Site maintenance Wednesday, November 13th, 2024. Please note that access to some content and account information will be unavailable on this date.
Skip to main content
Full access
Articles
Published Online: 13 March 2024

Subcortico-Cortical Dysconnectivity in ADHD: A Voxel-Wise Mega-Analysis Across Multiple Cohorts

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

Objective:

A large body of functional MRI research has examined a potential role for subcortico-cortical loops in the pathogenesis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but has produced inconsistent findings. The authors performed a mega-analysis of six neuroimaging data sets to examine associations between ADHD diagnosis and traits and subcortico-cortical connectivity.

Methods:

Group differences were examined in the functional connectivity of four subcortical seeds in 1,696 youths with ADHD diagnoses (66.39% males; mean age, 10.83 years [SD=2.17]) and 6,737 unaffected control subjects (47.05% males; mean age, 10.33 years [SD=1.30]). The authors examined associations between functional connectivity and ADHD traits (total N=9,890; 50.3% males; mean age, 10.77 years [SD=1.96]). Sensitivity analyses were used to examine specificity relative to commonly comorbid internalizing and non-ADHD externalizing problems. The authors further examined results within motion-matched subsamples, and after adjusting for estimated intelligence.

Results:

In the group comparison, youths with ADHD showed greater connectivity between striatal seeds and temporal, fronto-insular, and supplementary motor regions, as well as between the amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, compared with control subjects. Similar findings emerged when ADHD traits were considered and when alternative seed definitions were adopted. Dominant associations centered on the connectivity of the caudate bilaterally. Findings were not driven by in-scanner motion and were not shared with commonly comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems. Effect sizes were small (largest peak d, 0.15).

Conclusions:

The findings from this large-scale mega-analysis support established links with subcortico-cortical circuits, which were robust to potential confounders. However, effect sizes were small, and it seems likely that resting-state subcortico-cortical connectivity can capture only a fraction of the complex pathophysiology of ADHD.
Understanding the neural basis of complex behavioral phenotypes involves studying small effect sizes, requiring large sample sizes and validation across independent cohorts (1, 2). These considerations apply to efforts to parse the neural substrates of the core symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects around 5%–10% of school-age children (3).
Decades of research point to altered interactions between subcortical regions and cortex in ADHD. Most implicated is a fronto-striato-thalamic circuit comprising reciprocal connections between the caudate, putamen, thalamus, supplementary motor area, lateral prefrontal cortex, and parietal lobe. This circuit is critical to executive functions, including working memory and inhibitory control, known to be impaired in ADHD (4, 5). Additionally, a second fronto-striatal circuit involving the nucleus accumbens and orbitofrontal cortex has been associated with ADHD (68). Dysfunction within this loop may underlie deficits in delay of gratification, reinforcement sensitivity, and effort-related decision making, which are characteristic of ADHD motivation styles (9). Finally, there is emerging evidence for involvement of circuits connecting the amygdala with the insula and dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex, pivotal to affective processing, learning, and regulation, particularly in the context of negative emotions (10, 11). While the evidence for a role for the amygdala in ADHD is less compelling than for fronto-striatal loops, alterations within these amygdala-centered circuits have been observed in recent work in the disorder and have been proposed to underlie commonly co-occurring affective problems in youths with ADHD (10, 11).
Despite the large body of research implicating subcortico-cortical circuitry in ADHD, there has been a lack of convergence across studies examining the functioning of these circuits at rest, as the effects are likely to be small, and most individual studies are likely underpowered (12). Although typically including larger sample sizes, retrospective meta-analyses of published studies have also failed to detect robust group differences (12). However, these meta-analyses are severely limited by a lack of consistency in seed selection and region-of-interest definitions across different studies. Moreover, given the limited availability of unthresholded statistical maps, neuroimaging meta-analyses are typically conducted using published coordinates (5). Consequently, only group differences meeting thresholds for statistical significance in published studies are includable, with subthreshold group differences not considered. This is especially problematic considering the known issues in the literature concerning low statistical power and publication bias, which lead to inflated type I and type II error rates among published neuroimaging findings (1, 2). Furthermore, the reliance on published group-level summary statistics means that published meta-analyses have not been able to consider potential confounders at the individual subject level, including in-scanner motion and comorbid emotional and behavioral problems.
Our aim in this study was to overcome these limitations by applying a mega-analytic approach to data from six data sets. We compared 1,696 youths with ADHD diagnoses against 6,737 unaffected control subjects. We followed up this analysis by examining associations with ADHD traits in 9,890 individuals, assessed using the attention problems subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (13). All analyses controlled for key demographic variables, including age, sex, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status, as well as comorbid internalizing and non-ADHD externalizing problems and in-scanner motion. We examined the robustness of findings to considerations of estimated general intelligence and medication status, and examined associations in motion-matched subsamples. We also examined the specificity of findings relative to commonly comorbid internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Finally, we examined whether neuropsychological domains that are known to be subserved by subcortico-cortical circuits and commonly associated with ADHD were similarly associated with alterations in resting-state subcortico-cortical connectivity.
We hypothesized that the dominant patterns of associations between ADHD diagnosis and traits and subcortico-cortical dysfunction would center on the connectivity of striatal seeds, as the weight of the literature points to these striatal regions as pivotal in ADHD pathogenesis (4, 14). However, based on accumulating evidence for a role for the amygdala in the disorder (11, 14, 15), we also hypothesized ADHD-related abnormalities in amygdala connectivity, which may be tied to commonly comorbid affective problems (10, 11).

