Skip to main content
Full access
Special Articles
Published Online: 28 March 2017

Delusions and the Right Hemisphere: A Review of the Case for the Right Hemisphere as a Mediator of Reality-Based Belief

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Delusions are beliefs that remain fixed despite evidence that they are incorrect. Although the precise neural mechanism of delusional belief remains to be elucidated, there is a predominance of right-hemisphere lesions among patients with delusional syndromes accompanied by structural pathology, suggesting that right-hemisphere lesions, or networks with key nodes in the right hemisphere, may be playing a role. The authors discuss the potential theoretical basis and empiric support for a specific right-hemisphere role in delusion production, drawing on its roles in pragmatic communication; perceptual integration; attentional surveillance and anomaly/novelty detection; and belief updating.
Delusions have fascinated clinicians, researchers and philosophers for centuries. While the DSM-IV-TR required that delusional beliefs be “false,” “based on incorrect inference” and “firmly sustained,” the updated DSM-5 defines delusions on the basis of their fixedness alone, providing no commentary on their veracity or the reasoning used to reach them.1,2 This shift in definitional emphasis, from the content of beliefs to the tenacity with which their proponents cling to them, parallels a historic shift in the general approach to delusions and the mechanisms driving them. While psychoanalytic “motivational” models of delusions once dominated, framing delusions as ego defenses protecting against distressing unconscious conflict with their unique content essential to understanding the psychodynamic processes at play, more recent cognitive neuroscience approaches suggest instead that a single unifying mechanism may be responsible for the broad range of delusional beliefs.3 Within these latter models, it is the way in which the belief is developed and maintained, rather than content of the belief itself, which is most of interest.
William James observed that delusions, in many cases, were “certainly theories which patients invent to account for their abnormal bodily sensations.”4 In their original 1923 paper reporting the “illusion of doubles,” Capgras and Reboul-Lachaux proposed an “agnosia of identification” triggered by disconnect between cognitive and emotional recognition of faces:
Some faces that [the patient] sees with their normal features, the memory of which is not altered in any way, are nevertheless no longer accompanied by this feeling of exclusive familiarity which determines…immediate recognition… The patient, whilst picking up on a very narrow resemblance between two images, ceases to identify them because of the different emotions they elicit. (Capgras & Carrette 1924)5
While Capgras went on to propose a psychodynamic formulation of the syndrome based on oedipal conflict,5 modern studies in patients with what came to be known as the Capgras delusion have borne out his original hypothesis. Using skin conductance response (SCR) as a marker of autonomic activity, Ellis and Young6 found a SCR deficit in Capgras patients presented with familiar faces, a finding that others have replicated.7,8 This model of the Capgras delusion has at its core a fundamental abnormality of “covert,” or affective, facial recognition in the presence of preserved “overt,” or visual, recognition. In some patients with this abnormality—although notably not in all of them9—this discrepancy is explained by way of delusion about a lookalike impostor.
Capgras, a so-called monothematic delusion,10,11 is the best studied of all the specific delusions in large part due to its relative simplicity and the frequency with which it is encountered. Monothematic delusions in general have lent themselves more easily to scientific study due to their highly circumscribed nature. In addition to Capgras, other commonly encountered types of monothematic delusions include Frégoli, in which strangers are believed to be close friends or relatives in disguise12; Cotard, in which the individual feels that he or she is dead13,14; mirror agnosia, in which the individual’s own reflection is believed to be someone else15; reduplicative paramnesia, the belief that a person or place has been duplicated16,17; anosognosia, delusional denial of illness (often left hemiplegia after right hemisphere stroke)18,19 and the related phenomenon of asomatagnosia, in which a body part is denied as one’s own20; delusional jealousy (Othello Syndrome)21; delusional fidelity (Reverse Othello Syndrome)22; and erotomania (de Clérambault syndrome).23
Polythematic delusions, in contrast, involve multiple delusional beliefs, sometimes interrelated, covering a wide range of topics. The complex, multiform delusions professed by famed mathematician John Nash during the height of his psychosis exemplify this phenomenon: Nash believed, at various times, that he was at the center of a secret effort to build a world government and that he would be the Emperor of Antarctica within this government; that his picture was on the cover of Life magazine disguised as Pope John XXIII; that he was the left foot of God and that God was walking the earth through him; and that he was a Go board upon which a “first-order” game was being played by his two sons while a “second-order” game pitted him, as he put it later, “in an ideological conflict between me, personally, and the Jews collectively.”24,25
The deficit in autonomic reactivity observed in Capgras provides support for a general model of delusions as arising out of a core perceptual anomaly, not unlike the model proposed by James over a century ago. Maher26 first suggested that a sufficiently abnormal perception alone might be enough to produce a delusion. Noting that this model would not explain individuals with abnormal perceptions who did not develop delusions, Davies and colleagues proposed a two-factor theory10 which accepted this initial proposition and posited a second abnormality, this time of reasoning: faced with an unusual perceptual experience (“abnormal data”27), the patient in this model develops an explanation that should be rejected but, due to some second failure, erroneously is not.
While the model’s empiric support comes largely from studies of Capgras patients and has been applied primarily to misidentification delusions, its theoretical framework is consistent with and can incorporate other models of delusion. In the “comparator model” of delusions of control, the primary anomalous data are proposed to be a deficit in using “efference copy” generated by corollary discharges from a motor command to predict its sensory consequences, yielding a subsequent mismatch of predicted and experienced behavior that makes possible a belief that the movement was externally controlled.28,29 A similar hypothesis regarding a mismatch between predicted and actual experience of inner speech has been advanced to explain auditory hallucinations and related delusions of alien thought insertion.30,31 In the “aberrant salience” model of schizophreniform psychosis, built on an understanding of dopamine as central to mediating the experience of stimulus significance, a hyperdopaminergic state leads to an inappropriately heightened degree of salience assigned to internal and external events. Delusions arise as a cognitive attempt to explain this powerful, fundamental feeling about an event’s importance.32 Anosognosia, pathologic unawareness of a neurologic deficit, is a delusional denial in which a failure of normal sensory feedback (e.g., from a paralyzed limb, or from the visual system in the case of Anton syndrome) allows for the development of a belief that should be, but is not, rejected on the basis of overwhelming evidence that it is incorrect.33 In all of these cases, as in Capgras, the perceptual anomaly can exist without the delusion and thus must not be sufficient to produce it. A second process interfering with the rejection of implausible beliefs must be invoked.
Coltheart, Davies and colleagues propose that while a delusion can be triggered by any of a variety of abnormal perceptual experiences, the second factor common to all delusions is likely a defective “belief evaluation system” housed in the right frontal lobe.34 Devinsky similarly suggests that delusions arise when an unfettered left hemisphere “creative narrator” is allowed to confabulate explana-tions for experiences without the ongoing “monitoring of self, memory, and familiarity” normally offered by the right frontal lobe. Here, we examine the case for a right hemisphere contribution to delusion production by way of four interrelated lines of evidence relating to its roles in nonverbal communication; perceptual integration; attentional surveillance and anomaly/novelty detection; and belief updating.

