Skip to main content
Full access
Editorials
Published Online: 1 May 2018

Eradicating Traumatic Memories: Implications for PTSD Treatment

Plato suggested that memory was like an aviary in which a different bird represented each specific memory. Memory retrieval, therefore, was a process of capturing the correct bird. And once the right bird was captured, the conscious mind could review the intact memory it had been seeking (1). A more recent memory metaphor likened autobiographical memory to a vast filing system, with each file containing a representation of a particular past experience (2).
During the past 25 years, such metaphors have been replaced by a connectionist model of memory postulating that memory storage is more like a spider web in which specific memories are represented by the patterns of connections among fibers throughout the entire network. Memories are not stored as intact entities, like Plato’s birds, but rather must be reconstructed by recreating a pattern of associated threads of information across an entire network. After retrieval, memories must again be consolidated and stored within this connectionist network through a process called “reconsolidation” (2, 3). Preclinical studies indicate that memories are labile during both the retrieval and postretrieval reconsolidation process. Most importantly, pharmacological agents, such as anisomycin and propranolol, may disrupt this process, thereby weakening or even erasing such memories (4).
This is the context within which to consider the provocative, theory-driven therapeutic approach developed by Brunet and colleagues, who report findings in this issue (4). They propose that reduction, or even elimination, of traumatic memories during the reconsolidation process can produce a clinically significant improvement for patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
In a small randomized clinical trial, 30 patients with PTSD were given propranolol 90 minutes before they reactivated their traumatic memories by providing a written or oral account of the traumatic event. This procedure was repeated once a week for 6 weeks. When compared with a comparable group of 30 PTSD patients who received a placebo before undergoing the same brief traumatic memory activation procedure for 6 weeks, the propranolol group exhibited significantly greater reduction in PTSD symptoms. Unfortunately, the 6-month follow-up data are difficult to interpret because of participant attrition.
Among the strengths of the Brunet et al. study are that it represents a creative translational approach to pharmacotherapy for PTSD. The most successful treatments for PTSD are trauma-focused cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) approaches in which the active ingredients appear to be fear extinction and cognitive reprocessing of the traumatic memory itself (5). Extrapolating from the connectionist model of memory as well as from preclinical studies with fear conditioning/extinction paradigms, Brunet et al. suggest that a pharmacological approach that attenuates or eradicates traumatic memories will reduce the severity of PTSD symptoms.
It is also exciting to consider the possibility that an antiadrenergic agent might yet play a significant role in PTSD treatment. Previous interest in propranolol has focused primarily on its potential as a prophylactic agent that might prevent the later development of PTSD among recently traumatized individuals. But evidence to support such a possibility has been inconclusive (5). Furthermore, enthusiasm about prazosin, which appeared initially to show great promise, has been tempered by negative results from a large multisite VA Cooperative Study (ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT00532493). So, despite robust findings, dating back to the 1980s, regarding adrenergic dysregulation in PTSD (6), we have yet to demonstrate the efficacy of any antiadrenergic agent as an evidence-based treatment for PTSD.
What are the questions and concerns we might have about the study? There are the usual suspects: It is a small study, and one in which half the participants were lost to follow-up at the 6-month assessment point, so we don’t know the duration or stability of the propranolol effect. As the authors acknowledge, without additional controls we can’t rule out the possibility that propranolol actually disrupted memory retrieval, rather than reconsolidation. The study lacked a treatment manual (the authors considered it unnecessary because of the simplicity of the procedure), so we have no information on treatment fidelity and adherence. But these are all issues that can be addressed in future studies.
To me, the most intriguing and fundamental question is about the nature of the memories that were retrieved and, presumably, later reconsolidated during the study. There are different kinds of memories. Explicit/declarative memories record consciously available material, whereas implicit/nondeclarative memories are not consciously available. Traumatic memories are generally considered implicit, but they may be embedded in significant explicitly remembered contextual information. Different brain structures may play different roles in processing information within those two systems. Furthermore, different medications, such as propranolol, may exert different effects on these two memory systems. Intentional recall, as in the present study, is different from unintentional or flashback recall, such as the acute triggering of a traumatic memory by a sudden unexpected encounter with a traumatic reminder (a common scenario among PTSD patients). Indeed, there is good evidence that intentionally retrieved memories, as in the present study, are “deficient” because of short-term memory deficits, overgeneralization of autobiographical memories, and fragmentation of traumatic narratives, and because traumatic memories can be forgotten (7). Brewin et al. (8) have proposed a dual representation theory of PTSD that distinguishes between memories that can be retrieved and articulated in coherent written narratives and PTSD flashbacks, which involve a marked sense of reliving in the present and which appear to be stored in a separate, nonverbal, image-based, sensory memory system. Successful treatment requires the transfer of somatic/flashback memories to the verbally accessible memory system, where they can be reprocessed within a CBT protocol.
Last, but certainly not least, is the possibility that the hypothesis guiding this approach is overly simplistic. Successful recovery from PTSD may require much more than voluntary retrieval and reconsolidation of a traumatic memory after ingesting propranolol. After all, in effective evidence-based CBT treatments for PTSD, the work has just begun after retrieval of the traumatic memory; therapy consists of intense processing, reprocessing, and cognitive reframing of such memories before postsession reconsolidation occurs. Furthermore, powerful traumatic memories are not always voluntary and verbally accessible in PTSD patients, especially when dissociation occurs or when PTSD is comorbid with the retroactive amnesia of a traumatic brain injury (9). If it can be shown that this approach really works, we will need to understand how and why it has been successful. Such research will necessarily enrich our understanding of fundamental memory processes as well as of the altered memory mechanism underlying PTSD.
In summary, this study may open the door to a completely new and effective treatment for PTSD. It will be encouraging indeed if these results can be replicated. It would be better still if we could achieve a better understanding of how disruption, by propranolol, of intentionally retrieved traumatic memories results in beneficial treatment for PTSD.