Methods

Cohorts and Measures of ADHD

The methods section in the online supplement summarizes the recruitment methods, sampling strategies, protocols, and image acquisition parameters for each cohort. We contrasted individuals with ADHD against unaffected control subjects using data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study (ABCD; N=7,268), the Healthy Brain Network (N=766), the Neurobehavioral Clinical Research cohort (N=226), and the enhanced Nathan Kline Institute–Rockland cohort (N=173) (1619). Diagnoses were determined using DSM-5 criteria from semistructured interviews. Unaffected control subjects had minimal ADHD problems and were not taking psychostimulant medication (see the online supplement).
For the trait analyses, we used the attention problems subscale of the CBCL. We included data from the ABCD (N=7,703), Healthy Brain Network (N=846), Neurobehavioral Clinical Research (N=232), Nathan Kline Institute–Rockland (N=188), Human Connectome Project–Development (N=439), and National Consortium on Alcohol and Neurodevelopment in Adolescence (N=482) cohorts (1621).
All studies had ethical approval and obtained informed assent or consent using institutional review board–approved procedures. The main inclusion criteria were availability of all covariate data, usable neuroimaging data, and ages ≥6 and ≤18 years. This age range was chosen because it corresponds to the age range for the CBCL (13).

Resting-State Connectivity

Details on image acquisition parameters for each cohort are provided in the online supplement. Preprocessing was performed using a well-validated and standardized 36-parameter plus despiking pipeline (22). Seeds for the caudate, putamen, nucleus accumbens, and amygdala were selected from the Harvard-Oxford probabilistic anatomical atlas (threshold ≥25% probability) (23). In the first instance, we examined seed regions bilaterally. However, supplementary analyses tested for potential hemisphere-specific associations. Mean time series were extracted for each region of interest. These time series were then correlated with the time series of each gray matter voxel in the brain, thereby creating subject-level voxel-wise connectivity maps for each seed, which were subsequently Fisher-z-transformed.
Subtle differences in seed placement can impact resting-state neuroimaging findings. We therefore performed supplementary analyses using alternative seed definitions (24). These supplementary analyses considered potential functional heterogeneity between dorsal and ventral subdivisions of subcortical structures. See the online supplement for details; Figures S1 and S2 depict the spatial location for the adopted seeds.

Modeling Approach

Voxel-wise linear mixed-effects modeling was performed using the lmerTest package (25) for R (http://www.r-project.org). We examined connectivity at each voxel as a function of ADHD diagnosis, while controlling for age, sex, socioeconomic status (household income), race/ethnicity, internalizing and non-ADHD externalizing problems assessed using the CBCL broadband subscales, and in-scanner motion (mean root-mean-squared [RMS] and mean RMS-squared). These covariates were included as fixed effects. Nested random effects were included for study, scanner ID, and nuclear family. The resultant statistical maps were thresholded using an initial cluster-forming threshold of p<0.0001 and a family-wise-error cluster-level-corrected threshold of p<0.0125 (p<0.05/4 seed regions). We adopted a similar approach to examine associations with scores on the attention problems subscale. Sensitivity analyses and robustness checks included removing the associations between ADHD diagnosis or attention problems and in-scanner motion using a greedy matching algorithm (26), controlling for the potential confounder of estimated general intelligence and performing analyses in psychostimulant-free subgroups.
Owing to similar patterns of connectivity across subcortical seeds, partial correlation analyses were also performed to test for potentially more direct associations. Specifically, at the individual subject level, we assessed connectivity between the seed time series and the remaining voxels of the brain while controlling for the time series of the remaining three seed regions.
We next assessed disorder specificity of associations relative to commonly comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems assessed using the CBCL.
To examine the possibility that subcortico-cortical connectivity may be linked to ADHD via altered neuropsychological performance, within the large ABCD cohort we tested for associations between resting-state subcortico-cortical connectivity and performance on neuropsychological tests of cognitive domains commonly linked with ADHD (4, 5, 27), including working memory, inhibitory control, processing speed, and impulsive decision making (28, 29).
Finally, we examined whether associations between subcortico-cortical connectivity and ADHD diagnoses and traits changed or remained stable with age. As in previous work, to limit confounding between age range and cohort, we explored this question in the five data sets with suitably wide age ranges, excluding the ABCD cohort because subjects in that cohort were largely 9–10 years of age at the time of scanning (30).
See the online supplement for further details, including model syntax.

