Skip to main content
Full access
Special Articles
Published Online: 4 August 2017

The Anterior Temporal Lobes: New Frontiers Opened to Neuropsychological Research by Changes in Health Care and Disease Epidemiology

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Changes in health care and disease epidemiology have shifted the attention of neuropsychologists and cognitive neuroscientists from vascular lesions to degenerative diseases or other bilateral brain lesions. This displacement of attention from vascular patients to patients with degenerative brain diseases allowed the discovery of hitherto unexplored and unheralded aspects of the neural substrates of human cognition. Three aspects of research on the anterior parts of the temporal lobes (ATLs) are the focus of the present review. The first aspect is category-specific semantic disorders, including current accounts of categorical brain organization, the anatomical substrate of different categories (stressing the role of the ATLs with respect to the biological categories), and the “sources of knowledge” that contribute to construction of those categories. The second aspect is the role of the ATLs in conceptual knowledge, including the “hub-and-spokes” model of semantic representation and semantic control. The third aspect is the role of the right ATL in recognition of familiar people, including the distinction made between associative prosopagnosia and multimodal disorders of person recognition. Consistencies and inconsistencies of results obtained across these different domains are discussed, and the clinical implications of these findings are considered.
For more than a century, classical neuropsychological research, aiming to clarify the neural substrates of the human mind, derived from the study of aphasia and other instrumental cognitive disorders (such as limb apraxia, constructional apraxia, or unilateral spatial neglect) due to unilateral vascular lesions, most of which have a fairly typical brain distribution.1 In recent years, however, changes in health care and disease epidemiology have facilitated a shift away from the study of the neuropsychological consequences of vascular lesions and toward the study of such problems due to degenerative diseases or other bilateral lesions. After the introduction of the “diagnosis-related group” (DRG) system,2 the number of studies dealing with neuroanatomical and pathophysiological aspects of aphasia and other cognitive disorders resulting from vascular lesions dramatically decreased within the acute hospital setting. This reduction was due to the time constraints (e.g.,35) and diminished funding (e.g.,6) related to the introduction of the DRG system. Researchers of the neural substrates of cognitive functions were, therefore, pushed to shift the study of interesting patients from hospitals to rehabilitation facilities.
This displacement had two negative implications for research. First, rehabilitation techniques modify the structure of the observed cognitive defects, confounding their interpretation. Second, rehabilitation structures rarely have easy access to advanced structural and functional neuroimaging technologies, thereby hampering the study of precise anatomo-clinical correlations among persons admitted for neurorehabilitation.
Along with the introduction of the DRG system, population aging in economically developed countries (e.g.,7) also shifted the attention of neuropsychologists and cognitive neuroscientists from vascular lesions to age-related neurodegenerative diseases. This shift of scientific attention from patients with unilateral vascular lesions to patients with degenerative brain diseases or other conditions producing bilateral brain lesions and/or dysfunction facilitated the exploration of the neural substrates of previously unexplored aspects of human cognition and behavior.
Brain-behavior relationships of anterior parts of the temporal lobes therefore are the principal focus of this work. These cortical areas are rarely (and almost never bilaterally) damaged in vascular patients, as they receive a dual blood supply from the anterior temporal branches of the middle cerebral artery and from the anterior temporal branch of the posterior cerebral artery.810 Accordingly, the neurobehavioral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe (ATL) has not been a major focus of research in neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience until the present era. Studies in this area have provided insight into three areas, each of which will be considered in the present review: a) category-specific semantic disorders, b) the role of the ATL in conceptual representations, and c) the role of the right ATL in recognition of familiar people.

Category-Specific Semantic Disorders

Since Lichtheim’s11 classical paper proposing a neuroanatomy of language, it has been generally assumed that conceptual representations (being based on a convergence of different perceptual attributes) should be evenly distributed across the cerebral cortex. Warrington et al.1214 first challenged this assumption, showing that conceptual knowledge (instead of being evenly distributed across the cerebral cortex) is organized by categories and that the selective disruption of these categories is due to the lesion of well-defined brain structures. In particular, Warrington and Shallice11 noticed that in four patients affected by herpes simplex encephalitis (HSE) with lesions involving the ATLs, the semantic impairment preferentially disrupted the biological categories of animals, fruits, and vegetables. On the other hand, Warrington and McCarthy12,13 remarked that in two patients affected by vascular lesions involving the left fronto-temporo-parietal cortices, tools and other artifact categories were slightly but significantly more affected than items belonging to biological categories.

