Skip to main content
Full access
Editorials
Published Online: 1 April 2024

The Influence of Parenting on Callous-Unemotional Traits and the Implications for the Causes and Treatment of Conduct Disorder

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
Pezzoli and colleagues (1) provide another important set of analyses from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a large (N=9,260 twin pairs) longitudinal study of twins living in the United Kingdom. I do not provide a critique of the design of the study here, because the authors do an excellent job of highlighting its unique ability to separate genetic and environmental influences on the constructs of interest (i.e., negative parenting and callous-unemotional [CU] traits) and the relationships between them. The authors also do an excellent job of summarizing the key limitations of the study design (e.g., limits in the study measures, reliance on parental report, and use of a predominantly White Western sample). The authors appropriately focused most of their discussion on the implications of their results for research and for the treatment of children with elevated CU traits. In this editorial, I attempt to extend these implications for guiding both causal research and treatment for children who are diagnosed with conduct disorder.
To place my comments in context, it is important to note that CU traits were included as the “with limited prosocial emotions” specifier for the diagnosis of conduct disorder, first in DSM-5 and later in DSM-5-TR, and for the diagnosis of conduct-dissocial disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in ICD-11. The inclusion of this specifier in the most recent editions of these manuals was based on research showing the etiological and clinical utility of distinguishing subgroups of youths with serious behavior problems who differ in their level of CU traits (2). In terms of etiology, research suggests that when CU traits co-occur with conduct problems, they distinguish a subgroup of children and adolescents with serious conduct problems who differ from other youths with serious conduct problems on several risk factors that could implicate distinct causal processes leading to their conduct problems (3). In terms of clinical utility, research suggests that CU traits distinguish a subgroup of children and adolescents with serious conduct problems who show more severe, chronic, and aggressive conduct problems (3) and who respond differently to treatment (4) compared with other youths with conduct problems. With the inclusion of CU traits in diagnostic classification, they have become an important focus of research, especially research that can guide the development and implementation of more effective treatment for youths with conduct disorder.