Results

The participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Groups differed on key demographic variables, including age, sex, and race/ethnicity. Consequently, we controlled for these variables as covariates in all models.
TABLE 1. Characteristics of youths with ADHD and unaffected control subjects included in the case-control analysis, as well as subjects included in the analyses of ADHD traits (CBCL analyses)a
VariableADHD Group (N=1,696)Control Group (N=6,737)StatisticpEffect SizeCBCL Analyses (N=9,890)
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Age (years)10.832.1710.331.30t=9.53<0.001d=0.2910.771.96
Minutes of useable data12.734.9615.484.35t=−21.09<0.001d=−0.5915.054.93
In-scanner motion (mean RMS)0.1810.0540.1760.051t=3.38<0.001d=0.090.1740.05
IQ100.4116.54105.5716.47t=−5.24<0.001d=−0.31105.9316.83
Scaled matrix9.662.9110.322.85t=−6.63<0.001d=−0.2310.242.92
NIH Toolbox
 Working memory94.5915.4497.1716.22t=−4.92<0.001d=−0.1696.6316.12
 Processing speed85.3916.9488.5717.41t=−5.55<0.001d=−0.1988.0317.38
 Inhibitory control92.3911.7194.0813.26t=−4.20<0.001d=−0.1493.7613.08
MedianIQRMedianIQRMedianIQR
CBCL
 Attention problems (raw)8613W=10,735,550<0.001δ=0.8825
 Internalizing (raw)71035W=8,297,186<0.001δ=0.4536
 Externalizing (raw)7.51214W=9,047,786<0.001δ=0.5826
N%N%N%
Sexχ2=201.97<0.001OR=2.22
 Male112666.393,17047.054,97550.30
 Female57033.613,56752.954,91549.70
Cash choice taskχ2=0.460.50OR=1.05
 3 days39339.182,48440.373,03940.09
 3 months61060.823,66959.634,54259.91
Race/ethnicityχ2=18.400.001V=0.02
 Asian231.351592.362362.39
 Black/African American21512.6170810.511,11011.22
 Hispanic/Latino33319.651,26518.781,82218.42
 Mixed/other19111.266609.801,00110.12
 White93455.133,94558.565,72157.85
Household incomez=−0.510.61OR=0.98
 <$50,00044626.31,60923.882,40924.36
 $50,000–100,00048728.711,93728.752,77928.10
 $100,001–$200,00044426.182,20532.733,12831.63
 >$200,00031918.8198614.641,57415.92
a
ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; CBCL=Child Behavior Checklist; OR=odds ratio; RMS=root‐mean‐square; V=Cramér’s V; W=Wilcoxon signed rank test; δ=Cliff’s delta.

Within-Group Brain Findings

Group-average seed-based maps for 9,890 youths are provided in Figures S3 and S4 in the online supplement.

Group Comparison

The caudate, putamen, and nucleus accumbens seeds showed heightened connectivity with left and right middle and superior temporal gyri/insula/inferior parietal lobe, extending into inferior frontal gyri for the caudate and putamen seeds, for 1,696 children/adolescents with ADHD relative to 6,737 unaffected control subjects. Those with ADHD also showed heightened connectivity between the caudate and putamen seeds and clusters including supplementary motor area/precentral gyrus/postcentral gyrus/inferior parietal lobe regions. The amygdala seed was associated with heightened connectivity with the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in youths with ADHD relative to control subjects. Peak effect sizes were small, with d values ranging from 0.11 to 0.15 (Table 2 and Figure 1; see also Figures S5–S9 in the online supplement).
TABLE 2. Results of case-control comparisona
ClusterxyzPeak dMean dSize (voxels)Overlap (%)Talairach Label
Left and right caudate
164−7−30.150.1017,5559.2Left superior temporal gyrus
9.1Right superior temporal gyrus
7.7Right postcentral gyrus
5.9Right insula
5.8Left postcentral gyrus
5.3Right precentral gyrus
4.1Left insula
3.8Left precentral gyrus
3.0Right inferior parietal lobule
2.9Right medial frontal gyrus
2.8Left middle temporal gyrus
2.5Left medial frontal gyrus
2.4Right inferior frontal gyrus
2.3Left inferior parietal lobule
1.8Right paracentral lobule
1.7Left paracentral lobule
1.6Right middle temporal gyrus
Left and right putamen
1−51−3−10.130.092,19337.4Left superior temporal gyrus
34.1Left middle temporal gyrus
10.6Left insula
2.9Left postcentral gyrus
1.4Left inferior temporal gyrus
1.1Left fusiform gyrus
1.1Left precentral gyrus
1.0Left inferior parietal lobule
244−2580.120.0998237.9Right superior temporal gyrus
37.4Right middle temporal gyrus
7.5Right transverse temporal gyrus
5.3Right insula
1.8Right postcentral gyrus
1.5Right inferior temporal gyrus
3−31−39440.120.0956428.3Left inferior parietal lobule
21.3Left postcentral gyrus
13.0Left precentral gyrus
1.2Left superior parietal lobule
452−29360.120.0951754.9Right postcentral gyrus
14.2Right precentral gyrus
9.0Right inferior parietal lobule
53020−210.120.0947241.5Right inferior frontal gyrus
29.5Right insula
10.7Right superior temporal gyrus
8.4Right uncus
1.1Right middle frontal gyrus
65824120.110.0936795.6Right inferior frontal gyrus
1.0Right precentral gyrus
Left and right nucleus accumbens
16−23680.120.091,17929.6Right medial frontal gyrus
10.9Left medial frontal gyrus
9.1Right paracentral lobule
6.2Left precentral gyrus
5.5Right cingulate gyrus
3.5Right postcentral gyrus
2.1Left postcentral gyrus
2.1Left paracentral lobule
234−5120.130.091,14127.0Right precentral gyrus
17.7Right insula
17.3Right postcentral gyrus
14.8Right superior temporal gyrus
3.5Right inferior parietal lobule
2.8Right middle temporal gyrus
2.5Right inferior frontal gyrus
1.7Right claustrum
1.0Right transverse temporal gyrus
3−63−1920.120.0979642.6Left superior temporal gyrus
21.6Left insula
11.7Left lentiform nucleus
10.2Left precentral gyrus
3.7Left inferior parietal lobule
2.4Left middle temporal gyrus
1.7Left claustrum
4−39−15500.130.0957942.4Left precentral gyrus
42.2Left postcentral gyrus
7.7Left inferior parietal lobule
Left and right amygdala
1−9−1420.110.0924444.0Left cingulate gyrus
22.4Right cingulate gyrus
12.6Right medial frontal gyrus
12.6Left superior frontal gyrus
8.5Left medial frontal gyrus
a
Youths with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), N=1,696; unaffected control subjects, N=6,737. For all clusters, ADHD group > control group.
FIGURE 1. Findings from a mega-analysis of differences in seed-based subcortico-cortical connectivity in youths with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and unaffected control subjectsa
aPanels A–D show, respectively, results from the caudate, putamen, nucleus accumbens, and amygdala seeds. Positive effect sizes indicate ADHD group > control group. Voxels in significant clusters are opaque and boxed. Subthreshold voxels are presented translucently.

