD-2), which converts active glucocorticoids (cortisol and corticosterone) to inert 11-keto forms (cortisone, 11-dehydrocorticosterone) (34, 35). Placental 11β-HSD-2 thus forms a fetal barrier to maternal glucocorticoids. A deficiency of 11β-HSD-2 leads to overexposure of the fetus to cortisol and lower birth weight.
Placental 11β-HSD-2 is dynamically regulated by proinflammatory cytokines, malnutrition, and maternal stress or anxiety (35). Studies of 11β-HSD-2 null mice provide evidence for a causal association between 11β-HSD-2, reduced birth weight, and anxiety-like behavior in adulthood (37). This finding is consistent with findings that antenatal maternal stress affects neurodevelopment (14, 25). Studies in rodents and nonhuman primates show comparable effects of antenatal glucocorticoid exposure, suggesting that the neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal stress may be mediated by increased glucocorticoid action (24). In support of this idea, maternal cortisol levels predict right amygdala volume in childhood (38) as well as emotional problems that are mediated in part by amygdala volume. Likewise, antenatal maternal cortisol levels are associated with temperament in offspring (19).

Summary

Glucocorticoids are associated with variation in fetal growth and are implicated as mediators of the association between maternal adversity and offspring development. Comprehensive reviews provide working models unifying the findings from studies of fetal growth with those of both maternal stress and mental health (e.g., 39). These models suggest that multiple forms of maternal adversity, including malnutrition, stressful events, and increased symptoms of anxiety or depression, activate “stress mediators,” notably glucocorticoids and proinflammatory cytokines that regulate birth outcomes and, in parallel, stably “program” neural systems that underlie cognitive-emotional function (Figure 1). This research provides a compelling argument for the importance of maternal health and materno-fetal interactions, including placental function, for neurodevelopment. However, as with any emerging field, there are both empirical and conceptual challenges for studies of the fetal origins of individual differences in neurodevelopment and mental health. We focus on these issues, attempting to contribute to the further growth of DOHaD research.
FIGURE 1. A Schema Describing Proposed Pathways Linking Antenatal Maternal Mental Health to Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in the Offspringa
aThe wider lines depict associations for which there is stronger evidence, such as the associations between antenatal maternal symptoms of depression or anxiety or maternal stress, and child neurodevelopmental outcomes. The thin dashed lines represent proposed associations that currently lack support or for which the evidence is mixed, such as the relation between antenatal maternal emotional well-being and either maternal cortisol levels or birth outcomes. The model also highlights (*) factors that moderate the impact of maternal emotional well-being, maternal endocrine status, or birth outcome on child neurodevelopment, which merit greater emphasis in future research on the developmental origins of health and disease.

Challenges for DOHaD Models of Mental Health

The Term “Maternal Adversity”

Measures of stressful life events, the perception of stress, depressive symptoms, and levels of state, trait, or pregnancy-related anxiety are commonly used to indicate maternal adversity. This clustering implies an assumption of common influences and/or underlying mechanisms. The evidence suggests otherwise. When studied across the normal range, maternal anxiety appears to more consistently predict neurodevelopmental outcomes than do depressive symptoms (14, 40), although a direct comparison is difficult to operationalize. And subtle distinctions within specific forms of “adversity” seem important. For example, while trait anxiety accounts for a proportion of pregnancy-related anxiety, pregnancy-related anxiety may have greater predictive value for obstetric or child outcomes than more global measures of maternal anxiety (e.g., 41, 42).
One potential source of confusion is the assumption that various forms of maternal adversity, including depression, anxiety, and stress, are associated with increased glucocorticoid levels and thus reduced birth weight and birth weight–associated outcomes. There is indeed compelling evidence for the role of glucocorticoids in fetal growth restriction. However, only a subset of patients with clinical depression show hypercortisolemia, and to our knowledge there are conflicting data on the association of anxiety with cortisol levels. Studies employing momentary assessments of maternal mood states show covariation between negative mood in pregnancy and salivary cortisol (e.g., 43); however, a number of studies find little or no association between maternal cortisol levels and measures of maternal stress, anxiety, or depression (25, 4447). Indeed, pregnancy in humans and other mammals is associated with dampened HPA stress reactivity. Detailed studies reveal no association of maternal salivary (19), plasma, or amniotic cortisol levels (46, 47) with either maternal stress or anxiety. Sarkar et al. (47) reported a weak correlation between maternal anxiety and plasma cortisol, and only in early pregnancy. In contrast, O’Connor et al. (48) reported an association between maternal depression and diurnal cortisol levels in a low-socioeconomic-status sample, while one study (49) has reported increased cortisol levels in pregnant women with comorbid anxiety and depression. These findings suggest that the relation between maternal mental health and glucocorticoid exposure may lie within specific subgroups, including women with more severe mental health conditions.
Antenatal maternal mood, cortisol levels, and stress all predict neurodevelopmental outcomes in offspring, but the evidence to date suggests that these factors are not necessarily associated with each other, and there is even less evidence to suggest that maternal glucocorticoid levels mediate the effects of maternal adversity on neurodevelopment. Rather, the evidence suggests that these effects may operate through different pathways, and thus a broader survey of potential mediating mechanisms is called for (50). Indeed, studies of the specific outcomes associated with distinct antenatal influences should enhance our ability to elucidate specific mechanisms.