Parenting and Callous-Unemotional Traits: How Parenting Is Defined Matters

Pezzoli et al. chose to study parenting as the environmental influence of interest in their study of the genetic and environmental contributions to CU traits based on a number of studies showing an association between parenting and CU traits. Parenting has long been considered a critical part of most theories for how children develop serious conduct problems. Furthermore, bringing about improvements in parenting has long been a primary goal of the most effective treatments for children with serious conduct problems (5). For these reasons, the findings from Pezzoli et al. are highly relevant to research on conduct disorder. Their results showed that the variance and stability in negative parenting were influenced primarily by genetics in early childhood but were influenced primarily by shared environmental effects later in childhood. These findings are critical for interpreting previous research on the role of parenting and its influence on conduct disorder, which has typically ignored potential genetic influences on parenting and has not considered potential changes in the factors that influence parenting over the course of development.
Although these are critical findings, I focus my comments primarily on the results related to potential child effects. Specifically, not only was there weak evidence for the presence of child effects (i.e., the influence of children’s CU traits on later parenting), but also the phenotypic associations between the parenting measures and CU traits were modest overall. The authors note a number of possible reasons for these somewhat surprising results, including the age of the children in the sample (e.g., parenting influences may be stronger earlier in development) and the need to study parenting influences using more interactional measures (e.g., observing parent-child interactions). However, one possibility they raise that deserves particular attention is the potential that focusing on other aspects of parenting may have led to different findings.
Specifically, research over the past several decades has suggested that the influence of parenting on children’s conduct problems may differ depending on the child’s level of CU traits. This possibility is best illustrated by results that I published with students over two decades ago, showing that a measure of dysfunctional parenting was strongly associated with conduct problems only in children with low levels of CU traits (6). These findings were interpreted by us and others (7) as showing that parenting has more of an influence on conduct problems in children low on CU traits. Importantly, the measure of parenting that we used in our study included a wide range of parenting behaviors that had been related to conduct problems, including parental involvement with their child, the use of positive reinforcement, the inconsistent use of discipline, poor monitoring and supervision of the child, and the use of corporal punishment. About a decade later, Pasalich and colleagues (8) attempted to replicate our findings, but they separated harsh and inconsistent parenting from warm and responsive parenting. Their findings closely replicated ours for the measure of harsh and inconsistent parenting (i.e., harsh and inconsistent parenting was related to conduct problems only in children with low levels of CU traits). However, their measure of warm parenting showed a very different association with conduct problems; it was more strongly and inversely related to conduct problems in the children high on CU traits.
The importance of warm and responsive parenting for children high on CU traits has been supported by a host of other studies, both in predicting later conduct problems and in their association with CU traits themselves (see reference 9 for a review). Furthermore, these findings are consistent with developmental theories on how empathy, guilt, and other prosocial emotions develop, which emphasize the importance of warmth and responsiveness in the parent-child relationship (10). Although the vast majority of studies linking parental warmth to conduct problems have not used a genetically informed design, Hyde et al. (11) conducted an adoption study showing that responsive parenting moderated the genetic risk for CU traits in young children. Furthermore, there has been evidence for child effects of CU traits in predicting decreases in parental warmth over time. In a study we conducted (12) testing the cross-lagged effects of parental warmth and CU traits in adolescent males who had been arrested and assessed multiple times over a 5-year period, CU traits consistently predicted reductions in parental warmth over time, but parental warmth did not predict reductions in CU traits. Although this study did not use a genetically informed design, it used a methodology that separated between-individual or trait-level associations across individuals (e.g., people higher on CU traits are more likely to have relationships lower in warmth across development) from changes within the individual (e.g., changes in an individual’s level of CU traits at an earlier time point predict changes in warmth in that individual’s relationships at a later time point).
To be clear, this is not a criticism of the Pezzoli et al. study, but it is an important point in interpreting these results. The authors employed measures of negative parental discipline (e.g., smacking and shouting at their children) and negative parenting feelings toward their children (e.g., feeling frustrated and angry at them). Thus, their findings of modest associations between parenting and CU traits and minimal evidence for child effects may be specific to their definition of parenting. Again, the fact that different findings may have been found for measures of warm and responsive parenting was explicitly noted by Pezzoli et al. However, it is important to emphasize that research has accumulated suggesting that how parenting is defined can have a large impact on the associations of parenting with CU traits and conduct problems.

Callous-Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems: What It Means To Be a Specifier