The Right Hemisphere and Delusions: A Brief History

When structural or functional imaging abnormality can be demonstrated with delusions, the right hemisphere is frequently implicated.3539 Anosognosia has long been observed to occur disproportionately after right hemisphere stroke as compared with left, as have asomatognosia, in which the paralyzed limb is disowned, and somatoparaphrenia in which there is a delusional belief about the true identity or source of the limb.20 Delusional supernumerary limbs have also been reported with right hemisphere lesions.40 Delusional misidentification syndromes in particular show a right hemisphere association,38,41,42 with specific case reports and series associating right hemisphere pathology with reduplicative paramnesia16,43,44; the Cotard delusion45,46; the Capgras delusion35; mirror agnosia4749; Fregoli syndrome50,51; and Othello syndrome.52,53 In a recent review of 61 case reports of delusional misidentification syndromes associated with specific lesions, Darby and Prasad found right hemisphere lesions in 92% of cases, with right frontal lobe lesions present in 63%.54 Preexisting bilateral hemispheric pathology likely accentuates this effect: Levine et al, in a study of 25 right hemisphere injured patients, found the existence of preexisting brain atrophy to be significant in predicting a delusional syndrome, with no clear significance attributed to lesion size or location within the right hemisphere.55 Several functional imaging studies examining Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients with and without delusions found an association between the presence of delusion and relative right hemisphere (usually right frontal and/or temporal) hypometabolism or hypoperfusion.5659
One meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies examining time perception in healthy controls and schizophrenic patients showed significantly decreased activation of most right hemisphere regions during timing tasks as compared with controls, suggesting a role for right hemisphere dysfunction in the time perception abnormalities of schizophrenia.60 In a series of small studies examining the P300 component of auditory event-related potentials (thought to reflect conscious attention to a stimulus), patients with psychotic depression and delusional misidentification disorders showed reduced right frontal P300 amplitude, and the delusional misidentification patients showed an additional P300 amplitude reduction in the right parietal region as well as increased P300 latency in the central midline brain region.61
Some authors have reasoned that if right hemisphere underactivity allows delusions to occur, then stimulating the right hemisphere might suppress them. Studies of cold water caloric vestibular stimulation (CVS) suggest that this may be true, at least to some extent. CVS produces widespread, largely contralateral hemispheric activation. Left-sided CVS can resolve, transiently, left hemispatial neglect,62,63 somatoparaphrenia,64 and anosognosia65 after right-sided stroke. Levine and colleagues reported improvement in delusions and anosgnosia in schizoaffective disorder and schizophrenia66 after left ear as opposed to right ear CVS, and another group reported a case of improvement in conversion disorder following left CVS.67
But even with multiple lines of inquiry providing highly suggestive circumstantial evidence for a right hemisphere role in delusion production, direct evidence and specific neurophysiologic models of the relationship are lacking. Right hemisphere dysfunction remains a slippery suspect: present at the scene of delusions too often to be chalked up to chance, but not often enough to be implicated directly, and at times occurring without any delusion at all. Here, we explore four of the right hemisphere’s purported functions in depth and suggest that these functions, taken together, subserve a right hemisphere-dominated grip on reality that becomes increasingly tenuous the more impaired these functions become. While right hemisphere lesions likely do not “create” delusions per se, and while there is no clear single anatomic location or network to blame when delusions arise, we suggest that these four right hemisphere functions, when intact, work in tandem to provide at least a partial barrier against delusional belief. When one or a combination of these functions fails, delusions may arise.

Pragmatic Communication

While the left hemisphere enjoyed early celebrity status in the mid- to late 19th century thanks to the work of Dax68 and Broca69 localizing speech and language there, the right hemisphere has since proven itself to be a major mediator of human experience, at times by way of more abstract, less tangible modulatory effects on cognition, emotion, and verbal and behavioral output. Following an initial awareness of its role in visuospatial orientation beginning in the 1940s,70,71 what was previously thought of as the “minor” hemisphere has subsequently become known to play a major role in spatial attention,72 mental manipulation of objects in space73,74; and body image.18,75 Previously dismissed as lacking language, the right hemisphere is crucial for social communication, mediating comprehension of emotional content through interpretation of prosody, facial expressions and gestures.76 It is thought to control spontaneous facial expression of emotion, with the left face shown to express emotion more intensely in healthy people77 and right hemisphere-damaged individuals demonstrating relative reductions in spontaneous facial expressivity.78 Individuals with right hemisphere injuries have difficulty understanding verbal humor,79 idioms,80 and metaphors.81,82 They perform worse than left-hemisphere aphasic patients on tests of connotations of words, even while their understanding of word denotation is generally preserved.83,84 Some functional imaging studies support a role for the right hemisphere in the interpretation of metaphor,85,86 although others have disputed this.87,88 Beyond interpreting content at the sentence level, the right hemisphere is thought to play a key role in organizing complex narrative material: right hemisphere-injured individuals have difficulty making correct inferences89,90; distilling central themes (the “gist” of a narrative) from complex linguistic material9193; integrating elements of a story into a coherent narrative94,95; selecting appropriate endings to jokes79; and assessing plausibility of individual story elements.94 Right hemisphere injured individuals have difficulty distinguishing lies from jokes and have demonstrated deficits in theory of mind.96 Their speech, described by the aphasiologist Myers in 1977 as “copious and inappropriate… confabulatory, irrelevant, literal, and occasionally bizarre,” is adequate at the sentence level of language but fails at its pragmatic function.90,97
While schizophrenia has been associated at various times with both left-98 and right-sided99 dysfunction, numerous studies have identified deficits in the pragmatic aspects of communication and understanding nonliteral speech100102 and facial expressions,103 and it has been suggested that this deficit in discourse-level communication may in fact be a core feature of the illness.104

Perceptual Integration

The right hemisphere is thought to play a dominant role in our ability to integrate disparate perceptions into an overall “gist” or “gestalt” comprehension.93,105 Studies of visuospatial processing using hierarchical visual stimuli—e.g., a large letter made up of smaller letters—have long suggested a model of lateralized function in which the left hemisphere preferentially attends to an item’s component parts (“local” processing) while the right hemisphere attends to the item’s overall contour and gestalt impression (“global” processing).106108 A similar phenomenon is demonstrated in music, with the right hemisphere proposed to play a role in global appreciation of melodic contour and meter while the left deals with the more local features of pitch intervals and rhythm.109,110 Some authors, noting parallels in right hemisphere patients’ visuospatial and verbal deficits, have speculated that these may be two faces of a single central failure of perceptual and ideational integration—i.e., global processing—based in the right hemisphere. Wapner and colleagues suggested that their right hemisphere patients’ difficulties organizing and comprehending narratives might reflect a broader deficit in handling complex ideational materials.94 Benowitz and colleagues, finding a strong correlation between deficits in verbal story recall and visuospatial organization in right hemisphere injured patients, considered the same.91 Myers showed a correlation between visuospatial integration and interpretive language ability, hypothesizing that the tangential, overinclusive speech seen in right hemisphere injury might reflect a higher level difficulty with conceptualizing situations and using contextual cues to distinguish relevant from irrelevant details.97
Devinsky, in a discussion of the spectrum of disorders of body image and ego boundaries found in right hemisphere injury, argued for a dominant right hemisphere role in the most fundamental synthesizing task of all: construction of the corporeal and psychological self.75 Bogousslavsky and Regli described a “response-to-next-patient-stimulation” phenomenon in 11 right hemisphere stroke patients in which these patients were observed to follow commands directed to other patients as though they were directed to them; interpreted by the authors as a variant of perseveration, this behavior might also suggest the presence of impaired ego boundaries in which self and other are not clearly demarcated.111 Here, too, we find parallels in schizophrenia. Patients with schizophrenia have difficulties with complex visuospatial processing112,113 similar to those seen right hemisphere patients. Authors have long suggested a primary role for heteromodal perceptual integration deficits in driving what Borda and Sass have called a disorder of “basic-self experience” or “ipseity disturbance”: here, failure to adequately integrate the multimodal sensory experience of existing as a “self” in reality disrupts a patient’s “grip” or “hold” on that reality.114 Where the hold on reality has been disrupted, delusions can seep in. Postmes and colleagues suggest that such perceptual incoherence creates a “sensory vacuum” into which the brain pours imagined or remembered experiences in an effort to reinstate coherence: “thus, sensory coherence will be restored at the expense of reality monitoring,” and delusions and hallucinations “can be regarded as a ‘solution’ for incomprehensible, incoherent multisensory experiences.”115