Footnote

The opinions expressed here are those of the author and do not reflect an official position of the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs or Dartmouth College.

References

1.
Roediger HL 3rd: Memory metaphors in cognitive psychology. Mem Cognit 1980; 8:231–246
2.
Lindsay DS, Read JD: Memory, remembering, and misremembering. PTSD Res Q 1995; 6:1–7
3.
Schacter DL: Searching for Memory: The Brain, the Mind, and the Past. New York, Basic Books, 1996
4.
Brunet A, Saumier D, Liu A, et al: Reduction of PTSD symptoms with pre-reactivation propranolol therapy: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:427–433
5.
VA/DoD Clinical Practice Guideline for Management of Post-Traumatic Stress. Washington, DC, Department of Veterans Affairs and Department of Defense, Oct 2010
6.
Pitman RK, Rasmusson AM, Koenen KC, et al: Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat Rev Neurosci 2012; 13:769–787
7.
Brewin CR: Remembering and forgetting, in Handbook of PTSD: Science and Practice, 2nd ed. Edited by Friedman MJ, Keane TM, Resick PA. New York, Guilford, 2014, pp 200–218
8.
Brewin CR, Gregory JD, Lipton M, et al: Intrusive images in psychological disorders: characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment implications. Psychol Rev 2010; 117:210–232
9.
Bryant RA: Posttraumatic stress disorder and traumatic brain injury: can they co-exist? Clin Psychol Rev 2001; 21:931–948

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 391 - 392
PubMed: 29712466

History

Accepted: January 2018
Published online: 1 May 2018
Published in print: May 01, 2018

Keywords

  1. Administration
  2. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Authors

Affiliations

Matthew J. Friedman, M.D., Ph.D. [email protected]
From the National Center for PTSD, VA Medical Center, White River Junction, Vt.; and the Department of Psychiatry, Geisel School of Medicine at Dartmouth College, Hanover, N.H.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Friedman ([email protected]).

Funding Information

The author reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share