The fact that CU traits may alter the effects of a major risk factor for conduct disorder, such as parenting, highlights a broader issue related to the use of these traits as a specifier. That is, CU traits were included as a specifier in diagnostic classification because they seem to classify important subgroups within the broader diagnostic category of conduct disorder—subgroups that differ on etiologically important processes (e.g., parenting influences and emotional reactivity to other’s emotions) and clinically important characteristics (e.g., severity and types of aggression) (3). Some have argued for other personality dimensions to be included as specifiers for conduct disorder, based largely on the fact that they are highly associated with conduct problems (13). However, constructs can be highly related to the symptoms of conduct disorder for a number of reasons that do not make them good specifiers; for example, the constructs may be a general risk factor for conduct disorder (e.g., problems with impulse control for early-onset conduct disorder) or may be good indicators of the disorder itself (e.g., lying and conning others) (for a more extended discussion of this issue, see reference 2). In fact, if indicators of a construct are highly related to the symptoms of the disorder, it can make them less useful as a specifier (i.e., for distinguishing subgroups within the group of individuals with the disorder) because they may be present for a large number of people with the disorder.
Instead, evidence to support a specifier should focus on those characteristics that best differentiate individuals with the disorder who manifest the specifier from others with the disorder who do not manifest the specifier. This can be done by comparing subgroups within a group of individuals with the disorder defined by a specifier on important criteria that might suggest different causal factors for the group, might suggest different responses to treatment, or might indicate risk for more significant impairment. However, it is important to establish that these differences are not simply a result of variations in the number of symptoms of the disorder itself. For example, an early study from the TEDS sample showed very different patterns of genetic and environmental effects on serious conduct problems at age 7 depending on whether the children also had elevated CU traits, even after controlling for number of conduct problems (14). Alternatively, the usefulness of a specifier could also be tested by determining whether indicators of the specifier are related to important criteria independent of symptoms of the disorder (e.g., CU traits predicting bullying behavior after controlling for number of conduct problems [15]) or whether these indicators interact with symptoms of the disorder in their relationships to important criteria, as has been the case with parenting.
So how does this discussion of specifiers relate to the findings of Pezzoli et al. on the associations between negative parenting and CU traits? As noted above, the measure of hostile parenting behaviors and feelings may not have assessed the types of parenting that are most important for children with elevated CU traits, but instead, these measures assessed parenting behaviors that are either specific to children with conduct problems without CU traits or parenting behaviors that are related to conduct problems more generally. To illustrate this possibility, we showed (16) that in our sample of arrested adolescents followed over 5 years after arrest, parental warmth was uniquely associated with CU traits after controlling for levels of hostile parenting and antisocial behavior, whereas parental hostility was uniquely associated with antisocial behavior, after controlling for CU traits and parental warmth.
In short, not only must studies carefully consider what dimensions of parenting to study, but it is also important for studies of CU traits and conduct problems to consider their shared and unique associations with etiologically and clinically important variables, including parenting. Again, this possibility was recognized by Pezzoli et al. in their statement that their findings may have differed depending on the child’s level of antisocial behavior. However, it is important to consider this possibility as a guide for future research, which could test the separable associations between parental warmth with CU traits and parental hostility with conduct problems, as our group did (15), but by using a genetically informed design like the one employed by Pezzoli et al.

Implications for Enhancing Treatments for Conduct Disorder

The points I have chosen to highlight here in no way change any of the interpretations made by Pezzoli et al. but were specifically selected because of their potential implications for treating children with conduct disorder, especially those who meet criteria for the “with limited prosocial emotions” specifier that was added in DSM-5. Perlstein et al. (4) conducted an excellent meta-analysis summarizing the results of research on the effectiveness of existing treatments for children with elevated CU traits. Their review and analyses suggested that many evidence-based treatments for conduct problems, especially those that focus on improving parenting, lead to reductions in conduct problems in children with elevated CU traits. Thus, although CU traits are modestly stable and this stability is heavily influenced by genetics, as shown by the results of Pezzoli et al., children with CU traits do respond to treatment. However, the meta-analysis by Perlstein et al. also showed that children with elevated CU traits tended to start treatment with more severe conduct problems than other children in treatment for conduct problems, and despite improving with treatment, those with elevated CU traits often leave treatment with more severe conduct problems than other treated children. Furthermore, although Perlstein et al. noted significant reductions in the level of conduct problems after treatment, there was less clear evidence for significant reductions in CU traits.
It is possible that these findings on existing treatments may be a result of the fact that most evidence-based treatments for conduct problems, especially those with the goal of improving parenting in families of young children with conduct problems, focus only modestly on increasing parental warmth and responsiveness. Instead, they typically include a greater focus on reducing harsh parenting and increasing parental consistency in discipline, the types of harsh parenting studied by Pezzoli et al. As noted by their results, harsh and inconsistent parenting may be related only modestly to CU traits, and based on other research, such parenting may be less related to conduct problems for children with elevated CU traits. In support of this possibility, Kimonis and colleagues (17, 18) showed that when a standard parenting intervention was augmented to teach parents how to increase their warmth and emotional responsiveness in their interactions with their young children, children with elevated CU traits were more likely to leave treatment with their level of conduct problems in a normative range (17), to have their conduct problems remain low over time (18), and to have increases in their level of empathy toward others (17, 18). Thus, to further inform such interventions, research using strong genetically informed and longitudinal designs (such as that used by Pezzoli et al.) is needed. However, to advance the work by Pezzoli et al., researchers should consider carefully how parenting is defined and test whether the role of parenting differs in its relationship with conduct problems depending on the level of CU traits. Such findings could be critical for advancing both causal theories of conduct disorder and for designing more effective treatments for children with this diagnosis.

