Skip to main content
Full access
Special Articles
Published Online: 1 April 2014

Systematic Review of Neuroimaging Correlates of Executive Functioning: Converging Evidence From Different Clinical Populations

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Executive functioning (EF) is an important cognitive domain that is negatively affected in a number of neuropsychiatric conditions. The authors found that the frontal, parietal, and cerebellar lobes were most frequently associated with EF when comparing results from different clinical populations; the occipital lobe was not correlated with EF in any group.

Abstract

Executive functioning (EF) is an important cognitive domain that is negatively affected in a number of neuropsychiatric conditions. Neuroimaging methods have led to insights into the anatomical and functional nature of EF. The authors conducted a systematic review of the recent cognitive and neuroimaging literature to investigate how the neuroimaging correlates of EF compare between different diagnostic groups. The authors found that the frontal, parietal, and cerebellar lobes were most frequently associated with EF when comparing results from different clinical populations; the occipital lobe was not correlated with EF in any group. These findings suggest that individual disease processes affect circuits within an identifiable distributed network rather than isolated regions.
The study of executive function (EF) has become an area of increasing clinical and research interest over the last decade. EF is typically considered to comprise a broad category of several cognitive skills that are commonly referred to as “higher order” or “supervisory,” whose role is to control and coordinate other more basic cognitive functions like language, memory, visuospatial ability, and praxis. Miyake et al. conceptualize EF as “general purpose control mechanisms that modulate the operation of various cognitive subprocesses and thereby regulate the dynamics of human cognition.”1 As such, EFs defy classification into any single function but instead include capacities for planning, initiating, sequencing, and monitoring complex goal-directed behavior. Though recognized and described as a cognitive construct since the 1970s by Luria2 and others, there continues to be neuropsychological and neurobiological interest in better defining its components and understanding its function in relation to other cognitive skills in normal non-disease as well as disease states.
Neuroimaging has emerged as a powerful tool for understanding both the neural structure and function of cognitive processes. As such, EF has become a fruitful area of investigation using neuroimaging techniques. Functional imaging methods such as functional MRI (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) have shown areas of physiological and metabolic activation during EF tasks. Structural methods such as MRI and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) have shown localized areas of volume change or loss of white matter integrity in those who have EF deficits.
Whereas individual studies have focused on neuroimaging correlates of EF in specific disorders or populations, there has not been an attempt to compile and compare studies from different patient populations to examine whether the neuroanatomical correlates of EF among those populations are similar. Here, we present the results of a systematic review of the cognitive and neuroimaging literature. Our goal was to investigate whether the neural correlates of EF, as measured by clinical tests of EF, are similar between various diagnostic groups. Our hypothesis was that the same brain regions would be implicated regardless of patient population, pointing toward a consistent and identifiable network of brain regions. The results of this study, we hope, will point to converging evidence of the neuroimaging correlates of EF, further supporting the hypothesis that neuropsychiatric disorders associated with executive dyscontrol reflect impaired circuits as opposed to individual regions where specific diseases impact the functioning of those specific circuits.3 Further, broad agreement about the correlates of EF among differing groups of patients supports the neural basis of EF as being derived from a distributed network and will hopefully aide in improving diagnostic criteria and forming the biological basis for targets for therapeutic interventions.

Methods

Search Strategy

A systematic literature search was conducted to identify the relevant studies. Searches were thoroughly carried out in the following databases: PUBMED (MEDLINE), PsycINFO, and EMBASE from January 2002 to January 2012 for English-language articles using the following search terms: (“neuroimaging” or “magnetic resonance” or “MRI” or “positron emission tomography” or “PET” or “fMRI”) and (“executive function” or “executive” or “executive control”). Other associated search terms were related to other neuroimaging modalities and tests of executive function. We also searched reference lists of selected articles meeting selection criteria for other relevant studies. Initial search results were subjected to a thorough review (by one of the study’s authors, M.N.) of the study’s abstract and consequently included or excluded studies based on the following criteria.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

From the search results, we selected articles that described neuroimaging relationships to executive functioning tasks in adult or geriatric subject populations (excluding children or adolescents) without regard for specific diagnosis. We did not exclude studies without a control group so as to avoid missing important hypothesis-generating work. Although a correlation analysis was not required to be included in the selected articles, the study must have asserted positive or negative significant statistical or purported relationships based on presented data between neuroimaging findings, brain regions, and tests of executive functioning. We included articles of any structural or functional neuroimaging modality but excluded modalities such as evoked response potential (ERP) or MR spectroscopy to limit the number of articles to the most widely used imaging methods. We included only studies employing tests of EF that are currently and commonly used clinically. Specifically, we included individual tests traditionally considered to be measures of executive function that are included in (but not limited to) well-validated and commonly used neuropsychological batteries. Specific tasks include Trail Making Test (TMT), verbal fluency (letter fluency), design fluency, card sorting (and similar measures), color-word interference (e.g., Stroop), Twenty Questions Test, tower tests [e.g., Tower of London (ToL)], and proverbs test. We excluded novel functional cognitive paradigms specific to a particular study protocol though we included paradigms adapted from common tests as listed earlier. One study by Ueda et al.4 met inclusion and exclusion criteria but employed a Clock Drawing Task. Because this was the only study to use this task, the authors omitted it from the analysis.