Attentional Surveillance, Self-Monitoring, and Novelty/Anomaly Detection

The right hemisphere provides ongoing attentional surveillance of both hemifields in the visuospatial realm72 and is largely responsible for vigilance and detecting novel or incongruent stimuli across all perceptual modalities.116119 It is believed that it serves the same function at a heteromodal, conceptual level as well, providing ongoing monitoring of the self and its relationship to the environment and functioning as what Ramachandran has called an “anomaly detector.”120 The left hemisphere, focused on processing at the local level, seeks to establish order and consistency between individual features; it is, as Gazzaniga writes, “constantly looking for order and reason, even when there is none—which leads it to make mistakes.”121 These mistakes create inconsistencies within the explanatory model and between the model and reality which Ramachandran argues are explained away by the left hemisphere until some anom-aly threshold is reached, at which point the right hemisphere “forces a Kuhnian paradigm shift”120,122 in order to develop an alternate, more workable hypothesis.
There is evidence that the right hemisphere plays a role in novelty detection, and problems with novelty detection have been linked to delusions.123 Novelty detection is a function attributed to the hippocampus, specifically dopamine-related gating at CA1; now, emerging evidence shows that dysfunction in this novelty detection mechanism is related to psychosis and delusion.124,125 Notably, delusions correlated positively with the difference of the functional connectivity of the right hippocampus with the frontal lobe, suggesting that alterations of fronto-limbic novelty processing may contribute to the pathophysiology of delusions in patients with acute psychosis.123
Without appropriate salience given to novel and anomalous stimuli, right hemisphere injured patients are inappropriately blasé about bizarre occurrences and confabulate explanations for how these might fit into a previously established framework. In a story retelling task, while controls and left hemisphere injured patients looked puzzled on hearing nonsensical story elements and left them out on retelling, right hemisphere patients not only readily accepted these odd elements but added justifications for them.94 Rather than being totally insensitive to incongruities in the narrative, right hemisphere patients seemed “at least tangentially aware that something does not fit and yet, are either unwilling or unable to frankly label the anomalous element as such.”94
Anosodiaphoria, the inappropriate lack of concern about one’s illness that can occur with (and often outlasts) anosognosia after right hemisphere stroke, may similarly be understood as a failure to be adequately impressed by the very salient fact of one’s own new neurologic deficit. Studies of insight in Alzheimer’s Disease have shown a correlation between impaired insight and decreased right temporo-occipital perfusion on SPECT imaging126 and right lateral and dorsolateral frontal cortical perfusion on FDG-PET.127 Furthermore, AD patients with lower right insula volume have worse awareness of memory (metamemory).128 In frontotemporal dementia (FTD), patients with right frontal dominant disease present with the behavioral variant of FTD which is characterized with reduced symptom awareness as compared with the nonfluent, agrammatic aphasia FTD patients who have left frontal predominant disease and in whom symptom awareness is more frequently intact.129

Belief Updating

The related tasks of recognizing that an explanatory model has become outdated and shifting allegiance to a new, more workable one comprise the function of belief updating. The frontal lobes facilitate changing cognitive set, with the right frontal lobe in particular dominant for updating beliefs and avoiding repetitive responses.130,131 The right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is thought to play a major role in problem-solving in complex, “ill-structured” situations.132 Drake and colleagues, in a series of studies in healthy individuals, showed that counter-attitudinal messages were more persuasive, and disagreeing statements more readily recalled, when heard from the left133,134; they hypothesized this represented increased openness to cognitive set adjustment with relatively increased right hemisphere activation. In studies using mixed-handedness as a marker for relatively stronger nondominant hemisphere influence,135 mixed-handers were found to be more gullible and easily persuaded136; more apt to experience sensory illusions137; more prone to magical thinking138; and more likely to internalize false personality trait characterizations139 than strong left- or right-handers. Strong-handers, meanwhile, are suggested to be less sensation-seeking140; more likely to prefer authoritarianism and conservative politics140,141; and more likely to retain beliefs in creationism from childhood despite extensive scientific evidence for evolution.140 Sharot and colleagues showed an increased ability to incorporate new negative information into preexisting belief frameworks after transient disruption of the left—but not right—inferior frontal gyrus with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS).142 Cacioppa, Petty and Quintanar, using electroencephalography (EEG) to monitor cortical activity during exposure to pro- and counter-attitudinal beliefs, showed a relative shift in activity from left to right as subjects considered issues for longer periods of time.143
Patients with right hemisphere injuries, predictably, have difficulty updating their beliefs. In 16 unilateral anterior temporal lobectomy patients given a problem solving task, Rausch found that while all patients had difficulty solving problems as compared with controls, patients with left temporal lobectomies were more likely to shift from a hypothesis even when it was correct, while right temporal lobectomy patients tended to maintain a hypothesis even when told it was not.144 Right hemisphere patients perseverate more than left hemisphere patients on measures of design fluency145 and number fluency146 and they have more difficulty suppressing previously learned cognitive sets when switching tasks.147 On the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), an executive function task known to produce strong activation in the DLPFC, particularly on the right,148 patients with right frontal lobe tumors and patients with schizophrenia made perseverative errors at a rate that was equal to each other and significantly greater than that of normal controls and patients with left frontal and nonfrontal tumors.149 Another functional imaging study examining WCST performance after head trauma showed an inverse relationship between perseverative responses and metabolism in the right, but not left, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and caudate nucleus.150