Associations Between ADHD Traits and Functional Connectivity

The diagnostic findings were partially echoed by findings for ADHD traits (N=9,890). Specifically, connectivity between the caudate seed and left and right middle and superior temporal gyri/insula/inferior parietal lobe and the supplementary motor area/precentral gyrus/postcentral gyrus/inferior parietal lobe was positively associated with scores on the attention problems subscale, as was connectivity between the nucleus accumbens and left and right superior temporal lobe/insula and right inferior parietal lobe. Scores on the attention problems subscale were also positively associated with connectivity between the amygdala seed and right middle frontal gyrus and supramarginal gyrus/superior temporal lobe/inferior parietal lobe. Peak effect sizes were again small, with partial r values ranging from 0.05 to 0.07. These are provided in Table S1 in the online supplement; see also Figures S10–S14 in the online supplement.

Sensitivity Analyses and Robustness Checks

Matching on motion.

The primary findings remained significant after matching groups on in-scanner motion (ADHD group, N=1,642; control group, N=6,737). After removing significant associations between in-scanner motion and scores on the attention problems subscale (N=9,867), findings for the caudate seed remained significant, as did associations between scores on the attention problems subscale and connectivity between the amygdala and right middle frontal gyrus. Effect sizes were also largely unchanged. (See Tables S2 and S3 and Figures S15 and S16 in the online supplement.)

Controlling for estimated general intelligence.

The primary findings remained significant after controlling for estimated general intelligence. Effect sizes were also largely unchanged. (See Tables S4 and S5 and Figures S16 and S17 in the online supplement.)

Psychostimulant-free subgroup analysis.

When comparing 1,114 psychostimulant-free youths with ADHD against unaffected control subjects, widespread group differences (ADHD group > control group) in connectivity between striatal seeds and left and right middle and superior temporal gyri/inferior and superior parietal lobe/insula/inferior frontal gyri and left and right parietal lobe/precentral gyrus/postcentral gyrus regions were observed, albeit only at a relaxed cluster-forming threshold of p<0.005. This may reflect the reduction in sample size for the ADHD group and resultant loss of statistical power. At the same threshold, heightened connectivity between the amygdala seed and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in youths with ADHD relative to control subjects was retained from the primary analyses. (See Figure S19 and Table S6 in the online supplement for details.)

Partial correlation analyses.

After controlling for the time series of the other seeds, greater connectivity between the caudate and supplementary motor area/precentral gyrus/postcentral gyrus, right inferior parietal lobe, and right middle and superior temporal gyri was found in patients with ADHD relative to unaffected control subjects. At a relaxed cluster-forming threshold of p<0.005, the findings for the caudate seed closely resembled those observed in the primary analyses (i.e., heightened connectivity with left and right temporal lobe/insula/inferior parietal lobe/inferior frontal gyri and supplementary motor area/precentral gyrus/postcentral gyrus/parietal lobe in patients with ADHD relative to unaffected control subjects). Findings from the primary group comparison were not retained for the other seeds at either threshold.
Furthermore, after controlling for the time series of the other seeds, positive associations were observed between scores on the attention problems subscale and connectivity between the caudate and left and right superior temporal lobe (extending into inferior parietal lobe on the right side). At a liberal cluster-forming threshold of p<0.005, the findings for the caudate seed closely resembled those observed in the primary analyses. (See Figures S20 and S21 and Tables S7 and S8 in the online supplement.)