Results

Patient Groups and Diagnoses

Our systematic search yielded 147 articles meeting general selection criteria. Each of these articles was thoroughly evaluated and specific inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied. After this, 33 articles from the original group were identified as having met our selection requirements. A summary of these articles’ content is found in Table 1. Among these articles an aggregate of 2951 participants were studied with both neuroimaging and executive function testing. Of this pool of study participants, 1131 were normal or healthy controls without disease. Seven papers studied normal adults exclusively and six papers did not compare their test subjects with normal control subjects. All of the articles’ non-control participants were diagnosed with either a neurological or psychiatric condition. There were no studies pertaining to other medical diseases that met our selection criteria. All tests of EF reported in these studies were deemed by the authors of this review to be easily accessible and frequently used clinical tests of EF. Five different neuroimaging modalities were employed across the selected studies, all commonly used in research and clinical practice.
TABLE 1. Summary of Literature
 1st AuthorYearImagingSampleTest(s) of EFAssociated Region(s)Conclusions
1Bergeson52004MRI75 TBI, 75 NCTMT A and BLeft frontal (r=–0.41, p=0.003)a and total frontal (r=–0.37, p=0.008) (Trails B)Frontal and temporal atrophy correlate with deficits in memory and executive function. No other significant regional correlation with tests of EF
2Baillieux62010SPECTb18 focal cerebellar lesionsWCST, Stroop, TMT (A versus B not specified)Frontal, right cerebellumDamage to the cerebellar lobe can cause cognitive and affective disturbances.
3Chang72010MRI358 MCI, 222 NCTMT A and B, digit span backwardBilateral frontal cortex, bilateral posterior cingulate cortex (left r=0.22; right r=0.19)Reduced thickness in frontal lobes in MCI patients with low EF. Post hoc significant correlations with bilateral middle temporal and left inferior temporal regions.
4Dickerson82010MRI61 ExMCI, 44 MemMCI, 27 ExAD, 12 MemADTMT A and B, BNT, AVLT, discriminability, delayed free recall, Digit symbol, digit span forward and backwardSuperior frontal, superior parietalProminent cortical thinning in frontoparietal regions demonstrated in executive-predominant AD.
5Kaller92011fMRI30 NCToLDorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC)Bilateral dlPFC activation in complex tasks may reflect the concomitant operation of specific cognitive process that show opposing lateralization
6Kinnunen102011DTI28 TBI, 26 NCTMT A and B, D-KEFS: color-word subtest, letter fluencyLeft superior frontal white matter (Rpartial = 0.75, p<0.001), right posterior and medial parietal lobe (Rpartial = 0.30, p<0.01),Frontal lobe connections showed relationships with executive function between two test groups in elevated mean and radial diffusivity
7Koutsouleris112010MRI40 ARMS, 30 NCTMT BVentromedial prefrontal cortex, cerebellum, fronto-callosal white matterExecutive deficits in the ARMS for psychosis may reflect structurally altered networks.
8McDonald122010MRI103 MCI, NC 90TMT A and BBilateral dorsolateral frontal lobe, left medial prefrontal and bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal lobe. Pars orbitalis β (1, 94) = 0.33, p<0.001Regional association with frontal lobe atrophy and TMT-B decline. Pars orbitalis (left frontal lobe) was the only significant lobar predictor.
9Pa132009MRI26 amnestic MCI, 32 dysexecutive MCI, 36 NCTMT B, Stroop, letter fluency, abstractionsdlPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortexDysexecutive MCI had lower EF scores, increased behavioral symptoms. The brain imaging differences suggest that the two MCI subgroups have distinct patterns of brain atrophy.
10Sasson142012DTI52 NormalStroop, Go/no-goFrontal white matter, superior longitudinal fasciculusExecutive function correlated with DTI parameters in frontal white matter and in the superior longitudinal fasciculus. Information processing speed correlated with cingulum, corona radiata, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, parietal white matter and in the thalamus.
11Schmitz152008MRI24 migraine, 24 NCGo/no-go, Stroop, switch taskMiddle frontal gyrus, inferior parietal lobe with striatum.Network of fronto-striatal-parietal brain regions responsible for monitoring EF in migraineurs
12Stricker162011MRI105 AD, 125 NCTMT A and B, digit span backwardFrontal, parietal lobesOverlap between normal and AD-related MRI-based morphometric changes is greater in the very old than in the young old.
13Takahashi172008PETc23 NormalWCSTPrefrontal cortex, hippocampusOrchestration of prefrontal D1 and hippocampal D2 might be necessary for human executive function as part of a prefrontal-hippocampal pathway. No other regions studied
14Toepper182010fMRI20 NormalCorsi Block Tapping test, block suppression TestLeft dlPFCLeft dorsolateral prefrontal cortex plays a crucial role for executive controlled inhibition of spatial distraction.
15Turken192009MRI+DTI1 TBI, 43 NCTMT B, Color-word testFrontal cortex and underlying white matterTests of executive function were related to cortical abnormalities in the frontal lobes.
16Wolf202011fMRI16 pre-HDWCSTLeft dlPFCLeft DLPFC less active during working memory performance cross-sectionally but did not persist over time.