The Right Hemisphere at the Interface of Self, Environment, and Reality

Woven together, these threads reveal a picture of a right hemisphere that is essential for our ability to create and maintain accurate appraisals of mental objects holistically and in context—be they simple visuospatial figures, complex narratives, or the self. Returning to the two-factor theory of delusions, it follows that a primary somatic/perceptual abnormality creates an inconsistency in a previously functional explanatory model, which the relatively preserved left hemisphere does its best to explain while keeping the model intact. Overly drawn to the left hemisphere task of connecting individual dots, the right hemisphere injured patient is unable to appreciate that the picture thus created is bizarre and incoherent. If anomalies are noted, they lack the cognitive and emotional valence usually accorded to strange or surprising occurrences and do not capture the attention the way they should.
Sass and Byrom have described this phenomenon in schizophrenia as an “anything-goes orientation” in which patients “quickly identify, accept and take in stride phenomena that most people would find anomalous.”151 In the recent past, the default mode network (DMN) has gained attention as a possible mediator of function and dysfunction of ‘real-time’ thought and belief monitoring and constraints. The DMN is associated with daydreaming, imaginative planning, and stimulus-independent reflection, perhaps acting as a threshold between consciousness and behavior. Studies of patients with schizophrenia consistently demonstrate DMN hyperactivation (i.e., impaired suppression) on a variety of cognitive tasks152 as well as decreased connectivity to task-positive right frontal networks.153,154 Sass and Byrom proposed that an overactive default mode network (DMN) might be partly responsible for the “hyposalience” attributed to experiences by schizophrenic patients - experiences that should trigger alarm bells for strangeness but nevertheless do not.151 Gerrans, drawing on studies demonstrating an anticorrelation between DMN activity and activity in task-focused networks, has proposed a model in which the task-negative DMN is inhibited (“supervised”) by right prefrontal task-positive networks; when these fail, a person with a disinhibited DMN is free to generate a range of beliefs across a broad spectrum of likelihood, without the constraints usually imposed by the reality-based, environment-surveying right prefrontal cortex.155 Unimpressed by major inconsistencies and unable to revise beliefs, patients cling to old explanatory models while simultaneously acknowledging, either explicitly or implicitly, the existence of contradictory information. This may explain, in part, the widely observed phenomenon in delusional patients referred to as “double bookkeeping,” in which the delusion is upheld even while other statements or behaviors suggest that the patient, on some level, knows it is not true.156

Discussion

It is probably not the case that right hemisphere lesions directly cause false beliefs; more likely, without the complex cognitive skill set normally offered by the right frontal lobe, there may be fewer barriers preventing their occurrence. Our experience of reality is mediated in part by the stories we tell ourselves to explain it. With deficits in comprehension of metaphor, difficulty interpreting nonverbal conversational cues, and impaired attention to unexpected events, right hemisphere patients are at a disadvantage when it comes to collecting the evidence they need to build a workable explanatory hypothesis for any unusual or novel experience; and having constructed a hypothesis, they are unable to evaluate its validity in context of their preexisting knowledge. Their tangential, over-inclusive speech and difficulty with organizing complex narrative materials likely reflects a core deficit in filtering out irrelevant data – even when those data are their own memories. Because of their difficulties with nonliteral communication and affective regulation, their friends and loved ones really do behave differently around them, further widening the gap between the patient’s expectation of the world and what is actually experienced and making it even more difficult for the patient to keep up with reality.
It is important to mention here, as part of a complete discussion of false beliefs arising after brain injury, the phenomenon of confabulation. Notably described by Korsakoff as “pseudoreminiscences” occurring in patients with chronic alcohol use, seemingly out of proportion to the degree of cognitive impairment otherwise present, confabulation was historically thought of in terms of memory dysfunction with false or distorted memories arising to fill amnestic patients’ gaps in recall.157 Berlyne, in 1972, defined confabulation as “a falsification of memory occurring in clear consciousness in association with an organically derived amnesia.”158
Not all amnestic patients confabulate, however, and subsequent treatments of the topic have introduced the importance of ongoing reality-monitoring for effective use of memories relevant to the individual’s current situation. Kopelman first distinguished between “spontaneous” and “provoked” confabulation, arguing that the former likely required some frontal dysfunction in addition to a memory deficit, whereas the latter might be a normal response to the experience of impaired memory.159 In a subsequent study of 11 brain injured patients, Bajo, Kopelman and colleagues found an association between severity of confabulation and severity of memory impairment and executive dysfunction.160 Schnider notes that spontaneous confabulation “constitutes a syndrome of profound derangement of thought,” rather than of memory per se, “in which the concept of ongoing reality in thinking and planning is dominated by a patient’s past experiences and habits rather than true ongoing reality; the confabulations are simply the verbal manifestation of the thought disorder.”161 Noting that cases of spontaneous confabulation reported in the literature invariably involve lesions of the anterior limbic structures, and specifically the posteromedial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and its connections, he suggests a role for this set of structures in monitoring ongoing reality and “constantly suppressing activated, but currently irrelevant, memories.”161 Confabulation, then, becomes a frontally-mediated disorder of distinguishing “now” from “not-now,”162 in which memories and associations are allowed to bubble to the surface of conscious experience in a disorganized, “incoherent and context-free” way.159 Gilboa has proposed an overarching failure of “strategic retrieval” of memories in confabulation, of which temporal confusion is one symptom.163 He describes two interacting memory evaluation systems: an intuitive “feeling of rightness” attached to retrieved memories based on how well they fit with an overall cognitive schema; and a conscious monitoring process that checks these memories for internal and current contextual coherence. The first judgment, “rapid, automatic, and relatively impenetrable to reasoning,” is thought to be housed in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC); the latter system, in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). When this system breaks down, confabulation may occur.163 Cabeza and colleagues, showing increased left prefrontal cortex (PFC) activity on PET imaging of recall tasks and increased right PFC activity on recall tasks, similarly hypothesized a “production-monitoring” framework in which the left PFC generates semantically guided information “whereas the right PFC is more involved in monitoring operations, including the evaluation and verification of recovered information.”164
Whether or not delusions and confabulation in neurologic patients are in fact two distinct processes, or if they are merely variations along a single spectrum, has yet to be agreed upon in the literature. At minimum, it is likely that their mechanisms overlap, and they may interact with each other. Coltheart takes this approach in a discussion of provoked confabulation—instances in which the confabulated content is offered not spontaneously, but in response to some question or task—noting that delusional patients, amnestic patients, and healthy controls all can be shown to confabulate when asked to provide explanations for their own behavior in instances where a good explanation is lacking.165 Using Capgras as an example, he suggests that the patient, deeply attached to a belief in an imposter but with no plausible conscious explanation for this belief, might confabulate evidence to explain it. Linking this to Gopnik’s discussion of the human “drive for causal knowledge” (the successful culmination of which she likens to orgasm),166 Coltheart comments that the satisfaction derived from reaching an explanation for a previously unexplained behavior may override any explanatory implausibility: “it is better to have an explanation for a piece of one’s behaviour—any explanation, no matter how bizarre—than to have none.”165
One theme common to discussions of both delusions and confabulation is that of a two-step process in which there is an initial automatic, preconscious, autonomic or affective experience; and a secondary conscious mechanism in which this experience is evaluated with reference to a larger context. Linking these two steps, in most instances, is a thought; but whether the affective experience generates the thought (as in Ellis and Young6; Kapur32; and Davies et al.10), or the thought is experienced as entering consciousness already somatically or affectively tagged (as in Gilboa167 and Damasio168) remains yet to be determined.
The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) looms large in all of these conversations. The idea that ‘covert’, affective, autonomic, unconscious bodily processes might surreptitiously influence conscious decision-making has been advanced most notably by Damasio, whose “somatic marker hypothesis” proposes that emotion, as registered in the brain by its association with transient autonomic and visceral changes, alters conscious cognition at an unconscious level.168 Damasio argues that the VMPFC is essential for linking unconscious, affective awareness with conscious cognition, likely due to its strong reciprocal connections with the hippocampus and amygdala.168,169 Experimentally, patients with VMPFC injury do not generate appropriate skin conductance responses (SCRs) when shown emotionally charged stimuli.170 On a gambling task, they cannot adjust their behavior to account for biases that are too subtle to identify overtly but that nevertheless drive normal controls and patients without frontal injury to alter their behavior.171 Outside of the laboratory, these patients cannot link preconscious emotional awareness and intuition with conscious decision-making, leading to poor choices particularly in the social and interpersonal realm as well as in risk assessment and outcome prediction; in the lab and in life, they continue to play from “bad decks” long after everyone else has perceived a bias and changed course. The VMPFC almost certainly plays a central role in drawing upon the contextual cognitive and somatic “knowledge” held in the brain and body, respectively, in order to interact with the current environment in a way that makes sense. The CA1 region of the hippocampus is monosynaptically connected to the VMPFC172; with the CA1 region being crucial to comparator function/ novelty detection and evaluation,173,174 the VMPFC is part of this critical circuit for reality evaluation. It is clear, however, that injury to the VMPFC alone is not enough to produce delusions. The DLPFC is typically implicated in measures of executive function and the right DLPFC in particular has been shown to play a role in suppressing perseverative behaviors, navigating complex situations, and exerting “inhibitory cognitive control on affective impulses, being therefore particularly critical to limiting the influences of impulses in decision making behavior.”175 It is possible that the DLPFC provides the “top-down” experiential monitoring counterpart to the VMPFC’s “bottom-up” ‘intuitive’ sense that is not accessible to consciousness, with delusions being allowed to arise when there are lesions to both of these cortices or to essential connections between the two. Papageorgiou’s findings of reduced right frontal P300 amplitudes in both patients with delusional misidentification syndrome (DMS) and psychotic depression, but increased midline P300 latency only in DMS, would seem to support this multifocal model.176 When this injury or disconnection occurs on the right, the right hemisphere-specific deficits detailed above may make it difficult for patients to compensate when interpreting the world around them.
Delusions then, are neither a necessary outcome of right hemisphere injury nor solely dependent on the right hemisphere for their production; but it is significantly easier for them to emerge when failures of pragmatic communication, perceptual integration, attentional surveillance, and belief updating are superimposed on impairments of executive function and preconscious autonomic processing. It is not clear whether any one deficit is more important for the production of delusions than the others, and it may be the case that different combinations of deficits produce different delusional presentations. Future experimental work in this area will continue to clarify specific right hemisphere networks and their contribution to confabulated, delusional, and nonpathologic belief, and further extend our understanding of the fragile nature of our relationship to reality and the remarkable resourcefulness of the injured brain.