Relation Between Maternal Mental Health and Birth Outcomes

Intense maternal stress, such as exposure to Hurricane Katrina, is associated with low birth weight (<2.5 kg) (51). However, these effects are independent of maternal mental health, further underscoring the distinct influences of specific forms of maternal adversity. An extensive review (52) reveals an influence of severe stress (e.g., death of a spouse) on offspring birth weight, as well as of factors such as social support that moderate the impact of stress, but no consistent evidence for the influence of maternal anxiety or depression (also see reference 53). The exceptions are studies showing an association between “pregnancy-associated anxiety” and birth outcomes, which again underscores the specificity of different forms of maternal “adversity.”
While extremely stressful conditions may affect birth outcomes, large epidemiological studies with community samples (e.g., 45) have reported no association between maternal levels of depression, anxiety, or stress and either birth weight or gestational age. Likewise, a meta-analysis examining maternal depression and birth weight (53) revealed a relation using categorical measures of depression, reflecting symptoms in the clinical range, but little or no association between birth weight and continuous measures of depressive symptoms, despite the fact that these same measures predict neurodevelopmental outcomes (see above). Interestingly, the association between clinical depression and birth outcome is strong in developing but not developed countries (53). This finding may reflect greater access to resources for pregnant women in developed countries, such as better nutrition and prenatal health services, that buffer against compromised birth outcomes that would otherwise be apparent. Nevertheless, the existing findings suggest that while clinical levels of maternal psychopathology may be associated with low birth weight, across nonclinical populations the level of depressive symptoms or anxiety is unrelated to birth outcomes. The assumption that birth weight necessarily reflects maternal stress/adversity across the normal population, especially in developed countries, is not yet justified by the existing literature. Low birth weight seems most reasonably interpreted as reflecting a less-than-optimal intrauterine environment (4).

Antenatal Glucocorticoid Effects

Mothers with female fetuses at risk for virilization through congenital adrenal hyperplasia are treated with synthetic glucocorticoids in the first trimester to suppress endogenous pituitary-adrenal activity. The offspring show altered cognitive performance, including inattention, as well as increased fearfulness (for a review, see reference 54). Antenatal treatment with synthetic glucocorticoids predicts increased HPA reactivity to stress in neonates born at term (55). Antenatal glucocorticoid treatment is also associated with increased cortisol responses to the Trier social stress test in older children, an effect unique to female offspring (56). These findings are consistent with studies reporting an association between increased maternal antenatal cortisol levels and infant HPA reactivity to stress (e.g., 57, 58).
Glucocorticoids are also used clinically in the third trimester to advance pulmonary development in the fetus when there is a risk for preterm delivery. In contrast to the congenital adrenal hyperplasia findings, clinical trials report surprisingly few effects of antenatal glucocorticoid treatment on neurodevelopmental outcomes. These studies involve powerful synthetic glucocorticoid receptor agonists, such as dexamethasone and betamethasone, which, unlike cortisol, are poor substrates for placental 11β-HSD-2. A randomized controlled trial comparing antenatal betamethasone with placebo with follow-up in adulthood found no differences between groups exposed to betamethasone and placebo in cognitive functioning, working memory and attention, psychiatric morbidity, or health-related quality of life (59). Similarly, a trial comparing single versus multiple antenatal glucocorticoid treatments found no differences in measures of neurocognitive function (60). A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials found that chronic glucocorticoid treatment during pregnancy reduced fetal growth, but with no discernible effects on neurodevelopment (61, 62). These findings contrast with those described above, and especially noteworthy is the absence of effects of antenatal glucocorticoid treatment designed to enhance pulmonary maturation compared with treatment for congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Importantly, the latter routinely involves treatment in the first trimester, whereas the former targets mid to late pregnancy. The timing of glucocorticoid treatment or exposure over the course of pregnancy, at various stages of neurodevelopment, could potentially explain seemingly conflicting findings (63). Nevertheless, in relation to the broader DOHaD hypothesis, it should be noted that a glucocorticoid treatment in later pregnancy that reliably reduces birth weight is not consistently associated with long-term effects on neurodevelopment.
Two additional, important issues complicate our understanding of the role of antenatal glucocorticoids as mediators of maternal influences on neurodevelopment. The first concerns the unexpected specificity of effects observed in neuroimaging studies. Glucocorticoid receptors are expressed throughout the developing nervous system. Why, then, are effects of maternal mental health or maternal glucocorticoid levels specific to the amygdala, and largely the right amygdala, as well as cortical regions? Why is there such little evidence from clinical studies for effects on the hippocampus, when the rodent and nonhuman primate studies reveal consistent effects on hippocampal structure? The issue here again may be that of the timing of human corticolimbic development, although existing studies suggest that the hippocampus and amygdala show comparable developmental trajectories. Likewise, the issue of asymmetric effects, most notably in measures of amygdala volume, remains a puzzle.
A second issue is the consistent finding of gender differences. A variety of studies have reported effects of maternal cortisol levels or birth weight that are apparent only in female offspring (11, 27, 64). Models that rely on the importance of stress mediators will need to be expanded to explain the specificity of effects in terms of gender or neurodevelopmental outcome.