17Connolly212012fMRI18 cocaine-dependent, 9 NCGo/no-go taskPrefrontal, cingulate, cerebellar and inferior frontal gyriiIntegrity of prefrontal systems that underlie cognitive control functions may be an important characteristic of successful long-term abstinence
18Jacobs222012MRI337 MCITMT-B, StroopFrontal-parietal (B=–0.304, p=0.023); Frontal-parietal-subcortical (B=0.355, p=0.001)Parietal white matter hyperintensities are a significant contributor to executive decline in MCI over time. Frontal-subcortical networks did not relate significantly to executive function
19Nestor232011fMRI10 MA19, 18 NCStroopRight inferior frontal gyrus, supplementary motor cortex/anterior cingulate gyrus and the anterior insular cortex, posterior cingulate cortexHypofunction in cortical areas that are important for executive function underlies cognitive control deficits associated with MA dependence
20Eslinger372011MRI26 FTDTMT-B, StroopRight dlPFC, right parietal regions, and left superior temporal gyrus and temporal pole, subcortical areas of the right amygdala and left caudateBehavioral variant FTD causes multiple types of breakdown in empathy, social cognition, and executive resources, mediated by frontal and temporal disease.
21van Tol242011fMRI65 MDD, 82 MDD+anxiety, 63 NCToLLeft dlPFCPrefrontal hyperactivation in MDD but NOT In anxiety.
22Hunt252011PET2610 MCI, 10 AD, 14 NCTMT A and BRight middle frontal cortex and the right precentral gyrus (TMT B), left middle frontal cortex (TMT A).Executive dysfunction in AD as measured by TMT is frontal lobe mediated.
23Chang262009DTI17 CO poisoning, 34 NCDigit span backward, design fluencyLeft orbitofrontal (r2 = 0.81, p=0.02), right frontal (r2 = 0.35, p=0.04)Reduced connectivity between different cortical regions is a pathophysiologic mechanism in CO poisoning and cognitive performance.
24Fine272009MRI19 AD, 25 FTD, 13 Semantic Dementia, 12 PNFA, 9 PSP, 9 NCD-KEFS – sorting testLeft frontal lobeLeft frontal lobe significantly predicted performance on the D-KEFS Sorting Test
25Segarra282008MRI28 Schizophrenia, 28 NCTMT A and B, digit span, WCST, verbal working memory, letter-number sequencing, Controlled Oral Word ExaminationBilateral cerebellumCerebellar gray and white matter volume loss correlates with executive function deficits in schizophrenia.
26Haldane292008MRI44 Bipolar disorder, I, 44 NCStroopDorsal and ventral PFC, right parietal (compensatory)PFC dysfunction in bipolar I with compensatory involvement of the parietal cortices through response inhibition
27Sim302007MRI40 Cocaine dependent, 41 NCTMT A and B, StroopBilateral Cerebellum (Pearson: left=–0.70; right=–0.75)dCerebellum vulnerable to cocaine-associated brain volume changes. Cerebellar and frontal, temporal, and thalamic changes correlate with neuropsychological deficits.
28Grant312007DTI10 Borderline PD, 10 NCTMT A and B, Stroop, WCST, Controlled Word Association TestPosterior white matterPosterior white matter integrity correlated with measures of executive function.
29Lie322006fMRI12 NormalWCSTRostral and caudal ACC, right dlPFC, cerebellum, superior parietal cortex, retrospleniumCentral role of the right dlPFC in executive working memory and cognitive control. Functional dissociation of the rostral and caudal ACC in the implementation of attentional control.
30Moll332002fMRI7 NormalTMT A and BDlPFC and medial prefrontal cortices, intraparietal sulciCritical role of the dlPFC and medial prefrontal cortices as well as the intraparietal sulci in the regulation of cognitive flexibility, intention.
31Wilmsmeier342010fMRI36 schizophrenia, 28 NCWCSTRostral and dorsal ACCSet-shifting is associated with increased activation in the rostral and dorsal ACC
32Schmitz352006fMRI10 ASD, 12 NCStroop, Go/no-go, switch testLeft inferior and orbital frontal gyrus, left insula, parietal lobesAssociation between successful completion of EF tasks and increased brain activation in people with ASD
33Schall362003PETe + fMRI6 NormalToLBilateral dlPFC, inferior parietal cortex, cerebellumToL is a useful tool for investigating particularly prefrontal dysfunction in a broad range of neuropsychiatric conditions
ARMS: at-risk mental state; ASD: autism spectrum disorder; ExAD: executive predominant AD; ExMCI: executive predominant MCI; FDG: 18-fluoro-D-deoxy-glucose; MA: methamphetamine abuse; MemAD: memory predominant AD; MemMCI: memory predominant MCI; PNFA: progressive nonfluent aphasia; PSP: progressive supernuclear palsy; TMT: Trail Making Test; ToL: Tower of London
a
r=Spearman’s correlation statistic.
b
Tc–99m-ECD.
c
[11C]SCH23390 and [11C]FLB457.
d
Pearson correlation voxel maxima MNI coordinate.
e
[15O]H2O.
All non-control subjects studied carried psychiatric or neurological diagnoses. The most common neurological condition studied was dementia. Ten articles studied patients with dementia [Alzheimer’s Dementia (AD), frontotemporal dementia (FTD)] or prodromal dementia [mild cognitive impairment (MCI)]. Three studies included subjects with traumatic brain injury (TBI), one included individuals with carbon monoxide (CO) poisoning, and one study included asymptomatic Huntington’s Disease (HD) gene mutation carriers. Other neurological conditions included migraine, stroke, and brain tumor. Psychiatric conditions varied. Three articles studied patients with schizophrenia or those at risk for psychosis and three articles reported studies of patients with substance abuse or dependence (cocaine and methamphetamine). Three studies tested subjects with disorders of mood or personality [major depressive disorder (MDD), bipolar disorder, and borderline personality disorder]. Finally, one article studied patients in the adult autism spectrum.