References

1.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed., Text Rev. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2000
2.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013
3.
McKay R, Langdon R, Coltheart M: “Sleights of mind”: delusions, defences, and self-deception. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2005; 10:305–326
4.
James W: Principles of Psychology. New York, Holt, 1890
5.
Capgras J, Carrette P: Illusion des sosies et complexe d’Oedipe. Ann Med Psychol (Paris) 1924; 82:48–68
6.
Ellis HD, Young AW, Quayle AH, et al: Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras delusion. Proc Biol Sci 1997; 264:1085–1092
7.
Hirstein W, Ramachandran VS: Capgras syndrome: a novel probe for understanding the neural representation of the identity and familiarity of persons. Proc Biol Sci 1997; 264:437–444
8.
Brighetti G, Bonifacci P, Borlimi R, et al: “Far from the heart far from the eye”: evidence from the Capgras delusion. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2007; 12:189–197
9.
Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Double dissociation between overt and covert face recognition. J Cogn Neurosci 1995; 7:425–432
10.
Davies M, Breen N, Coltheart M, et al: Monothematic delusions: towards a two-factor account. Philos Psychiatry Psychol 2001; 8:133–158
11.
Coltheart M: On the distinction between monothematic and polythematic delusions. Mind Lang 2013; 28:103–112
12.
Ellis HD, Whitley J, Luauté JP: Delusional misidentification. The three original papers on the Capgras, Frégoli and intermetamorphosis delusions (Classic Text No. 17). Hist Psychiatry 1994; 5:117–146
13.
Debruyne H, Portzky M, Van den Eynde F, et al: Cotard’s syndrome: a review. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2009; 11:197–202
14.
Gardner-Thorpe C, Pearn J: The Cotard syndrome. Report of two patients: with a review of the extended spectrum of ‘délire des négations’. Eur J Neurol 2004; 11:563–566
15.
Breen N, Caine D, Coltheart M: Mirrored-self misidentification: two cases of focal onset dementia. Neurocase 2001; 7:239–254
16.
Benson DF, Gardner H, Meadows JC: Reduplicative paramnesia. Neurology 1976; 26:147–151
17.
Politis M, Loane C: Reduplicative paramnesia: a review. Psychopathology 2012; 45:337–343
18.
Roth M: Disorders of the body image caused by lesions of the right parietal lobe. Brain 1949; 72:89–111
19.
Heilman KM. Anosognosia: possible neuropsychological mechanisms; in Awareness of Deficit After Brain Injury: Clinical and Theoretical Issues. Edited by Prigatano GP, Schacter DL. New York, Oxford University Press, pp 53–62, 1991
20.
Feinberg TE, Venneri A, Simone AM, et al: The neuroanatomy of asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2010; 81:276–281
21.
Todd J, Dewhurst K: The Othello syndrome; a study in the psychopathology of sexual jealousy. J Nerv Ment Dis 1955; 122:367–374
22.
Butler PV: Reverse Othello syndrome subsequent to traumatic brain injury. Psychiatry 2000; 63:85–92
23.
Hollender MH, Callahan AS 3rd: Erotomania or de Clérambault syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1975; 32:1574–1576
24.
Capps D: John Nash’s delusional decade: a case of paranoid schizophrenia. Pastoral Psychol 2004; 52:193–218
25.
Nasar S: A Beautiful Mind. New York, Simon and Schuster, 2011
26.
Maher B: Delusional thinking and cognitive disorder. Integr Physiol Behav Sci 2005; 40:136–146
27.
Coltheart M, Menzies P, Sutton J: Abductive inference and delusional belief. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2010; 15:261–287
28.
Frith C: Explaining delusions of control: the comparator model 20 years on. Conscious Cogn 2012; 21:52–54
29.
Feinberg I: Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophr Bull 1978; 4:636–640
30.
Maher B: Schizophrenia, aberrant utterance and delusions of control: the disconnection of speech and thought, and the connection of experience and belief. Mind Lang 2003; 18:1–22
31.
Frith CD: The positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia reflect impairments in the perception and initiation of action. Psychol Med 1987; 17:631–648
32.
Kapur S: Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:13–23
33.
Davies M, Davies AA, Coltheart M: Anosognosia and the two‐factor theory of delusions. Mind Lang 2005; 20:209–236
34.
Coltheart M, Langdon R, McKay R: Schizophrenia and monothematic delusions. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:642–647
35.
Ellis HD: The role of the right hemisphere in the Capgras delusion. Psychopathology 1994; 27:177–185
36.
Feinberg TE, Deluca J, Giacino J, Roane D, Solms M. Right-hemisphere pathology and the self: delusional misidentification and reduplication; in The Lost Self: Pathologies of the Brain and Identity. Edited by Feinberg TE, Keenan JP. New York, Oxford University Press, pp 100–130, 2005
37.
Ortigue S, Bianchi-Demicheli F: Intention, false beliefs, and delusional jealousy: insights into the right hemisphere from neurological patients and neuroimaging studies. Med Sci Monit 2011; 17:RA1–RA11
38.
Feinberg TE, Shapiro RM: Misidentification-reduplication and the right hemisphere. Cogn Behav Neurol 1989; 2:39–48
39.
Silva JA, Leong GB, Wine DB: Misidentification delusions, facial misrecognition, and right brain injury. Can J Psychiatry 1993; 38:239–241
40.
Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Wade DT: Three arms: a case study of supernumerary phantom limb after right hemisphere stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993; 56:159–166
41.
Malloy PF, Richardson ED: The frontal lobes and content-specific delusions. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:455–466
42.
Cutting J: Delusional misidentification and the role of the right hemisphere in the appreciation of identity. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1991; (14):70–75
43.
Hakim H, Verma NP, Greiffenstein MF: Pathogenesis of reduplicative paramnesia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988; 51:839–841
44.
Lee K, Shinbo M, Kanai H, et al: Reduplicative paramnesia after a right frontal lesion. Cogn Behav Neurol 2011; 24:35–39
45.
Nishio Y, Mori E. Delusions of death in a patient with right hemisphere infarction. Cogn Behav Neurol 2012; 25:216–223.
46.
Young AW, Robertson IH, Hellawell DJ, et al: Cotard delusion after brain injury. Psychol Med 1992; 22:799–804
47.
Ramachandran VS, Altschuler EL, Hillyer S: Mirror agnosia. Proc Biol Sci 1997; 264:645–647
48.
Villarejo A, Martin VP, Moreno-Ramos T, et al: Mirrored-self misidentification in a patient without dementia: evidence for right hemispheric and bifrontal damage. Neurocase 2011; 17:276–284
49.
Priftis K, Rusconi E, Umiltà C, et al: Pure agnosia for mirror stimuli after right inferior parietal lesion. Brain 2003; 126:908–919
50.
de Pauw KW, Szulecka TK, Poltock TL: Frégoli syndrome after cerebral infarction. J Nerv Ment Dis 1987; 175:433–438
51.
Young AW, Flude BM, Ellis AW: Delusional misidentification incident in a right hemisphere stroke patient. Behav Neurol 1991; 4:81–87
52.