Alternative seed definitions.

Findings for the Harvard-Oxford seeds broken down by hemisphere are presented in Figures S22–S25 and Tables S9–S12 in the online supplement.
When the primary analyses were rerun using alternative seed definitions, associations similar to those in the primary analyses based on the Harvard-Oxford seeds emerged for the dorsal/ventral caudate and nucleus accumbens seeds. Specifically, ADHD was associated with greater connectivity relative to unaffected control subjects between striatal seeds and left and right middle and superior temporal gyri/insula/inferior parietal lobe (extending into the inferior frontal gyri bilaterally for the caudate seeds) and supplementary motor area/precentral gyrus/postcentral gyrus/parietal lobe regions. For the putamen seed, similar patterns of greater connectivity in youths with ADHD relative to unaffected control subjects were found for the ventral subdivision only. Similarly, greater connectivity between the amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex was found for the ventral, but not the dorsal, amygdala seed. (See Figure S26 and Table S13 in the online supplement.)
As in the primary analyses using the Harvard-Oxford seeds, connectivity between the caudate seeds and left and right middle/superior temporal lobe/insula/inferior parietal lobe regions was positively associated with scores on the attention problems subscale. However, associations between attention problems scores and connectivity between the caudate and supplementary motor area/precentral gyrus/postcentral gyrus/parietal lobe were significant only for the dorsal caudate seed. Further associations for the remaining seeds are reported in Figure S27 and Table S14 in the online supplement.
Effect sizes were similar across seed definitions (range for peak voxel effect sizes for alternative seed definitions: d, 0.11–0.14; partial r, 0.05–0.07).

Disorder specificity.

No significant associations were observed for scores on the internalizing problems subscale. Scores on the externalizing problems subscale had negative associations with connectivity between subcortical seeds and predominantly middle and superior temporal and parietal regions. (See Figures S28 and S29 and Table S15 in the online supplement.) All clusters from the primary analysis examining associations with attention problems scores emerged as differentially associated with scores on this subscale compared with the externalizing problems subscale. Furthermore, for the caudate seed, connectivity with left and right temporal lobe/insula/inferior parietal lobe/inferior frontal gyri regions also emerged in our direct comparisons with the internalizing problems subscale. (See Figures S30 and S31 and Tables S16 and S17 in the online supplement.)

Associations with neuropsychological measures.

There were minimal associations between scores on the neuropsychological tests or decision-making task and subcortico-cortical resting-state connectivity. Subthreshold associations also point to a lack of overlap with brain regions showing greater connectivity in youths with ADHD relative to unaffected control subjects. (See Figures S32–S36 and Table S18 in the online supplement.)

Interactions with age.

There were minimal significant interaction effects with age on subcortico-cortical resting-state connectivity. None overlapped with primary findings. (See Figures S37–S40 and Table S19 in the online supplement.)