The “Differential Weighting” Account of Category-Specific Semantic Disorders

Warrington et al.1214 claimed that the different neural substrate of biological and artifact categories is due to the “differential weight” that visual and functional/somatosensory attributes have in the construction of these categories of knowledge (the “differential weighting hypothesis”). The distinction between members of biological categories are, indeed, mainly based upon subtle visual-perceptual features, such as the plain, striped, or spotted skin that distinguishes a lion from a tiger or a leopard, whereas tools and other artifacts are distinguished more by the different functions for which they were designed and by the fact that their knowledge is mainly based on actions and somato-sensory information than by their visual features. These pioneering observations were confirmed in the following years by other clinico-pathological reports, attesting the selective disruption of biological categories in patients with HSE lesions encroaching upon the ATL structures (e.g.,1517) and the prevalent impairment of man-made artifacts in patients with vascular lesions involving the left fronto-temporo-parietal areas (e.g.,18,19). Drawing on the “differential weighting hypothesis,” Gainotti20 posited that the brain structures disrupted in patients with a given type of category-specific semantic disorder correspond to the convergence zones21 of sensory-motor information integral to construction of that category.

The Study of the “Sources of Knowledge” for Semantic Categories

The influence of different “sources of knowledge” (i.e., perceptual and motor activities) on mental representation of animate and inanimate biological objects and of artifacts (man-made objects such as tools, clothing, furniture, vehicles) has been studied in healthy subjects by means of feature lists (e.g.,2225) or Likert scales (e.g.,2629) in which subjects are asked to assess the relevance that different sensory modalities have in the representation of various category members. These studies have consistently shown that a) the visual modality is usually considered as the main source of knowledge for all (biological and artifact) categories and b) in biological categories the most important source of knowledge after vision is represented by other perceptual modalities, whereas in artifact categories it is represented by the actions performed with the target artifact. The anterior parts of the temporal lobes should, therefore, play a critical role in the representation of biological categories, because they are placed in the rostral part of the ventral stream of visual processing, where highly processed visual data converge with auditory, olfactory, and gustatory inputs, which are also very important in the representation of biological entities.30 On the other hand, the left fronto-parietal cortices should play a major role in the representation of artifacts because in these areas the dorsal stream of visual processing converges with body-related and action-oriented sources of knowledge.31

The Distinction Within Biological Entities Between Animals and Plant-Life Categories

The simple dichotomy between biological entities and man-made objects cannot account for the brain substrate of different semantic categories, since some differences have been found, within the biological entities, between the neural correlates of defects mainly affecting animals and plant-life categories. Gainotti32,33 and Capitani et al.34 demonstrated neuroanatomical and gender-related differences between patients with temporal lobe lesions showing prevalent impairments for animals versus plant-life categories. A prevalent impairment of the fruits and vegetables category was observed in men with lesions in the left posterior cerebral artery territory encroaching upon the caudal and the infero-mesial aspects of the left temporal lobe,34 whereas a prevalent impairment of the animals category was observed in women showing bilateral lesions of the anterior temporal cortices.33 Different “sources of knowledge,” such as the importance of colors for flowers, fruits, and vegetables,35,36 and of movement and sounds for animal knowledge,29 could explain the abovementioned anatomical differences, whereas social role-related familiarity factors could account for the gender-linked asymmetries.37 Furthermore, the presence of bilateral anterior temporal lesions in patients with category-specific disorders for animals and of left unilateral (parietal and occipital) lesions in patients with defects affecting man-made objects and fruits and vegetables could arise from bilateral representations of the main sources of knowledge about animals (visual, sounds, and other perceptual inputs). By contrast, in right-handed individuals, the motor and somato-sensory functions, which provide an important source of knowledge not only about artifacts but also about fruits and vegetables, are mainly represented in the left hemisphere, which controls the actions performed with the right side of the body.20,33

The Inborn or Experience-Dependent Nature of Categorical Brain Organization

An important theoretical aspect of the categorical brain organization that is still highly controversial concerns its innate or experience-dependent nature.38 In contrast to the “differential weighting hypothesis” offered by Warrington et al.,1214 Caramazza et al.3942 proposed a model (the “domains of knowledge hypothesis”) that assumes the existence of an innate categorical organization of conceptual knowledge. More specifically, the domains of knowledge hypothesis posit a) that category-specific impairments for animals (potential predators), plant life (possible source of food), and artifacts reflect the disruption of innate brain networks, shaped by natural selection to support rapid identification of objects very relevant for survival, and b) that innate connectivity patterns may underlie categorical organization. Strong empirical data supporting the innate nature of these patterns of connectivity come from work indicating that congenitally blind subjects show activation for words (presented in Braille) in the same regions of the ventral stream that are activated by visually presented words in sighted individuals.43 Mahon et al.44 showed that the same medial-to-lateral bias in category preferences for artifacts versus animals, which is present in the ventral surface of the temporo-occipital cortex in sighted individuals,4547 is also present in congenitally blind subjects. On the basis of this observation, Mahon et al.44 suggested that if visual experience is unnecessary for the emergence of category-specificity in the ventral stream, then innate connectivity between regions of the ventral stream and other regions of the brain could drive category-specificity.
On the other hand, several recent investigations (e.g.,4852) have convincingly proved the importance of prior perceptual and motor experience in the cortical representation of previously familiar or unknown objects, whose knowledge had been learned through intensive training. A discussion of this complex problem exceeds the scopes of the present review (for a detailed review, see Gainotti38). For the present review, it suffices to say that, irrespectively of the inborn or experience-dependent nature of the brain categorical organization, the critical role of the ATL in processing animal knowledge has also been acknowledged by authors belonging to Caramazza's group.53