Narumoto J, Nakamura K, Kitabayashi Y, et al: Othello syndrome secondary to right orbitofrontal lobe excision. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006; 18:560–561
53.
Richardson ED, Malloy PF, Grace J: Othello syndrome secondary to right cerebrovascular infarction. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1991; 4:160–165
54.
Darby R, Prasad S: Lesion-related delusional misidentification syndromes: a comprehensive review of reported cases. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2016; 28:217–222
55.
Levine DN, Grek A: The anatomic basis of delusions after right cerebral infarction. Neurology 1984; 34:577–582
56.
Staff RT, Shanks MF, Macintosh L, et al: Delusions in Alzheimer’s disease: spet evidence of right hemispheric dysfunction. Cortex 1999; 35:549–560
57.
Sultzer DL, Brown CV, Mandelkern MA, et al: Delusional thoughts and regional frontal/temporal cortex metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:341–349
58.
Geroldi C, Akkawi NM, Galluzzi S, et al: Temporal lobe asymmetry in patients with Alzheimer’s disease with delusions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000; 69:187–191
59.
Nakano S, Yamashita F, Matsuda H, et al: Relationship between delusions and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006; 21:16–21
60.
Ortuño F, Guillén-Grima F, López-García P, et al: Functional neural networks of time perception: challenge and opportunity for schizophrenia research. Schizophr Res 2011; 125:129–135
61.
Papageorgiou C, Ventouras E, Lykouras L, et al: Psychophysiological evidence for altered information processing in delusional misidentification syndromes. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2003; 27:365–372
62.
Rubens AB: Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect. Neurology 1985; 35:1019–1024
63.
Adair JC, Na DL, Schwartz RL, et al: Caloric stimulation in neglect: evaluation of response as a function of neglect type. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003; 9:983–988
64.
Bisiach E, Rusconi ML, Vallar G: Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 1991; 29:1029–1031
65.
Cappa S, Sterzi R, Vallar G, et al: Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 1987; 25:775–782
66.
Levine J, Toder D, Geller V, et al: Beneficial effects of caloric vestibular stimulation on denial of illness and manic delusions in schizoaffective disorder: a case report. Brain Stimulat 2012; 5:267–273
67.
Noll-Hussong M, Holzapfel S, Pokorny D, et al: Caloric vestibular stimulation as a treatment for conversion disorder: a case report and medical hypothesis. Front Psychiatry 2014; 5:63
68.
Dax M. Lésions de la moitié gauche de l'encéphale coïncidant avec l'oubli des signes de la pensée. Gaz Hebdomadaire Méd Chirurgie 1865; 2:259–262.
69.
Broca P: Perte de la parole, ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lobe antérieur gauche du cerveau. Bull Soc Anthropol 1861; 2:235–238
70.
Brain WR: Visual orientation with special reference to lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain 1941; 41:244-272
71.
Paterson A, Zangwill O: Disorders of visual space perception associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain 1944; 67:331–358
72.
Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T: Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology 1980; 30:327–330
73.
Ratcliff G: Spatial thought, mental rotation and the right cerebral hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 1979; 17:49–54
74.
Corballis MC: Mental rotation and the right hemisphere. Brain Lang 1997; 57:100–121
75.
Devinsky O: Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense of corporeal and emotional self. Epilepsy Behav 2000; 1:60–73
76.
Blonder LX, Bowers D, Heilman KM: The role of the right hemisphere in emotional communication. Brain 1991; 114:1115–1127
77.
Sackeim HA, Gur RC, Saucy MC: Emotions are expressed more intensely on the left side of the face. Science 1978; 202:434–436
78.
Blonder LX, Burns AF, Bowers D, et al: Right hemisphere facial expressivity during natural conversation. Brain Cogn 1993; 21:44–56
79.
Brownell HH, Michel D, Powelson J, et al: Surprise but not coherence: sensitivity to verbal humor in right-hemisphere patients. Brain Lang 1983; 18:20–27
80.
Myers PS, Linebaugh CW. Comprehension of idiomatic expressions by right-hemisphere-damaged adults; in Clinical Aphasiology: Conference Proceedings. Edited by Brookshire RH. Minneapolis, BRK Publishers, 1981
81.
Winner E, Gardner H: The comprehension of metaphor in brain-damaged patients. Brain 1977; 100:717–729
82.
Brownell HH, Simpson TL, Bihrle AM, et al: Appreciation of metaphoric alternative word meanings by left and right brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia 1990; 28:375–383
83.
Brownell HH, Potter HH, Michelow D, et al: Sensitivity to lexical denotation and connotation in brain-damaged patients: a double dissociation? Brain Lang 1984; 22:253–265
84.
Gardner H, Denes G: Connotative judgements by aphasic patients on a pictorial adaptation of the semantic differential. Cortex 1973; 9:183–196
85.
Bottini G, Corcoran R, Sterzi R, et al: The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language. A positron emission tomography activation study. Brain 1994; 117:1241–1253
86.
Mashal N, Faust M, Hendler T: The role of the right hemisphere in processing nonsalient metaphorical meanings: application of principal components analysis to fMRI data. Neuropsychologia 2005; 43:2084–2100
87.
Rapp AM, Leube DT, Erb M, et al: Laterality in metaphor processing: lack of evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging for the right hemisphere theory. Brain Lang 2007; 100:142–149
88.
Lee SS, Dapretto M: Metaphorical vs. literal word meanings: fMRI evidence against a selective role of the right hemisphere. Neuroimage 2006; 29:536–544
89.
Bryan KL: Assessment of language disorders after right hemisphere damage. Br J Disord Commun 1988; 23:111–125
90.
Brownell HH, Potter HH, Bihrle AM, et al: Inference deficits in right brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang 1986; 27:310–321
91.
Benowitz LI, Moya KL, Levine DN: Impaired verbal reasoning and constructional apraxia in subjects with right hemisphere damage. Neuropsychologia 1990; 28:231–241
92.
Hough MS: Narrative comprehension in adults with right and left hemisphere brain-damage: theme organization. Brain Lang 1990; 38:253–277
93.
Beeman M, Chiarello C: Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension: Perspectives From Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, 1998
94.
Wapner W, Hamby S, Gardner H: The role of the right hemisphere in the apprehension of complex linguistic materials. Brain Lang 1981; 14:15–33
95.
Delis DC, Wapner W, Gardner H, et al: The contribution of the right hemisphere to the organization of paragraphs. Cortex 1983; 19:43–50
96.
Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, et al: Distinguishing lies from jokes: theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang 1998; 62:89–106
97.
Myers PS: Profiles of communication deficits in patients with right cerebral hemisphere damage: implications for diagnosis and treatment; in Clinical Aphasiology Conference Proceedings. Edited by Brookshire RH. Minneapolis, BRK Publishers, 1979