Discussion

In this study, we applied voxel-wise mega-analytic methods to examine patterns of resting-state subcortico-cortical connectivity associated with ADHD diagnosis (1,696 youths with ADHD and 6,737 unaffected control subjects) and ADHD traits (in 9,890 participants). In line with fronto-striatal models of the disorder, ADHD diagnosis and traits were associated with abnormal connectivity between striatal seeds and inferior frontal, insular, supplementary motor, and inferior parietal regions, with the dominant and most widespread associations centered on the connectivity of the caudate bilaterally (4, 5). Greater connectivity was also observed between the amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in youths with ADHD relative to control subjects. The overall pattern of results was robust across two sets of region-of-interest definitions, after adjustments for estimates of general intelligence, and after matching subjects on in-scanner motion. Furthermore, this pattern of findings was not shared with commonly comorbid internalizing or externalizing problems.
Associations with ADHD diagnosis and traits were most widespread for connectivity of the caudate seed, and after including the time series for all subcortical seeds in first-level partial-correlation models, group differences were observed only for this region of interest. These associations were not shared with scores on the internalizing and externalizing problems subscales. Such findings align with well-established neurobiological models of ADHD, which emphasize alterations in caudate functioning (4, 5, 31). Moreover, they are supported by decades of research that have linked caudate alterations to the disorder through techniques such as in vivo receptor imaging, structural MRI, and task-based fMRI (5, 14, 31). The specificity of these findings in relation to internalizing and externalizing problems is consistent with previous studies. These studies have demonstrated the disorder-specific nature of task-based connectivity and activation within the same set of regions, including the caudate, inferior frontal, and supplementary motor regions, when compared with various psychiatric conditions commonly observed in childhood (5, 32). Furthermore, the present findings suggest that these brain alterations are specifically associated with ADHD and are not indicative of general features of childhood psychopathology or influenced by comorbid symptoms (5, 30, 32).
Contemporary accounts often link alterations in resting-state connectivity to ADHD symptoms via neuropsychological functions such as working memory, inhibitory control, and impulsive decision making (3234). These functions are closely relevant to the symptom profile of ADHD and have been linked to subcortico-cortical functioning (4, 5, 32). However, in our study, no significant associations were found between neuropsychological performance and subcortico-cortical connectivity. Furthermore, while the regions implicated by our connectivity findings resemble those from previous imaging meta-analyses of task-based fMRI studies of inhibitory control in ADHD (5, 32), a recent literature review of task-based functional connectivity studies pointed to hypoconnectivity, not hyperconnectivity as we found, in similar regions during inhibitory control tasks in ADHD (32, 35). Thus, while our findings are broadly consistent with models centered on roles for fronto-striatal circuits in ADHD (4, 5), they also indicate the need for models that can explain the absence of associations with neuropsychological task performance and the contradictory direction of effects observed under task-based and resting-state conditions.
The small effect sizes observed in the present mega-analysis (largest peak Cohen’s d, 0.15; largest peak partial r, 0.07) align with the emerging consensus that reproducible associations between individual differences in brain functioning and complex psychological phenotypes such as ADHD will almost certainly involve small univariate effect sizes, and further indicate that most previous neuroimaging studies of ADHD have been significantly underpowered. Consequently, small-scale, cross-sectional, mass-univariate observational studies are expected to offer limited utility in advancing the field. However, the neuroimaging research of ADHD is entering an exciting phase, with the ever-expanding availability of large-scale longitudinal data sets that encompass genetic, neuroimaging, clinical, and family data (19, 21, 36, 37). These data sets hold promise for investigating important clinically relevant questions and ensuring the reproducibility of brain-behavior associations (2, 38). For instance, contrary to the traditional understanding of ADHD as an early-onset disorder with symptoms gradually diminishing over time, recent longitudinal clinical investigations have revealed greater variability in ADHD symptom course. This includes late adolescent/adult onset, idiosyncratic fluctuations in symptom trajectories and diagnostic status, and shifts in dominant symptom domains over time (19, 39). With the advent of multiple large-scale independent discovery and test longitudinal data sets, the field will soon be empowered to meaningfully apply longitudinal multivariate prediction methods. This can aid in exploring questions such as whether brain imaging data can predict later ADHD symptom trajectories (2, 19). Furthermore, future research may leverage sophisticated imaging genetics and within-subject, repeated-measures designs to enable quasi-causal inferences (2). Such studies can help differentiate features of brain structure and functioning that play mechanistic roles in the etiology of ADHD from those that are secondary to ADHD symptoms or otherwise linked to the disorder in a non-causal manner (2).
Some important limitations of our study must be kept in mind. First, analyses were performed in volume space, and previous work has indicated improvements in both statistical sensitivity and spatial accuracy with surface-based relative to volume-based fMRI (40). Second, subjects were instructed to keep their eyes open during scanning, and eye-tracking data were not available to ensure compliance with these instructions or for use in models controlling for eyes-open/eyes-closed status at the level of individual subjects. Third, we integrated data from several diverse data sets characterized by distinct imaging protocols, recruitment procedures, and diagnostic tools. Research conducted on more homogeneous samples might exhibit larger effect sizes, although this approach might compromise the generalizability of the findings. Fourth, it is important to acknowledge that the mega-analytic study sample did not accurately reflect the demographics of the U.S. population. Notably, over 15% of the children and adolescents included in the study came from households with incomes exceeding $200,000. This skewed representation likely rendered the sample unrepresentative of the entire ADHD population, which is a well-known concern in neuroimaging studies focusing on neurodevelopmental disorders (30). Therefore, it is inappropriate to consider our effect size estimates as representative of the entire U.S. child population. Fifth, because of our reliance on cross-sectional data, we were limited in our ability to investigate whether the connections between resting-state connectivity and ADHD diagnoses and traits varied with age. Although we addressed this matter using a cross-sectional approach, such methods are susceptible to cohort effects and fail to capture individual-level fluctuations in brain functioning and ADHD traits. Moreover, our utilization of cross-sectional data prevented us from making definitive statements regarding the direction of effects (2).
In summary, we conducted the largest study to date on changes in subcortico-cortical connectivity in ADHD. The brain regions showing altered connectivity align with fronto-striatal models of the disorder, but the effects observed were small. Resting-state subcortico-cortical connectivity can only capture a small fraction of the complex pathophysiology of ADHD.