Genetic Influences

The study of antenatal influences relies on measures of maternal conditions or birth outcomes that are linked to genetic variation (e.g., 65, 66). Yet few studies have addressed the possibility that the reported associations share a common genetic basis. For example, perceived stress is a heritable trait (67), suggesting that the effects of heritable genetic variants may account, in part, for reported associations between maternal stress and measures of child temperament, including stress reactivity, or more generally the risk for psychopathology in the offspring. Rice et al. (68) examined this issue using a variation of a prenatal cross-fostering study in humans in which pregnant mothers were genetically related or unrelated to their child as a result of in vitro fertilization. Associations between antenatal stress and offspring outcome that are environmental in origin should be observed in both unrelated and related mother-child pairs. Antenatal stress in late pregnancy is associated with birth weight, anxiety, and antisocial behavior in both related and unrelated mother-offspring pairs, consistent with an environmental influence. In contrast, the link between prenatal stress and ADHD-like features is present only in related mother-offspring pairs and is therefore potentially attributable to heritable genetic influences or gene-environment interplay.
While antenatal maternal cortisol levels may be associated with HPA function in offspring, the interpretation is subject to concerns of heritable genetic influences. HPA function shows an intriguing, context-dependent heritability (69). High heritability of HPA reactivity is evident among children in families with low adversity, but not in families with high adversity. High familial adversity is associated with greater cortisol reactivity to stress. Since the majority of children enrolled in community-based studies are likely drawn from low-adversity settings, the association between antenatal maternal cortisol and HPA function in the offspring may reflect heritable genetic influences. Moreover, since glucocorticoids are a well-established mechanism for low birth weight, these same considerations might influence the interpretation of associations between birth weight and HPA function. The concern for genetic mediation is underscored by a study showing that among seven confirmed genome-wide loci that account for variance in birth weight, with effects comparable to maternal smoking, three are also associated with cardiovascular (ADRB1) or metabolic disease (ADCY5, CDKAL1) (66), suggesting that the well-documented relation between birth weight and cardiometabolic outcomes may partly reflect common underlying genetic features.
There has been surprisingly little emphasis on the moderating effect of offspring genotype in studies linking birth weight or maternal stress to neurodevelopment. The consideration of heritable genetic variation is not simply an issue for the interpretation of epidemiological studies. The inclusion of genetics in DOHaD research models provides the opportunity to expand the breadth of research focusing on antenatal environmental influences to discern the degree to which such effects are moderated by offspring genotype. Existing studies reveal that associations between birth weight and child socioemotional development are moderated by variants in multiple genes implicated in serotonergic or dopaminergic function (e.g., TPH2, HTR2A, SCL6A4, DRD4) (7072), and stratification by genotype yielded effect sizes beyond those commonly reported for studies of birth weight alone. Qiu et al. (22) reported that offspring COMT genotype actually determined the nature and regional specificity of the influence of antenatal maternal anxiety on cortical morphology.
One approach to examining genetic variation is to compare the moderating influence of the maternal and offspring genotypes. For example, the BDNF genotype of the infant, but not that of the mother, moderates the influence of antenatal maternal anxiety on genome-wide DNA methylation (21). Indeed, a survey of interindividual variability in DNA methylation across the genome in umbilical cord samples shows that the vast majority (∼80%) of such variation is principally determined by an interaction between maternal condition or birth outcome and infant genotype (73). The increased risk of major depressive disorder among individuals born small (<2.5 kg) is greater among children of depressed than nondepressed parents (74). The implication is that the effect of birth weight on later depression is greater among those with a depressed parent, which suggests moderation by genetic influences. A polygenic risk score calculated from genome-wide association studies of major depressive disorder could be used in such studies as a measure of individual cumulative genetic vulnerability (75). Studies of the relation between maternal conditions and birth outcomes might likewise benefit from genetically informed designs. Hypofunctional 11β-HSD-2 variants, for example, might define instances where increased maternal stress is more likely to be associated with increased fetal cortisol exposure (35, 37, 76) and thus greater effects on birth weight and neurodevelopment. These considerations suggest that the transition of DOHaD science from observational studies to those of biological mechanisms will require the integration of genetic information into developmental models. Since vulnerability is seemingly best defined by measures of both environmental risk and genetic predisposition, the inclusion of genetic information is also important for the most precise targeting of interventions.