References

1.
Pezzoli P, Pingault J‐B, Malanchini M, et al: Reciprocal effects between negative parenting and children’s callous-unemotional traits from mid to late childhood. Am J Psychiatry 2024; 181:310–321
2.
Frick PJ: Some critical considerations in applying the construct of psychopathy to research and classification of childhood disruptive behavior disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 2022; 96:102188
3.
Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, et al: Can callous–unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull 2014; 140:1–57
4.
Perlstein S, Fair M, Hong E, et al: Treatment of childhood disruptive behavior disorders and callous-unemotional traits: a systematic review and two multilevel meta-analyses. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2023; 64:1372–1387
5.
McMahon RJ, Goulter N, Frick PJ: Moderators of psychosocial intervention response for children and adolescents with conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2021; 50:525–533
6.
Wootton JM, Frick PJ, Shelton KK, et al: Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: the moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. J Consult Clin Psychol 1997; 65:301–308
7.
Oxford M, Cavell TA, Hughes JN: Callous/unemotional traits moderate the relation between ineffective parenting and child externalizing problems: a partial replication and extension. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2003; 32:577–585
8.
Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, et al: Do callous‐unemotional traits moderate the relative importance of parental coercion versus warmth in child conduct problems? An observational study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52:1308–1315
9.
Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW: What are the associations between parenting, callous-unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clin Psychol Rev 2013; 33:593–608
10.
Frick PJ, Kemp EC: Conduct disorders and empathy development. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2021; 17:391–416
11.
Hyde LW, Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, et al: Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. Am J Psychiatry 2016; 173:903–910
12.
Vaughan EP, Frick PJ, Robertson EL, et al: Interpersonal relationships and callous-unemotional traits during adolescence and young adulthood: an investigation of bidirectional effects in parent, peer, and romantic relationships. Clin Psychol Sci 2023; 11:391–408
13.
Salekin RT: Research review: What do we know about psychopathic traits in children? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017; 58:1180–1200
14.
Viding E, Blair RJR, Moffitt T E, et al: Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005; 46:592–597
15.
Thornton LC, Frick PJ, Crapanzano AM, et al: The incremental utility of callous-unemotional traits in predicting aggression and bullying in a community sample of boys and girls. Psychol Assess 2013; 25:366–378
16.
Vaughan EP, Frick PJ, Ray JV, et al: The associations of maternal warmth and hostility with prosocial and antisocial outcomes in justice-involved adolescents. Dev Psychol 2021; 57:2179–2191
17.
Kimonis ER, Fleming G, Briggs N, et al: Parent-Child Interaction Therapy adapted for preschoolers with callous-unemotional traits: an open trial pilot study. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2019; 48:S347–S361
18.
Fleming GE, Neo B, Briggs NE, et al: Parent training adapted to the needs of children with callous–unemotional traits: a randomized controlled trial. Behav Ther 2022; 53:1265–1281

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 265 - 268
PubMed: 38557142

History

Accepted: 30 January 2024
Published online: 1 April 2024
Published in print: April 01, 2024

Keywords

  1. Callous-Unemotional Traits
  2. Conduct Disorder
  3. Environmental Influences
  4. Genetic Influences
  5. Limited Prosocial Emotions
  6. Parenting

Authors

Details

Paul J. Frick, Ph.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Frick ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The author reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share