Neuroimaging

Structural MRI was the most common neuroimaging technique employed in 14 articles, followed by fMRI−10 articles, DTI−four articles, PET−three articles, and SPECT−two articles. Two studies utilized multimodal imaging methods (DTI+MRI or PET+fMRI).19,36 Imaging protocols, scanning parameters, and image processing methods varied widely among studies. Among articles using MRI, most scans were obtained on 1.5 Tesla MRI scanners. Two studies utilized a 3.0 Tesla scanner.26,37 Most studies employed voxel-wise analyses of data primarily comparing brain surface morphometry, specifically cortical and subcortical thickness. One study used a region of interest (ROI) approach based on regions that were thought to be involved early in AD pathology,16 otherwise most other studies utilized whole brain (left and right hemisphere) approaches to analysis. Regions correlated with cognitive measures generally represented areas of relative atrophy where worse performance usually indicated increased atrophy. Studies employing fMRI methods utilized either 3.0 Tesla or 1.5 Tesla scanners but scanning parameters, image processing, and statistical mapping methods varied widely based on task paradigms. Regions of significant positive correlation to cognitive tasks were generally indicated as regions of increased blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal. DTI studies collected fractional anisotropy and/or diffusivity (mean, axial, or radial) measurements. Correlations between these measures and cognitive performance were made based on integrity of white matter where decreasing anisotropy and increasing diffusivity represented decreased integrity of white matter tracts. Molecular imaging methods, PET and SPECT, utilized several different radioligands for quantification of perfusion and metabolism.

EF Measures

Several measures of EF were used. Many studies used a combination of tests or a formal battery though some isolated one test of EF to study. The most common test was the TMT, used in 17/35 studies. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), was used in 7 of the selected studies. The Stroop Color-Word Association Test was used in 11. Other tests included, digit spans backward and forward, ToL task, and tests of verbal fluency. The entire Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System (D-KEFS) battery was used in one study. In general, there did not seem to be an association between the type of task used and the primary diagnosis of the subjects. Functional neuroimaging methods (fMRI and PET/SPECT) tended to employ single tests of EF whereas structural methods (MRI, DTI) included multiple tests.

Neural Correlates of EF Measures

To compare brain regions across studies, we first determined the primary results of each study. We then designated each brain region as a lobe (frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital), and/or cingulate gyrus, which spans frontal and parietal lobes. This procedure resulted in grouping together multiple regions within the same lobe, when present, into a single lobe. We did this to simplify comparisons between studies employing a variety of neuroimaging methodologies to better illustrate broad commonalities in areas of high correlation. Where and when available, we included correlation coefficients and effect sizes to further illustrate strength of relationships as seen in Table 1. To associate significant region to diagnostic group, we combined studies of similar patient groups [e.g., patients with dementia, brain injury (vascular, traumatic, carbon monoxide), psychosis, affective disorders and personality disorders (MDD, bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder)], substance use disorders, and normal healthy subjects. Table 2 summarizes our findings, which show that EF measures were correlated primarily with measures of the frontal, parietal, and cerebellar lobes. There were fewer correlations to the temporal lobe, and when they did occur, they existed exclusively in the dementia patient group. In studies of patients with brain injury, EF measures were most correlated with measures involving the frontal lobes. Within psychotic and substance use disorders, frontal and cerebellar lobes, and the cingulate gyrus were the most frequently associated with EF deficits. In affective disorders, measures of frontal and parietal lobes were most often correlated with EF measures. In normal healthy individuals, a wider range of lobar correlations to EF was found. These included frontal, parietal, and cerebellar lobes and the cingulate cortex.
TABLE 2. Lobar Relationships to Diagnosis
DementiaBrain InjuryOther NeurologicalPsychosisAffective and PersonalitySubstance UseNormal
Frontal, cingulate cortex7Frontal5Frontal and cerebellum6Frontal, cerebellum11Frontal24Frontal, cerebellar21Frontal, parietal, cerebellar32
Frontal, parietal8Frontal, parietal10Frontal, parietal15Cerebellum28Frontal, Parietal29Cingulate23Frontal9
Frontal12Frontal19Frontal20Anterior cingulate cortex34“Posterior”31Cerebellar30Anterior cingulate, frontal, cerebellar, parietal36
Frontal/prefrontal Frontal26   Frontal14
Frontal, parietal16 Frontal, parietal35   Frontal17
Frontal, parietal22     Frontal33
Frontal, parietal, temporal, amygdala, caudate37     Frontal18
Frontal      
Frontal27      
To examine the associations between individual tests of EF and associated brain region, we identified studies that utilized a single test and applied our lobar method as described above to associated brain regions (Table 3). The TMT (A or B) was associated most frequently with the frontal lobe but in one study was associated with temporal and cerebellar regions. The WCST was associated with the frontal lobe and the cingulum. The Stroop task was associated with frontal and parietal lobes, and cingulum. The Tower of London task was most commonly associated with the frontal lobe but in one study with the parietal lobe and cerebellum. Letter fluency (including controlled oral word association) was examined in two studies and was associated with frontal, parietal, and cerebellar lobes, and subcortical white matter.
TABLE 3. Lobar Relationships to Single-Administered Test of Executive Functioning (EF)
ToLTMT A or BWCSTStroopD-KEFS SortingGo-no-goLetter Fluency
Frontal24Frontal25Frontal20Frontal, cingulate cortex23Frontal27Frontal, cingulum, cerebellum21Frontal, parietal10
Frontal24Frontal5Frontal17Parietal29  Frontal13
Frontal, parietal, cerebellum36Frontal33ACC, frontal, parietal, cerebellum32Frontal, parietal8  Cerebellum28
Frontal9Frontal, cerebellum11ACC34   Posterior white matter31
Frontal5Frontal-parietal-subcortical22     
 Frontal, parietal8     
 Frontal, parietal10     
ACC: Anterior cingulate cortex; D-KEFS: Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System; TMT: Trail Making Test; ToL: Tower of London; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