Clinical Significance

Maternal mental health and birth outcomes predict child health and development. But are these effects functionally relevant and clinically informative? The impact of maternal emotional well-being on child outcomes has been assessed (14, 15). After controlling for multiple potential confounders, children of mothers with increased antenatal anxiety show a twofold increase in behavioral problems. Importantly, these effects equate to an approximate doubling of the population prevalence of childhood psychiatric disorders (15). A similar analysis showed that prenatal maternal stress accounted for 17% of the variance in childhood cognitive abilities (77). The situation is less clear for birth weight. Individuals born small show worse outcomes in terms of school achievement test scores, use of disability programs, residence in high-income areas, and wages (10). These findings suggest an economic impact. However, children born small are also more likely to be born into poverty to mothers of lower education (10), as well as to mothers with high-risk lifestyles that include smoking and increased alcohol consumption, which predict low birth weight. In the 1958 British cohort study of 10,845 children, birth weight accounted for 0.5%−1.0% of the variation in math scores, whereas social class accounted for 2.9%−12.5% (78). Another study (15) showed that obstetric outcomes (birth weight and gestation age) explained <1% of additional variance in child emotional/behavioral difficulties when maternal mental health and socioeconomic status variables were accounted for. These studies suggest that although the impairments in neurodevelopment associated with low birth weight are functionally relevant, the magnitude of the impact uniquely associated with fetal growth per se remains unclear.
The relevance of variation in fetal growth across the population for clinical medicine and intervention programs must be determined. This issue is not unique to the study of mental health outcomes. Despite the many years since the initial reports linking fetal growth to the metabolic syndrome, it remains unclear to what extent fetal development might account for clinical cases of metabolic or cardiovascular disease: What percentage of the patients with the metabolic syndrome were born small? Does fetal growth retardation predict differential clinical outcomes or, importantly, suggest alternative treatment strategies? Studies addressing these issues should occupy the forefront of future DOHaD research.

Fetal Neurodevelopment and “Meta-Plasticity”

There is considerable evidence that the influence of intrauterine factors on neurodevelopment is modified by postnatal environmental conditions. Low birth weight predicts adolescent depression more strongly in girls with a history of adverse postnatal experiences, which suggests that the effects of prenatal experiences are conditional on the postnatal environment (11). Likewise, the association between birth weight and hippocampal volume is moderated by the quality of parental care (27). Socioeconomic status appears to moderate the effects of fetal growth restriction on irritability and impulsivity (79). The well-established association between birth weight and ADHD is absent in suburban communities (80), suggesting effects that are context dependent, and an enhanced quality of maternal care eliminates the association between birth weight and ADHD symptoms (81). Likewise, mother-infant attachment moderates the influence of maternal glucocorticoids on socioemotional development (82). These findings reveal that the consequences of fetal adversity on child development are dependent on the quality of the postnatal environment.
Indeed, various forms of prenatal adversity may actually increase the sensitivity of the developing organism to the influences of the postnatal environment (83). This “meta-plasticity” refers not to the influence of antenatal adversity on any specific outcome, but rather to the degree to which the developing organism is susceptible to subsequent environmental influences. The quality of intrauterine environment might explain, in part, the wide diversity among children in the degree to which developmental outcomes are influenced by prevailing context (84), a possibility that could inform and improve early interventions strategies.
In the studies noted above, the effects of socioeconomic status or maternal care are greater among low compared with normal birth weight offspring (27, 7881). The effect of breastfeeding on cognitive development is greater in low compared with normal birth weight children (85). Likewise, there is evidence for the idea that the impact of interventions or of high-quality day-care programs may be greater among children born at low birth weight (e.g., 86). This meta-plasticity may extend to the influence of antenatal glucocorticoids. The effect of socioeconomic status on long-term memory is greater among children exposed to antenatal glucocorticoid treatment (87).
This issue is of considerable importance for the evaluation of treatment outcomes of prevention/intervention programs, as the failure to account for differential vulnerability could underestimate the “treatment” effect among more vulnerable individuals. These considerations also underscore the importance of longitudinal approaches with sample sizes sufficient to effectively stratify subjects according to risk factors. Such studies might better address the issue of clinical significance, as environmental effects could be evaluated on the basis of adversity in earlier developmental periods.