Footnote

Data used in this study were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). The ABCD Study is supported by NIH and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html, and a list of participating sites and a complete list of the study investigators is available at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_ members/. The ABCD data used in this study came from https://doi.org/10.15154/1519007 and from the fast-track data release (raw neuroimaging data); the raw data are available at https://nda.nih.gov/edit_collection.html?id=2573. Data and/or research tools used in the preparation of this manuscript were obtained from the NIMH NDA, data set identifiers 2573, 2846, and 3165 (see https://doi.org/10.15154/1527788). This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of NIH or of the submitters submitting original data to NDA. This study included data from a limited-access data set obtained from the Child Mind Institute Biobank, of the Healthy Brain Network study (http://www.healthybrainnetwork.org). Data collection and sharing for this project were provided by the National Consortium on Alcohol and Neurodevelopment in Adolescence (NCANDA), which is funded by NIAAA, with co-funding from NIDA, NIMH, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and the NIH Office of the Director (grants AA021695 [principal investigators, S.A. Brown, S.F. Tapert], AA021697 [principal investigators, A. Pfefferbaum, K.M. Pohl], AA021692 [principal investigator, S.F. Tapert], AA021681 [principal investigator, M.D. DeBellis], AA021690 [principal investigator, D.B. Clark], AA021691 [principal investigator, B. Nagel], AA021696 [principal investigators, I.M. Colrain, F.C. Baker], and AA021697-04S1 [principal investigator, K.M. Pohl]). NCANDA data are disseminated by the Center for Health Sciences, SRI International. The data used in this study are from NCANDA_PUBLIC_4Y_REDCAP_V01 (https://doi.org/10.7303/syn22216455), NCANDA_PUBLIC_BASE_RESTINGSTATE_V01 (https://doi.org/10.7303/syn11605291) and NCANDA_PUBLIC_BASE_STRUCTURAL_V01 (https://doi.org/10.7303/syn11541569). The Nathan Kline Institute–Rockland study was supported by NIH grant U01MH099059 and NIMH BRAINS grant R01MH094639-01. Additional support was provided by NIH grants R01MH101555 and R01AG047596 and grant 1FDN2012-1 from the Child Mind Institute. Funding for key personnel was also provided in part by the New York State Office of Mental Health, NIH grant R01MH120601, and the Research Foundation for Mental Hygiene. This study reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of other individuals or institutions, including NIH, the ABCD, NCANDA, or HCP consortium investigators, the Child Mind Institute, SRI International, or other funding agencies. Image processing was conducted using the high-performance computing capabilities of the NIH Biowulf cluster. The authors thank the NIMH Data Science and Sharing Team for help with accessing and processing the ABCD-BIDS data set.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.20230026.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, et al: Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature 2022; 603:654–660
2.
Tervo-Clemmens B, Marek S, Barch DM: Tailoring psychiatric neuroimaging to translational goals. JAMA Psychiatry 2023; 80:765–766
3.
Polanczyk GV, Salum GA, Sugaya LS, et al: Annual research review: a meta‐analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2015; 56:345–365
4.
Arnsten AF, Rubia K: Neurobiological circuits regulating attention, cognitive control, motivation, and emotion: disruptions in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012; 51:356–367
5.
Norman LJ, Carlisi C, Lukito S, et al: Structural and functional brain abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparative meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:815–825
6.
Albaugh MD, Orr C, Chaarani B, et al: Inattention and reaction time variability are linked to ventromedial prefrontal volume in adolescents. Biol Psychiatry 2017; 82:660–668
7.
Albaugh MD, Ivanova M, Chaarani B, et al: Ventromedial prefrontal volume in adolescence predicts hyperactive/inattentive symptoms in adulthood. Cereb Cortex 2019; 29:1866–1874
8.
Bralten J, Greven CU, Franke B, et al: Voxel-based morphometry analysis reveals frontal brain differences in participants with ADHD and their unaffected siblings. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2016; 41:272–279
9.
Plichta MM, Scheres A: Ventral-striatal responsiveness during reward anticipation in ADHD and its relation to trait impulsivity in the healthy population: a meta-analytic review of the fMRI literature. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 38:125–134
10.
Brotman MA, Kircanski K, Stringaris A, et al: Irritability in youths: a translational model. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174:520–532
11.
Maier SJ, Szalkowski A, Kamphausen S, et al: Altered cingulate and amygdala response towards threat and safe cues in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychol Med 2014; 44:85–98
12.
Cortese S, Aoki YY, Itahashi T, et al: Systematic review and meta-analysis: resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2021; 60:61–75
13.
Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA: Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles: An Integrated System of Multi-Informant Assessment. Burlington, VT, UASEBA, 2001
14.
Hoogman M, Bralten J, Hibar DP, et al: Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: a cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2017; 4:310–319
15.
Karalunas SL, Fair D, Musser ED, et al: Subtyping attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using temperament dimensions: toward biologically based nosologic criteria. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71:1015–1024
16.
Nooner KB, Colcombe SJ, Tobe RH, et al: The NKI-Rockland sample: a model for accelerating the pace of discovery science in psychiatry. Front Neurosci 2012; 6:152
17.
Alexander LM, Escalera J, Ai L, et al: An open resource for transdiagnostic research in pediatric mental health and learning disorders. Sci Data 2017; 4:170181
18.
Feczko E, Conan G, Marek S, et al: Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) community MRI collection and utilities. bioRxiv, 2021 (https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.07.09.451638)
19.
Sudre G, Frederick J, Sharp W, et al: Mapping associations between polygenic risks for childhood neuropsychiatric disorders, symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, cognition, and the brain. Mol Psychiatry 2020; 25:2482–2492
20.
Somerville LH, Bookheimer SY, Buckner RL, et al: The Lifespan Human Connectome Project in Development: a large-scale study of brain connectivity development in 5–21 year olds. Neuroimage 2018; 183:456–468
21.
Brown SA, Brumback T, Tomlinson K, et al: The National Consortium on Alcohol and Neurodevelopment in Adolescence (NCANDA): a multisite study of adolescent development and substance use. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2015; 76:895–908
22.
Ciric R, Rosen AFG, Erus G, et al: Mitigating head motion artifact in functional connectivity MRI. Nat Protoc 2018; 13:2801–2826
23.
Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, et al: An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 2006; 31:968–980
24.
Di Martino A, Scheres A, Margulies DS, et al: Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 2008; 18:2735–2747
25.
Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB: lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J Stat Softw 2017; 82:1–26
26.
Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Ruparel K, et al: Heterogeneous impact of motion on fundamental patterns of developmental changes in functional connectivity during youth. Neuroimage 2013; 83:45–57
27.
Rubia K, Alegria AA, Cubillo AI, et al: Effects of stimulants on brain function in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 76:616–628
28.
Luciana M, Bjork JM, Nagel BJ, et al: Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: overview of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2018; 32:67–79
29.
Weintraub S, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, et al: Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology 2013; 80:S54–S64
30.
Sudre G, Norman L, Bouyssi-Kobar M, et al: A mega-analytic study of white matter microstructural differences across 5 cohorts of youths with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2023; 94:18–28
31.
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Newcorn J, et al: Brain dopamine transporter levels in treatment and drug naive adults with ADHD. Neuroimage 2007; 34:1182–1190
32.
Rubia K: Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and its clinical translation. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12:100
33.
Norman LJ, Sudre G, Price J, et al: Evidence from “big data” for the default-mode hypothesis of ADHD: a mega-analysis of multiple large samples. Neuropsychopharmacology 2023; 48:281–289
34.
Castellanos FX, Proal E: Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16:17–26
35.
Kowalczyk OS, Mehta MA, O’Daly OG, et al: Task-based functional connectivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. Biol Psychiatry Glob Open Sci 2021; 2:350–367
36.
Casey BJ, Cannonier T, Conley MI, et al: The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2018; 32:43–54
37.
Nigg JT, Karalunas SL, Mooney MA, et al: The Oregon ADHD-1000: a new longitudinal data resource enriched for clinical cases and multiple levels of analysis. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2023; 60:101222
38.
Mooney MA, Hermosillo RJ, Feczko E, et al: Cumulative Effects of Resting-State Connectivity Across All Brain Networks Significantly Correlate With ADHD Symptoms. medRxiv, 2021 (https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.11.16.21266121)
39.
Norman LJ, Price J, Ahn K, et al: Longitudinal trajectories of childhood and adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder diagnoses in three cohorts. EClinicalMedicine 2023; 60:102021
40.
Brodoehl S, Gaser C, Dahnke R, et al: Surface-based analysis increases the specificity of cortical activation patterns and connectivity results. Sci Rep 2020; 10:5737