98.
Gur RE: Left hemisphere dysfunction and left hemisphere overactivation in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 1978; 87:226–238
99.
Cutting JC: Evidence for Right Hemisphere Dysfunction in Schizophrenia. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, 1994
100.
Langdon R, Coltheart M, Ward PB, et al: Disturbed communication in schizophrenia: the role of poor pragmatics and poor mind-reading. Psychol Med 2002; 32:1273–1284
101.
Mitchell RL, Crow TJ: Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: the forgotten hemisphere? Brain 2005; 128:963–978
102.
Schettino A, Lauro LR, Crippa F, et al: The comprehension of idiomatic expressions in schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:1032–1040
103.
Borod JC, Martin CC, Alpert M, et al: Perception of facial emotion in schizophrenic and right brain-damaged patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 1993; 181:494–502
104.
Bambini V, Arcara G, Bechi M, et al: The communicative impairment as a core feature of schizophrenia: frequency of pragmatic deficit, cognitive substrates, and relation with quality of life. Compr Psychiatry 2016; 71:106–120
105.
Robertson LC, Lamb MR: Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization. Cognit Psychol 1991; 23:299–330
106.
Van Kleeck MH: Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: new data and a meta-analysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia 1989; 27:1165–1178
107.
Devinsky O: Delusional misidentifications and duplications: right brain lesions, left brain delusions. Neurology 2009; 72:80–87
108.
Fink GR, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, et al: Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual stimuli. Brain 1997; 120:1779–1791
109.
Brust JC: Music and the neurologist. A historical perspective. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2001; 930:143–152
110.
Peretz I: Processing of local and global musical information by unilateral brain-damaged patients. Brain 1990; 113:1185–1205
111.
Bogousslavsky J, Regli F: Response-to-next-patient-stimulation: a right hemisphere syndrome. Neurology 1988; 38:1225–1227
112.
Ferman TJ, Primeau M, Delis D, et al: Global-local processing in schizophrenia: hemispheric asymmetry and symptom-specific interference. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1999; 5:442–451
113.
Schweitzer L: Evidence of right cerebral hemisphere dysfunction in schizophrenic patients with left hemisphere overactivation. Biol Psychiatry 1982; 17:655–673
114.
Borda JP, Sass LA: Phenomenology and neurobiology of self disorder in schizophrenia: primary factors. Schizophr Res 2015; 169:464–473
115.
Postmes L, Sno HN, Goedhart S, et al: Schizophrenia as a self-disorder due to perceptual incoherence. Schizophr Res 2014; 152:41–50
116.
Pardo JV, Fox PT, Raichle ME: Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature 1991; 349:61–64
117.
Goldberg E, Costa LD: Hemisphere differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems. Brain Lang 1981; 14:144–173
118.
Goldberg E, Podell K, Lovell M: Lateralization of frontal lobe functions and cognitive novelty. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:371–378
119.
Heilman KM, Bowers D, Valenstein E, et al: The right hemisphere: neuropsychological functions. J Neurosurg 1986; 64:693–704
120.
Ramachandran VS: Anosognosia in parietal lobe syndrome. Conscious Cogn 1995; 4:22–51
121.
Gazzaniga MS: The split brain revisited. Sci Am 1998; 279:50–55
122.
Ramachandran VS: The evolutionary biology of self-deception, laughter, dreaming and depression: some clues from anosognosia. Med Hypotheses 1996; 47:347–362
123.
Schott BH, Voss M, Wagner B, et al: Fronto-limbic novelty processing in acute psychosis: disrupted relationship with memory performance and potential implications for delusions. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:144
124.
Lisman JE, Pi HJ, Zhang Y, et al: A thalamo-hippocampal-ventral tegmental area loop may produce the positive feedback that underlies the psychotic break in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 68:17–24
125.
Lisman JE, Otmakhova NA: Storage, recall, and novelty detection of sequences by the hippocampus: elaborating on the SOCRATIC model to account for normal and aberrant effects of dopamine. Hippocampus 2001; 11:551–568
126.
Ott BR, Lafleche G, Whelihan WM, et al: Impaired awareness of deficits in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1996; 10:68–76
127.
Harwood DG, Sultzer DL, Feil D, et al: Frontal lobe hypometabolism and impaired insight in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005; 13:934–941
128.
Cosentino S, Brickman AM, Griffith E, et al: The right insula contributes to memory awareness in cognitively diverse older adults. Neuropsychologia 2015; 75:163–169
129.
Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, et al: Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology 2011; 76:1006–1014
130.
Brugger P, Monsch AU, Johnson SA: Repetitive behavior and repetition avoidance: the role of the right hemisphere. J Psychiatry Neurosci 1996; 21:53–56
131.
Fiore SM, Schooler JW. Right hemisphere contributions to creative problem solving: converging evidence for divergent thinking; in Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension: Perspectives From Cognitive Neuroscience. Edited by Beeman M, Chiarello C. Hove, United Kingdom, Psychology Press, pp 349–371, 1998
132.
Gilbert SJ, Zamenopoulos T, Alexiou K, et al: Involvement of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ill-structured design cognition: an fMRI study. Brain Res 2010; 1312:79–88
133.
Drake RA: Processing persuasive arguments: recall and recognition as a function of agreement and manipulated activation asymmetry. Brain Cogn 1991; 15:83–94
134.
Drake RA, Bingham BR: Induced lateral orientation and persuasibility. Brain Cogn 1985; 4:156–164
135.
Prichard E, Propper RE, Christman SD. Degree of handedness, but not direction, is a systematic predictor of cognitive performance. Front Psychol 2013; 4:9
136.
Christman SD, Henning BR, Geers AL, et al: Mixed-handed persons are more easily persuaded and are more gullible: inter-hemispheric interaction and belief updating. Laterality 2008; 13:403–426
137.
Niebauer CL, Aselage J, Schutte C: Hemispheric interaction and consciousness: degree of handedness predicts the intensity of a sensory illusion. Laterality: asymmetries of body. Brain Cogn 2002; 7:85–96
138.
Barnett KJ, Corballis MC: Ambidexterity and magical ideation. Laterality: asymmetries of body. Brain Cogn 2002; 7:75–84
139.
Jasper JD, Prothero M, Christman SD: I’m not sexist!!! Cognitive dissonance and the differing cries of mixed-and strong-handers. Pers Individ Dif 2009; 47:268–272
140.
Christman S: Individual differences in personality as a function of degree of handedness: consistent-handers are less sensation seeking, more authoritarian, and more sensitive to disgust. Laterality: asymmetries of body. Brain Cogn 2014; 19:354–367