Conclusions

Research on the fetal origins of individual differences in neurodevelopment has attracted attention to a period of development previously undervalued in importance for later mental health. DOHaD studies provide an empirical basis for multidisciplinary programs across obstetrics/gynecology, neonatology, pediatrics, neuroscience, and psychiatry/psychology and are essential for a comprehensive understanding of the relation between maternal health, fetal growth, and neurodevelopment. This same partnership is critical for clinical programs that target maternal health.
Fetal development clearly matters. Nevertheless, the science of fetal origins of psychopathology faces important challenges. First, current models suffer from the inclusion of multiple antenatal maternal conditions in the broad category of “adversity.” Symptoms of depression or anxiety or responses to stressful circumstances are not synonymous in their underlying biology. Nor do these conditions readily map onto variations in fetal growth. Studies that relate specific maternal conditions and birth outcomes to specific neurodevelopmental consequences bear greater promise in our studies of underlying mechanisms.
Second, glucocorticoids are strongly linked to fetal growth, and there is compelling evidence for the association between maternal cortisol levels and neurodevelopment. However, there is surprisingly little evidence for an association between levels of maternal emotional well-being and increased HPA axis activity. Maternal anxiety or depression may increase transplacental passage of glucocorticoids (e.g., 76, 88), but this possibility awaits direct study. Studies of glucocorticoids or any candidate mechanisms must also account for the unexpected specificity in the effects of antenatal maternal mental health as well as for the prominent effects of gender (Figure 1). Future research will need to resolve why a glucocorticoid treatment regimen sufficient to produce a decrease in birth weight does not necessarily affect neurodevelopment. It will also need to investigate why the amygdala, and in particular the right hemisphere, is so sensitive to the influences of antenatal maternal mood.
Third, future research must clearly assess the clinical relevance of the individual antenatal factors that influence fetal neurodevelopment. Nanni et al. (89) have provided evidence for the role of childhood maltreatment as a significant predictor of illness course and treatment outcomes in depression: childhood maltreatment predicts treatment resistance to antidepressant medications, although not psychotherapy. A comparable level of evidence is lacking for antenatal exposures and should assess the degree to which such factors independently account for variation in mental health outcomes as well as the degree to which such factors might inform clinical practice (14). Future research should also focus on the potential importance of antenatal factors for the effective identification of high-risk individuals in early childhood for prevention programs.
Finally, it is difficult to imagine how environmental influences could operate in the absence of genetic moderation. Environmental signals influence brain development and function through effects on intracellular signaling pathways, the activity of which inevitably varies across individuals in part as a result of sequence-based, heritable genetic variation. Thus, the influence of antenatal maternal conditions on neurodevelopment will vary as a function of the genotype of the offspring. Studies that incorporate the analysis of genomic variation may identify gene networks that moderate the impact of antenatal environmental influences and thus elucidate the underlying biological pathways.
These are formidable challenges, and they are not unique to studies of environmental influences occurring during fetal development. However, progress in our understanding of the origins of mental health will depend on our ability to successfully meet the demands of research that focuses on neurodevelopmental outcomes at the level of the individual child.