Discussion

Impairment of EF is an important finding in many neurological and psychiatric conditions because it has been linked to numerous functional and behavioral outcomes.3841 There remains, however, a lack of agreement about its definition, the tests used to approximate it, and its neurobiological substrates. In an effort to address these questions, there has been rapidly increasing interest in correlating measures of EF with brain regions using neuroimaging. The research that has emerged from this area has focused on studying EF within specific clinical populations. There is a need, however, to understand if the findings agree between clinical groups to further support generalizing hypotheses about EF. In this systematic review, aimed to compare the neuroimaging correlates of EF among various clinical populations, our main finding is that the same brain regions (frontal, parietal, and cerebellar) correlate with performance on tests of EF in different clinical populations as well as in healthy individuals. Although the temporal lobes were least often associated with EF in the articles selected for this review, when correlations did exist, they were in patients with dementia and not in patients with other disorders. The occipital lobes were not found to be related at all.
Because EF is not a unitary concept, researchers (e.g., Royall and colleagues,42) have argued that a single measure could not possibly serve as the gold standard in assessing it. Indeed, many factor analytic methods have converged upon three components underlying EF: 1) inhibition and switching,43,44 2) working memory,1,45,46 and 3) attention.47,48 Although attempts have been made at operationalizing these control processes, there continues to be a lack of agreement for a general model of EF. An incomplete understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of EF is, in part, a reason for these difficulties. As such, the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of EF have become areas of increasing scientific investigation and the search for neurobiological markers for cognitive abilities has intensified.
In addition to difficulties defining EF, another issue complicating the study of EF concerns its associations with other domains of cognition. Spearman’s classic theory49,50 posits that general ability (referred to as “g”) underlies performance on a broad range of cognitive tests, including tests of executive function.4951 It has long been recognized that performances on tests of different cognitive abilities are positively correlated, and the concept of “g” has been used to represent the statistical variance among different cognitive tests. An alternative model proposes that overall intelligence (as measured by tests of intelligence; that is, “IQ tests”) reflects the combination of various discrete cognitive processes, rather than a single factor that underlies them (e.g., Thompson 195152). Whether EF reflects or contributes to “g” is a matter of continued debate. Regardless, the notion that EF is psychometrically related to other domains of cognition is well established. Isolating this clinically important cognitive domain with the use of neuropsychological tests, and examining its relation to specific brain regions is, therefore, complicated by the statistical correlation of EF test performance to performance on tests of other cognitive domains.
Not only have conceptual and statistical issues complicated the study of EF, the specific brain regions believed to be most principally involved in EF are becoming increasingly expanded. Classically, the frontal lobes (and prefrontal cortex) have been considered sine qua non of executive function. The most recent studies, however, have supported posterior brain region involvement, including the temporal, parietal, and cerebellar regions. Emerging is the assertion that complex higher-order cognition is represented by networks of meaningful and functionally active circuits. For example, working memory, which refers to the capacity to attend to and update information that is available for manipulation and conscious evaluation, is thought to largely depend on an intact dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.5355 However, working memory also engages attentional systems based in lateral and superior frontoparietal regions that include the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, intraparietal sulcus, and the supramarginal sulcus.56,57
In our review, we found that across a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders, frontal, parietal, and cerebellar regions were consistently associated with EF. Interestingly, a meta-analysis by Rottschy et al.58 uncovered similarly correlated brain regions of increased fMRI activity during a working memory task. Taken together, their findings, similar to ours, might suggest a distributed network of regions involved in executive cognition. For example, information processing that is subserved by the parietal lobes may be required for successful performance of EF tasks. Indeed, lesion studies (e.g., Barbey et al.59) have found associations between parietal lobe lesions and EF task performance. In contrast, other studies have found relatively isolated EF task impairment as a result of ventromedial prefrontal and/or dorsolateral lesions.60,61 Thus, elucidating specific aspects of EF that may require multiple brain areas (and are therefore associated with other cognitive skills) versus those that occur in isolation is an area for future study. Moreover, other brain structures, including the basal ganglia, temporal lobe, and other subcortical structures have also been implicated in EF. These studies continue to fuel further research.
Future studies might utilize other neuroimaging techniques to correlate EF—components within both large- and small-scale networks across diseases and health groups. The cognitive processes emerging from networks, then, span multiple cortical sites connected through afferent and efferent projections to the frontal lobes closely collaborating with each other and having overlapping functions. Complimentary multimodal neuroimaging techniques are set to identify more specific areas that may be part of this large-scale network. DTI and resting state fMRI methods, for example, are beginning to characterize the structural and functional connectivity between brain regions identified by white matter tracts that correlate with EF subdomains.6265
As illustrated in this review, EF impairment has been recognized in a wide variety of neurological and psychiatric disorders. Neurological disease including neurodegenerative processes,6669 traumatic brain injury,10,70 and vascular disease including stroke7173 are only some conditions that have reliably reported deficits in EF. Psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia7476 and depression,7779 for example, have long noted significant changes in executive cognitive ability through the course of the disease. Changes to EF in these conditions and others have been important clinically because of their association to functional decline and disability. Cognitively engaging in and completing goal-directed tasks such as activities of daily living and complex decision-making tasks are known to be impaired in many of these conditions. Accordingly, an improved understanding of the phenomenology and biology of EF will be helpful in forming better diagnostic, treatment, and management strategies.
This review has several limitations. First, although we identified positive associations between tests of executive functioning and several brain regions, the associations were, by and large, weak to moderate. Some reasons for this may include the inherent limitations of the neuroimaging and neuro-computational methods (still early in development) as well as theoretical and practical limitations of test design and administration. Moreover, we caution interpretation of these studies and the correlations between neuropsychological tests and neuroimaging findings together. Because this was not a meta-analysis, comparing correlations found between different studies (particularly studies employing different neuroimaging methods) may be hazardous because it is still not entirely clear whether metabolic activity and atrophy, for example, can be reliably compared. Second, as we have identified several regions of associations, we note that the cerebellum was found to be related to tests of EF in only six of 36 papers. Although the current literature suggests an important role for the cerebellum in cognitive functioning, the results of this review may suggest caution in over-interpreting the role of this specific region as it relates to EF and other brain regions. Finally, a “network” by definition is interconnected. Even though the studies reviewed here have identified common regions, there is no summative evidence that these regions share common physio-biological “connections” per se. Though it is likely that that these connections do exist, it is the work of future research to identify them and describe their physical characteristics.