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 553 - 562
PubMed: 38476041

History

Received: 9 January 2023
Revision received: 14 July 2023
Accepted: 30 August 2023
Published online: 13 March 2024
Published in print: June 01, 2024

Keywords

  1. Neurodevelopmental Disorders
  2. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
  3. Neurocircuitry
  4. Cognitive Neuroscience

Authors

Details

Luke J. Norman, Ph.D. [email protected]
Office of the Clinical Director, NIMH, Bethesda, Md. (Norman, Shaw); Section on Neurobehavioral and Clinical Research, Social and Behavioral Research Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, NIH, Bethesda, Md. (Sudre, Price, Shaw).
Gustavo Sudre, Ph.D.
Office of the Clinical Director, NIMH, Bethesda, Md. (Norman, Shaw); Section on Neurobehavioral and Clinical Research, Social and Behavioral Research Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, NIH, Bethesda, Md. (Sudre, Price, Shaw).
Jolie Price, B.A.
Office of the Clinical Director, NIMH, Bethesda, Md. (Norman, Shaw); Section on Neurobehavioral and Clinical Research, Social and Behavioral Research Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, NIH, Bethesda, Md. (Sudre, Price, Shaw).
Philip Shaw, Ph.D.
Office of the Clinical Director, NIMH, Bethesda, Md. (Norman, Shaw); Section on Neurobehavioral and Clinical Research, Social and Behavioral Research Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, NIH, Bethesda, Md. (Sudre, Price, Shaw).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Norman ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Funded by the NIMH Intramural Research Program and the National Human Genome Research Institute (grant ZIAHG200378 to Dr. Shaw). This funding supported data collection for the Neurobehavioral Clinical Research cohort (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT01721720).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share