References

1.
Bateson P, Gluckman P, Hanson M: The biology of developmental plasticity and the predictive adaptive response hypothesis. J Physiol 2014; 592:2357–2368
2.
Hales CN, Barker DJ: Type 2 (non-insulin-dependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Diabetologia 1992; 35:595–601
3.
Insel TR, Wang PS: Rethinking mental illness. JAMA 2010; 303:1970–1971
4.
Schlotz W, Phillips DI: Fetal origins of mental health: evidence and mechanisms. Brain Behav Immun 2009; 23:905–916
5.
Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Roseboom TJ, et al: Early determinants of mental health. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 2012; 26:599–611
6.
Breslau N, Chilcoat HD: Psychiatric sequelae of low birth weight at 11 years of age. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:1005–1011
7.
Banerjee TD, Middleton F, Faraone SV: Environmental risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatr 2007; 96:1269–1274
8.
Wiles NJ, Peters TJ, Heron J, et al: Fetal growth and childhood behavioral problems: results from the ALSPAC cohort. Am J Epidemiol 2006; 163:829–837
9.
Sucksdorff M, Lehtonen L, Chudal R, et al: Preterm birth and poor fetal growth as risk factors of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 2015; 136:e599–e608
10.
Aizer A, Currie J: The intergenerational transmission of inequality: maternal disadvantage and health at birth. Science 2014; 344:856–861
11.
Costello EJ, Worthman C, Erkanli A, et al: Prediction from low birth weight to female adolescent depression: a test of competing hypotheses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:338–344
12.
Thomas K, Harrison G, Zammit S, et al: Association of measures of fetal and childhood growth with non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: the ALSPAC cohort. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 194:521–526
13.
Abel KM, Wicks S, Susser ES, et al: Birth weight, schizophrenia, and adult mental disorder: is risk confined to the smallest babies? Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:923–930
14.
Glover V: Maternal depression, anxiety, and stress during pregnancy and child outcome: what needs to be done. Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol 2014; 28:25–35
15.
O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Barker ED, et al: The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 2014; 26:393–403
16.
Pearson RM, Evans J, Kounali D, et al: Maternal depression during pregnancy and the postnatal period: risks and possible mechanisms for offspring depression at age 18 years. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:1312–1319
17.
Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, et al: Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002; 41:1078–1085
18.
Werner EA, Myers MM, Fifer WP, et al: Prenatal predictors of infant temperament. Dev Psychobiol 2007; 49:474–484
19.
Davis EP, Glynn LM, Schetter CD, et al: Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007; 46:737–746
20.
Velders FP, Dieleman G, Cents RA, et al: Variation in the glucocorticoid receptor gene at rs41423247 moderates the effect of prenatal maternal psychological symptoms on child cortisol reactivity and behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 2012; 37:2541–2549
21.
Chen L, Pan H, Tuan TA, et al: Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism influences the association of the methylome with maternal anxiety and neonatal brain volumes. Dev Psychopathol 2015; 27:137–150
22.
Qiu A, Tuan TA, Ong ML, et al: COMT haplotypes modulate associations of antenatal maternal anxiety and neonatal cortical morphology. Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172:163–172
23.
Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, et al: Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2011; 14:1–27
24.
Weinstock M: The long-term behavioural consequences of prenatal stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008; 32:1073–1086
25.
O’Donnell K, O’Connor TG, Glover V: Prenatal stress and neurodevelopment of the child: focus on the HPA axis and role of the placenta. Dev Neurosci 2009; 31:285–292
26.
Raznahan A, Greenstein D, Lee NR, et al: Prenatal growth in humans and postnatal brain maturation into late adolescence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109:11366–11371
27.
Buss C, Lord C, Wadiwalla M, et al: Maternal care modulates the relationship between prenatal risk and hippocampal volume in women but not in men. J Neurosci 2007; 27:2592–2595
28.
Qiu A, Anh TT, Li Y, et al: Prenatal maternal depression alters amygdala functional connectivity in 6-month-old infants. Transl Psychiatry 2015; 5:e508
29.
Sandman CA, Buss C, Head K, et al: Fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms is associated with cortical thickness in late childhood. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 77:324–334
30.
Buss C, Davis EP, Muftuler LT, et al: High pregnancy anxiety during mid-gestation is associated with decreased gray matter density in 6–9-year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35:141–153
31.
DiPietro JA, Kivlighan KT, Costigan KA, et al: Prenatal antecedents of newborn neurological maturation. Child Dev 2010; 81:115–130
32.
Monk C, Myers MM, Sloan RP, et al: Effects of women’s stress-elicited physiological activity and chronic anxiety on fetal heart rate. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2003; 24:32–38
33.
Rifkin-Graboi A, Meaney MJ, Chen H, et al: Antenatal maternal anxiety predicts variations in neural structures implicated in anxiety disorders in newborns. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2015; 54:313.e2–321e2
34.
Meaney MJ, Szyf M, Seckl JR: Epigenetic mechanisms of perinatal programming of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function and health. Trends Mol Med 2007; 13:269–277