141.
Lyle KB, Grillo MC: Consistent-handed individuals are more authoritarian. Laterality: asymmetries of body. Brain Cogn 2014; 19:146–163
142.
Sharot T, Kanai R, Marston D, et al: Selectively altering belief formation in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109:17058–17062
143.
Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Quintanar LR: Individual differences in relative hemispheric alpha abundance and cognitive responses to persuasive communications. J Pers Soc Psychol 1982; 43:623–636
144.
Rausch R: Cognitive strategies in patients with unilateral temporal lobe excisions. Neuropsychologia 1977; 15:385–395
145.
Jones-Gotman M, Milner B: Design fluency: the invention of nonsense drawings after focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia 1977; 15:653–674
146.
Loetscher T, Brugger P: Random number generation in neglect patients reveals enhanced response stereotypy, but no neglect in number space. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47:276–279
147.
Aron AR, Monsell S, Sahakian BJ, et al: A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain 2004; 127:1561–1573
148.
Mentzel H-J, Gaser C, Volz H-P, et al: Cognitive stimulation with the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: functional MR imaging at 1.5 T. Radiology 1998; 207:399–404
149.
Haut MW, Cahill J, Cutlip WD, et al: On the nature of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1996; 65:15–22
150.
Lombardi WJ, Andreason PJ, Sirocco KY, et al: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance following head injury: dorsolateral fronto-striatal circuit activity predicts perseveration. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1999; 21:2–16
151.
Sass L, Byrom G: Phenomenological and neurocognitive perspectives on delusions: a critical overview. World Psychiatry 2015; 14:164–173
152.
Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Thermenos HW, Milanovic S, et al: Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106:1279–1284
153.
Jang JH, Jung WH, Choi J-S, et al: Reduced prefrontal functional connectivity in the default mode network is related to greater psychopathology in subjects with high genetic loading for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2011; 127:58–65
154.
Wotruba D, Michels L, Buechler R, et al: Aberrant coupling within and across the default mode, task-positive, and salience network in subjects at risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2014; 40:1095–1104
155.
Gerrans P: Delusional attitudes and default thinking. Mind Lang 2013; 28:83–102
156.
Bortolotti L: Double bookkeeping in delusions: explaining the gap between saying and doing; in New Waves in Philosophy of Action. Edited by Aguilar JH, Buckareff A, Frankish K. London, United Kingdom, Palgrave MacMillan, pp 237–256, 2011
157.
Victor M, Yakovlev PISS: S.S. Korsakoff’s psychic disorhic disorder in conjunction with peripheral neuritis; a translation of Korsakoff’s original article with comments on the author and his contribution to clinical medicine. Neurology 1955; 5:394–406
158.
Berlyne N: Confabulation. Br J Psychiatry 1972; 120:31–39
159.
Kopelman MD: Two types of confabulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987; 50:1482–1487
160.
Bajo A, Fleminger S, Metcalfe C, et al: Confabulation: what is associated with its rise and fall? A study in brain injury. Cortex 2017; 87:31–43
161.
Schnider A: Spontaneous confabulation, reality monitoring, and the limbic system—a review. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2001; 36:150–160
162.
Schnider A: Spontaneous confabulations, disorientation, and the processing of ‘now’. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38:175–185
163.
Gilboa A, Alain C, Stuss DT, et al: Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: a strategic retrieval account. Brain 2006; 129:1399–1414
164.
Cabeza R, Locantore JK, Anderson ND: Lateralization of prefrontal activity during episodic memory retrieval: evidence for the production-monitoring hypothesis. J Cogn Neurosci 2003; 15:249–259
165.
Coltheart M: Confabulation and conversation. Cortex 2017; 87:62–68
166.
Gopnik A: Explanation as orgasm and the drive for causal knowledge: the function, evolution, and phenomenology of the theory formation system; in Cognition and Explanation. Edited by Keil F, Wilson R. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2000
167.
Gilboa A: Strategic retrieval, confabulations, and delusions: theory and data. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2010; 15:145–180
168.
Damasio AR: The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1996; 351:1413–1420
169.
Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10:295–307.
170.
Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H: Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behav Brain Res 1990; 41:81–94
171.
Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, et al: Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 1997; 275:1293–1295
172.
Jay TM, Witter MP: Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular efferents in the prefrontal cortex of the rat studied by means of anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 1991; 313:574–586
173.
Vinogradova O. Registration of information and the limbic system; in Short-Term Changes in Neural Activity and Behavior. Edited by Hinde RA, Horn G. Cambridge, United Kingdom, University Press, pp 99–140, 1970
174.
Numan R: A prefrontal-hippocampal comparator for goal-directed behavior: the intentional self and episodic memory. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:323
175.
Colombo B, Balzarotti S, Mazzucchelli N: The influence of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on attentional behavior and decision making: A t-DCS study on emotionally vs. functionally designed objects. Brain Cogn 2016; 104:7–14
176.
Papageorgiou CC, Alevizos B, Ventouras E, et al: Psychophysiological correlates of patients with delusional misidentification syndromes and psychotic major depression. J Affect Disord 2004; 81:147–152

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 225 - 235
PubMed: 28347214

History

Received: 14 June 2016
Accepted: 16 January 2017
Published online: 28 March 2017
Published in print: Summer 2017
Revision received: 29 October 2017

Keywords

  1. Psychosis
  2. Hemispheric Asymmetries and Lateralization
  3. Organic Mental Disorders
  4. Stroke and Other Cerebral Vascular Disease (Neuropsychiatric Aspects)
  5. Traumatic Brain Injury

Authors

Details

Lindsey Gurin, M.D. [email protected]
From the Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York (LG); and the Departments of Neurology and Rehabilitation Medicine, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York (SB).
Sonja Blum, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York (LG); and the Departments of Neurology and Rehabilitation Medicine, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York (SB).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Gurin; e-mail: [email protected]

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share