Conclusions

At the core of this review is our suggestion that impaired EF is a manifestation of a distributed network of brain regions that appear to be affected similarly by a variety of different underlying pathologies and identified by a number of diagnostic modalities (imaging or clinical testing). Various imaging modalities as well as neuropsychological tests of EF have pointed to similar brain regions that may be linked functionally and anatomically. We hope that this study will help point toward different lines of evidence that seem to be converging on a common biological system.

References

1.
Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, et al.: The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol 2000; 41:49–100
2.
Vinken P, Bruyn G: Frontal lobe syndromes. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Amsterdam, North Holland, 1969, pp 725–757
3.
Lyketsos CG: Lessons from neuropsychiatry. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006; 18:445–449
4.
Ueda H, Kitabayashi Y, Narumoto J, et al.: Relationship between clock drawing test performance and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease: a single photon emission computed tomography study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002; 56:25–29
5.
Bergeson AG, Lundin R, Parkinson RB, et al.: Clinical rating of cortical atrophy and cognitive correlates following traumatic brain injury. Clin Neuropsychol 2004; 18:509–520
6.
Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Dobbeleir A, et al.: Cognitive and affective disturbances following focal cerebellar damage in adults: A neuropsychological and SPECT study. Cortex 2010; 46:869-879.
7.
Chang YL, Jacobson MW, Fennema-Notestine C, et al.; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: Level of executive function influences verbal memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and predicts prefrontal and posterior cingulate thickness. Cereb Cortex 2010; 20:1305–1313
8.
Dickerson BC, Wolk DA: Initiative tAasDN. Dysexecutive versus amnesic phenotypes of very mild Alzheimer’s disease are associated with distinct clinical, genetic and cortical thinning characteristics. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2010; 82:45–51
9.
Kaller CP, Rahm B, Spreer J, et al.: Dissociable contributions of left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in planning. Cereb Cortex 2011; 21:307–317
10.
Kinnunen KM, Greenwood R, Powell JH, et al.: White matter damage and cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury. Brain 2011; 134:449–463
11.
Koutsouleris N, Patschurek-Kliche K, Scheuerecker J, et al.: Neuroanatomical correlates of executive dysfunction in the at-risk mental state for psychosis. Schizophr Res 2010; 123:160–174
12.
McDonald CR, Gharapetian L, McEvoy LK, et al.: Relationship between regional atrophy rates and cognitive decline in mild cognitive impairment. NBA 2012; 33:242–253
13.
Pa J, Boxer A, Chao LL, et al.: Clinical-neuroimaging characteristics of dysexecutive mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 2009; 65:414–423
14.
Sasson E, Doniger GM, Pasternak O, et al.: Structural correlates of cognitive domains in normal aging with diffusion tensor imaging. Brain Struct Funct 2012; 217:503–515
15.
Schmitz N, Arkink EB, Mulder M, et al.: Frontal lobe structure and executive function in migraine patients. Neurosci Lett 2008; 440:92–96
16.
Stricker NH, Chang YL, Fennema-Notestine C, et al.; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: distinct profiles of brain and cognitive changes in the very old with Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2011; 77:713–721
17.
Takahashi H, Kato M, Takano H, et al.: Differential contributions of prefrontal and hippocampal dopamine D(1) and D(2) receptors in human cognitive functions. J Neurosci 2008; 28:12032–12038
18.
Toepper M, Gebhardt H, Beblo T, et al.: Functional correlates of distractor suppression during spatial working memory encoding. Neuroscience 2010; 165:1244–1253
19.
Turken AU, Herron TJ, Kang X, et al.: Multimodal surface-based morphometry reveals diffuse cortical atrophy in traumatic brain injury. BMC Med Imaging 2009; 9:20
20.
Wolf RC, Sambataro F, Vasic N, et al.: Longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging of cognition in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Exp Neurol 2011; 231:214–222
21.
Connolly CG, Foxe JJ, Nierenberg J, et al.: The neurobiology of cognitive control in successful cocaine abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend 2012; 121:45–53
22.
Jacobs HIL, Visser PJ, Van Boxtel MPJ, et al. The association between white matter hyperintensities and executive decline in mild cognitive impairment is network dependent. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33:201.e1–201.e8
23.
Nestor LJ, Ghahremani DG, Monterosso J, et al.: Prefrontal hypoactivation during cognitive control in early abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Psychiatry Res 2011; 194:287–295
24.