35.
Chapman K, Holmes M, Seckl J: 11β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenases: intracellular gate-keepers of tissue glucocorticoid action. Physiol Rev 2013; 93:1139–1206
36.
Gitau R, Cameron A, Fisk NM, et al: Fetal exposure to maternal cortisol. Lancet 1998; 352:707–708
37.
Holmes MC, Abrahamsen CT, French KL, et al: The mother or the fetus? 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 null mice provide evidence for direct fetal programming of behavior by endogenous glucocorticoids. J Neurosci 2006; 26:3840–3844
38.
Buss C, Davis EP, Shahbaba B, et al: Maternal cortisol over the course of pregnancy and subsequent child amygdala and hippocampus volumes and affective problems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109:E1312–E1319
39.
Buss C, Entringer S, Wadhwa PD: Fetal programming of brain development: intrauterine stress and susceptibility to psychopathology. Sci Signal 2012; 5:pt7
40.
O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, et al: Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003; 44:1025–1036
41.
Huizink AC, Mulder EJ, Robles de Medina PG, et al: Is pregnancy anxiety a distinctive syndrome? Early Hum Dev 2004; 79:81–91
42.
Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA, Porto M, et al: The association between prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age at birth: a prospective investigation. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1993; 169:858–865
43.
Giesbrecht GF, Campbell T, Letourneau N, et al: Psychological distress and salivary cortisol covary within persons during pregnancy. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012; 37:270–279
44.
Voegtline KM, Costigan KA, Kivlighan KT, et al: Concurrent levels of maternal salivary cortisol are unrelated to self-reported psychological measures in low-risk pregnant women. Arch Women Ment Health 2013; 16:101–108
45.
Kramer MS, Lydon J, Séguin L, et al: Stress pathways to spontaneous preterm birth: the role of stressors, psychological distress, and stress hormones. Am J Epidemiol 2009; 169:1319–1326
46.
Baibazarova E, van de Beek C, Cohen-Kettenis PT, et al: Influence of prenatal maternal stress, maternal plasma cortisol, and cortisol in the amniotic fluid on birth outcomes and child temperament at 3 months. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013; 38:907–915
47.
Sarkar P, Bergman K, O’Connor TG, et al: Maternal antenatal anxiety and amniotic fluid cortisol and testosterone: possible implications for foetal programming. J Neuroendocrinol 2008; 20:489–496
48.
O’Connor TG, Tang W, Gilchrist MA, et al: Diurnal cortisol patterns and psychiatric symptoms in pregnancy: short-term longitudinal study. Biol Psychol 2014; 96:35–41
49.
Evans LM, Myers MM, Monk C: Pregnant women’s cortisol is elevated with anxiety and depression—but only when comorbid. Arch Women Ment Health 2008; 11:239–248
50.
Zijlmans MA, Riksen-Walraven JM, de Weerth C: Associations between maternal prenatal cortisol concentrations and child outcomes: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 53:1–24
51.
Xiong X, Harville EW, Mattison DR, et al: Exposure to Hurricane Katrina, post-traumatic stress disorder and birth outcomes. Am J Med Sci 2008; 336:111–115
52.
Graignic-Philippe R, Dayan J, Chokron S, et al: Effects of prenatal stress on fetal and child development: a critical literature review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 43:137–162
53.
Grote NK, Bridge JA, Gavin AR, et al: A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:1012–1024
54.
Cartier J, Zeng Y, Drake AJ: Glucocorticoids and the prenatal programming of neurodevelopmental disorders. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016; 7:1–7
55.
Davis EP, Waffarn F, Sandman CA: Prenatal treatment with glucocorticoids sensitizes the HPA axis response to stress among full-term infants. Dev Psychobiol 2011; 53:175–183
56.
Alexander N, Rosenlöcher F, Stalder T, et al: Impact of antenatal synthetic glucocorticoid exposure on endocrine stress reactivity in term-born children. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2012; 97:3538–3544
57.
Gutteling BM, de Weerth C, Buitelaar JK: Maternal prenatal stress and 4–6 year old children’s salivary cortisol concentrations pre- and post-vaccination. Stress 2004; 7:257–260
58.
O’Connor TG, Bergman K, Sarkar P, et al: Prenatal cortisol exposure predicts infant cortisol response to acute stress. Dev Psychobiol 2013; 55:145–155
59.
Dalziel SR, Lim VK, Lambert A, et al: Antenatal exposure to betamethasone: psychological functioning and health related quality of life 31 years after inclusion in randomised controlled trial. BMJ 2005; 331:665
60.
Asztalos EV, Murphy KE, Willan AR, et al: Multiple courses of antenatal corticosteroids for preterm birth study: outcomes in children at 5 years of age (MACS-5). JAMA Pediatr 2013; 167:1102–1110
61.
Peltoniemi OM, Kari MA, Hallman M: Repeated antenatal corticosteroid treatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 2011; 90:719–727
62.
McKinlay CJ, Crowther CA, Middleton P, et al: Repeat antenatal glucocorticoids for women at risk of preterm birth: a Cochrane Systematic Review. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2012; 206:187–194
63.
Davis EP, Sandman CA: The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev 2010; 81:131–148
64.
Van Lieshout RJ, Boylan K: Increased depressive symptoms in female but not male adolescents born at low birth weight in the offspring of a national cohort. Can J Psychiatry 2010; 55:422–430
65.
Freathy RM, Mook-Kanamori DO, Sovio U, et al: Variants in ADCY5 and near CCNL1 are associated with fetal growth and birth weight. Nat Genet 2010; 42:430–435
66.
Horikoshi M, Yaghootkar H, Mook-Kanamori DO, et al: New loci associated with birth weight identify genetic links between intrauterine growth and adult height and metabolism. Nat Genet 2013; 45:76–82
67.
Wüst S, Federenko I, Hellhammer DH, et al: Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2000; 25:707–720
68.
Rice F, Harold GT, Boivin J, et al: The links between prenatal stress and offspring development and psychopathology: disentangling environmental and inherited influences. Psychol Med 2010; 40:335–345
69.
Ouellet-Morin I, Boivin M, Dionne G, et al: Variations in heritability of cortisol reactivity to stress as a function of early familial adversity among 19-month-old twins. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:211–218
70.
Pluess M, Velders FP, Belsky J, et al: Serotonin transporter polymorphism moderates effects of prenatal maternal anxiety on infant negative emotionality. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 69:520–525
71.
Broekman BF, Chan YH, Goh L, et al: Influence of birth weight on internalizing traits modulated by serotonergic genes. Pediatrics 2011; 128:e1250–e1258
72.
Wazana A, Moss E, Jolicoeur-Martineau A, et al: The interplay of birth weight, dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4), and early maternal care in the prediction of disorganized attachment at 36 months of age. Dev Psychopathol 2015; 27:1145–1161
73.
Teh AL, Pan H, Chen L, et al: The effect of genotype and in utero environment on interindividual variation in neonate DNA methylomes. Genome Res 2014; 24:1064–1074
74.
Nomura Y, Wickramaratne PJ, Pilowsky DJ, et al: Low birth weight and risk of affective disorders and selected medical illness in offspring at high and low risk for depression. Compr Psychiatry 2007; 48:470–478
75.
Wray NR, Goddard ME, Visscher PM: Prediction of individual genetic risk to disease from genome-wide association studies. Genome Res 2007; 17:1520–1528
76.
O’Donnell KJ, Bugge Jensen A, Freeman L, et al: Maternal prenatal anxiety and downregulation of placental 11β-HSD2. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012; 37:818–826
77.
Bergman K, Sarkar P, O’Connor TG, et al: Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007; 46:1454–1463
78.
Jefferis BJ, Power C, Hertzman C: Birth weight, childhood socioeconomic environment, and cognitive development in the 1958 British birth cohort study. BMJ 2002; 325:305
79.
Kelly YJ, Nazroo JY, McMunn A, et al: Birthweight and behavioural problems in children: a modifiable effect? Int J Epidemiol 2001; 30:88–94
80.
Bohnert KM, Breslau N: Stability of psychiatric outcomes of low birth weight: a longitudinal investigation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:1080–1086
81.
Tully LA, Arseneault L, Caspi A, et al: Does maternal warmth moderate the effects of birth weight on twins’ attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and low IQ? J Consult Clin Psychol 2004; 72:218–226
82.
Bergman K, Sarkar P, Glover V, et al: Maternal prenatal cortisol and infant cognitive development: moderation by infant-mother attachment. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:1026–1032
83.
Pluess M, Belsky J: Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity? Dev Psychopathol 2011; 23:29–38
84.
Boyce WT, Ellis BJ: Biological sensitivity to context, I: an evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol 2005; 17:271–301
85.
Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT: Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 1999; 70:525–535
86.
Blair C: Early intervention for low birth weight, preterm infants: the role of negative emotionality in the specification of effects. Dev Psychopathol 2002; 14:311–332
87.
Grant KA, Sandman CA, Wing DA, et al: Prenatal programming of postnatal susceptibility to memory impairments: a developmental double jeopardy. Psychol Sci 2015; 26:1054–1062
88.
Glover V, Bergman K, Sarkar P, et al: Association between maternal and amniotic fluid cortisol is moderated by maternal anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34:430–435
89.
Nanni V, Uher R, Danese A: Childhood maltreatment predicts unfavorable course of illness and treatment outcome in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:141–151

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 319 - 328
PubMed: 27838934

History

Received: 2 February 2016
Revision received: 26 July 2016
Accepted: 22 August 2016
Published online: 14 November 2016
Published in print: April 01, 2017

Keywords

  1. Developmental Programming
  2. DOHaD
  3. Antenatal Anxiety and Depression
  4. Prenatal Stress
  5. Perinatal Psychiatry

Authors

Details

Kieran J. O’Donnell, Ph.D.
From the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology, McGill University, Montreal; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal; and the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore.
Michael J. Meaney, Ph.D.
From the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology, McGill University, Montreal; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal; and the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Meaney ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

The Ludmer Foundation
Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology
Canadian Institute for Advanced Research10.13039/100007631
Hope for Depression Research Foundation10.13039/100006346
Fondation Brain Canada10.13039/100009408
Supported by funding from the Sackler Foundation, the Ludmer Foundation, Brain Canada, and the Hope for Depression Research Foundation. Dr. O’Donnell is a Global Scholar of the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research Child and Brain Development Program.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share