van Tol MJ, van der Wee NJA, Demenescu LR, et al.: Functional MRI correlates of visuospatial planning in out-patient depression and anxiety. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011; 124:273–284
25.
Hunt A, Haberkorn U, Schröder J, et al.: Neural Correlates of Executive Dysfunction in Prodromal and Manifest Alzheimer’s Disease. GeroPsych 2011; 24:77–81
26.
Chang CC, Lee YC, Chang WN, et al.: Damage of white matter tract correlated with neuropsychological deficits in carbon monoxide intoxication after hyperbaric oxygen therapy. J Neurotrauma 2009; 26:1263–1270
27.
Fine EM, Delis DC, Dean D, et al.: Left frontal lobe contributions to concept formation: a quantitative MRI study of performance on the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Sorting Test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2009; 31:624–631
28.
Segarra N, Bernardo M, Valdes M, et al.: Cerebellar deficits in schizophrenia are associated with executive dysfunction. Neuroreport 2008; 19:1513–1517
29.
Haldane M, Cunningham G, Androutsos C, et al.: Structural brain correlates of response inhibition in Bipolar Disorder I. J Psychopharmacol 2008; 22:138–143
30.
Sim ME, Lyoo IK, Streeter CC, et al. Cerebellar gray matter volume correlates with duration of cocaine use in cocaine-dependent subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007; :2229–2237.
31.
Grant JE, Correia S, Brennan-Krohn T, et al.: Frontal white matter integrity in borderline personality disorder with self-injurious behavior. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007; 19:383–390
32.
Lie C-H, Specht K, Marshall JC, et al.: Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage 2006; 30:1038–1049
33.
Moll J, de Oliveira-Souza R, Moll FT, et al.: The cerebral correlates of set-shifting: an fMRI study of the trail making test. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2002; 60:900–905
34.
Wilmsmeier A, Ohrmann P, Suslow T, et al.: Neural correlates of set-shifting: decomposing executive functions in schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2010; 35:321–329
35.
Schmitz N, Rubia K, Daly E, et al.: Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:7–16
36.
Schall U, Johnston P, Lagopoulos J, et al.: Functional brain maps of Tower of London performance: a positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 2003; 20:1154–1161
37.
Eslinger PJ, Moore P, Anderson C, et al.: Social cognition, executive functioning, and neuroimaging correlates of empathic deficits in frontotemporal dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2011; 23:74–82
38.
Cahn-Weiner DA, Boyle PA, Malloy PF: Tests of executive function predict instrumental activities of daily living in community-dwelling older individuals. Appl Neuropsychol 2002; 9:187–191
39.
Marshall GA, Rentz DM, Frey MT, et al.; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011; 7:300–308
40.
Rosenberg PB, Mielke MM, Appleby B, Oh E, Leoutsakos J-M, Lyketsos CG. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI subtypes: the importance of executive dysfunction. Int J Geriat Psychiatry 2011; 26: 364–372.
41.
Aretouli E, Brandt J: Everyday functioning in mild cognitive impairment and its relationship with executive cognition. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25:224–233
42.
Royall DR, Lauterbach EC, Cummings JL, et al.: Executive control function: a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002; 14:377–405
43.
Barceló F, Knight RT: Both random and perseverative errors underlie WCST deficits in prefrontal patients. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40:349–356
44.
Barceló F, Rubia FJ: Non-frontal P3b-like activity evoked by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroreport 1998; 9:747–751
45.
Baldo JV, Shimamura AP, Delis DC, et al.: Verbal and design fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2001; 7:586–596
46.
Welsh MC, Satterlee-Cartmell T, Stine M: Towers of Hanoi and London: contribution of working memory and inhibition to performance. Brain Cogn 1999; 41:231–242
47.
Barceló F, Muñoz-Céspedes JM, Pozo MA, et al.: Attentional set shifting modulates the target P3b response in the Wisconsin card sorting test. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38:1342–1355
48.
Stuss DT, Shallice T, Alexander MP, et al.: A multidisciplinary approach to anterior attentional functions. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1995; 769:191–211
49.
Spearman C: The Abilities of Man: Their Nature and Measurement. London, MacMillan and Co, 1927
50.
Spearman C: The proof and measurement of association between two things. By C. Spearman, 1904. Am J Psychol 1987; 100:441–471
51.
Nisbett RE, Aronson J, Blair C, et al.: Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. Am Psychol 2012; 67:130–159
52.
Thompson GN: Cerebral area essential to consciousness. Bull Los Angel Neuro Soc 1951; 16:311–334
53.
Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Meyer E, et al.: Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1993; 90:878–882
54.
Owen AM, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, et al.: Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 1990; 28:1021–1034
55.
Jonides J, Smith EE, Koeppe RA, et al.: Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature 1993; 363:623–625
56.
Champod AS, Petrides M: Dissociable roles of the posterior parietal and the prefrontal cortex in manipulation and monitoring processes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007; 104:14837–14842
57.
Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL: The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 2008; 58:306–324
58.
Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, et al.: Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Neuroimage 2012; 60:830–846
59.
Barbey AK, Colom R, Solomon J, et al.: An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain 2012; 135:1154–1164
60.
Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10:295–307
61.
Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, et al.: Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain 2002; 125:624–639
62.
Chen ST, Sultzer DL, Hinkin CH, et al.: Executive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: association with neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional impairment. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998; 10:426–432
63.
Chen T-F, Chen Y-F, Cheng T-W, et al.: Executive dysfunction and periventricular diffusion tensor changes in amnesic mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp 2009; 30:3826–3836
64.
Huang J, Auchus AP: Diffusion tensor imaging of normal appearing white matter and its correlation with cognitive functioning in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1097:259–264
65.
Sjöbeck M, Elfgren C, Larsson E-M, et al.: Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and executive dysfunction. A case-control study on the significance of frontal white matter changes detected by diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2010; 50:260–266
66.
Geschwind DH, Robidoux J, Alarcón M, et al.: Dementia and neurodevelopmental predisposition: cognitive dysfunction in presymptomatic subjects precedes dementia by decades in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 2001; 50:741–746
67.
Salmon E, Lekeu F, Bastin C, et al.: Functional imaging of cognition in Alzheimer’s disease using positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46:1613–1623
68.
Schroeter ML, Vogt B, Frisch S, et al.: Executive deficits are related to the inferior frontal junction in early dementia. Brain 2012; 135:201–215
69.
Teipel SJ, Willoch F, Ishii K, et al.: Resting state glucose utilization and the CERAD cognitive battery in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2006; 27:681–690
70.
Podell K, Gifford K, Bougakov D, et al.: Neuropsychological assessment in traumatic brain injury. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2010; 33:855–876
71.
Waldstein SR, Wendell CR: Neurocognitive function and cardiovascular disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2010; 20:833–842
72.
Debette S, Markus HS: The clinical importance of white matter hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 2010; 341:c3666
73.
Black SE: Vascular cognitive impairment: epidemiology, subtypes, diagnosis and management. J R Coll Physicians Edinb 2011; 41:49–56
74.
Freedman D, Brown AS. The developmental course of executive functioning in schizophrenia. Int J Dev Neurosci 2011; :237–43.
75.
Eisenberg DP, Berman KF. Executive function, neural circuitry, and genetic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; :258–277
76.
Pratt JA, Winchester C, Egerton A, et al.: Modelling prefrontal cortex deficits in schizophrenia: implications for treatment. Br J Pharmacol 2008; 153(Suppl 1):S465–S470
77.
Royall DR: Frontal systems impairment in major depression. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 1999; 4:13–23
78.
Cotter D, Mackay D, Landau S, et al.: Reduced glial cell density and neuronal size in the anterior cingulate cortex in major depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:545–553
79.
Cotter D, Mackay D, Chana G, et al.: Reduced neuronal size and glial cell density in area 9 of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in subjects with major depressive disorder. Cereb Cortex 2002; 12:386–394

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 114 - 125
PubMed: 24763759

History

Received: 18 July 2012
Revision received: 21 December 2012
Accepted: 17 April 2013
Published online: 1 April 2014
Published in print: Spring 2014

Authors

Details

Milap A. Nowrangi, M.D., M.Be.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD.
Constantine Lyketsos, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD.
Vani Rao, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD.
Cynthia A. Munro, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Nowrangi; e-mail: [email protected]

Funding Information

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share