The Role of the Anterior Temporal Lobes in Conceptual Representations

Warrington54 was probably the first author to describe patients with a degenerative brain disease, who presented a selective disruption of semantic knowledge, in a context of intact day-to-day memory, perceptual and spatial abilities, and syntactic and phonological processing. Some years later, Snowden et al.55 and Hodges et al.56 attributed this selective impairment of semantic knowledge to a bilateral atrophy of the ATLs. They proposed the term “semantic dementia” (SD) to denote this new pathological entity, which corresponded to the temporal variant of fronto-temporal dementia (FTD).57 From the neuropsychological point of view, several papers have established that, in the moderate to severe forms of this disease, SD patients show semantic defects for items presented in every verbal and nonverbal modality and that this defect concerns not only the meaning of common words, but also that of the corresponding pictorial images.

The Hub-and-Spoke” Model of Conceptual Representation

Starting from data gathered in SD patients, Rogers et al.58 developed a computational “hub-and-spoke” model, in which modality-specific regions provide the basic sensory, motor, and verbal ingredients (“spokes”) and are networked neuroanatomically to a “hub” that supports additional amodal representations. Some years later, Patterson et al.59 proposed that the neural network for semantic memory requires a single convergence zone (i.e., a “hub”) and that the ATLs bilaterally serve this role by supporting the interactive activation of representations in all modalities and for all semantic categories.
Gainotti60,61 noted, however, that it is only in the moderate to advanced stages of diseases that affect the ATLs bilaterally that semantic impairment is “multimodal.” In the early stages of such diseases, when important asymmetries can be observed at the level of the ATLs, semantic impairments can be modality-specific. In these cases, the impairment mainly involves lexical-semantic knowledge when the left temporal lobe is more affected and pictorial representations when the disease process is predominantly right-sided. These data suggest that the semantic disorder observed in SD is due to the co-occurrence of verbal and nonverbal defects, resulting from left and right ATL atrophy, and that the multimodal semantic impairment observed in advanced stages of SD is due to the combined disruption of pictorial and verbal representations, rather than to the loss of amodal knowledge, bilaterally stored in the ATL.
A similar model has been more recently proposed in studies conducted by Lambon Ralph and colleagues6265 and framed as the “graded hub hypothesis.” This hypothesis posits that multimodal, cross-categorical semantic representations are jointly supported by both left and right ATLs, but maintains that subtle functional gradations may emerge as a consequence of their differential connectivity with primary sensory/motor/limbic regions. In particular, it is suggested that stronger connections with the language areas explain impairments of lexical-semantic knowledge when the left temporal lobe is principally affected. Assuming that the ATL functions are at least in part connectivity-driven, this hypothesis also provides a possible explanation for other graded functional specializations within the ATLs. Thus, the anterior parts of the left superior temporal gyrus could contribute more to abstract words and verbal semantic processing by virtue of their greater connectivity to the language areas,66 whereas the temporal pole could mainly contribute to social concepts by virtue of its connectivity to the amygdala and other structures supporting social cognition and affects.67,68

The Distinction Between Semantic Representation and Semantic Control Disorders

Since nonverbal conceptual disorders (e.g., the inability to recognize meaningful sounds,69 to understand simple symbolic gestures,70 or to draw objects from memory71) have also been reported in individuals with poststroke aphasia, Lambon Ralph and colleagues65,7274 have suggested that the semantic disturbances observed in SD are distinct from those observed in semantic aphasia (SA). Semantic disturbances in SD reflect loss of semantic representations stored in the ATLs, whereas semantic disturbances in SA result from defects in semantic control (i.e., impairment of the executive processes that direct and control semantic activation in a task-appropriate fashion). Several qualitative features (e.g., consistency of performance across tests, sensitivity to the frequency and familiarity of stimuli, effects of cuing and miscuing on task performance, degree of inhibition produced by weak versus strong competitors) distinguish disorders of semantic representation from disorders of semantic control (for a review, see55,75). Furthermore, and at variance with the unitary location of semantic representations within the ATLs, Lambon Ralph and colleagues describe a distributed semantic control network that includes, among other structures, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex,75 the temporo-parietal cortex,76 the dorsal angular gyrus, and the posterior middle temporal gyrus.77 The evidence they provide undermines the thesis that there is a unitary location for semantic representations within the ATLs and further suggests that each component of this distributed semantic control network may have a graded functional specialization.

The Role of the Right ATL in Recognizing Familiar People

Bodamer78 first described a specific form of visual agnosia selectively affecting face recognition, which he labeled “prosopagnosia” (from the Greek: “prosopon”=“face,” “agnosia”=“not knowing”). The study of prosopagnosia has been, for many decades, the almost exclusive context for the study of the neural bases of recognition of familiar people.

The Modality-Specific Forms of Disorders of Recognition of Familiar People

In each social species, identification of individuals belonging to a social group is a fundamental biological function that is accomplished mainly through face recognition in the visual modality and voice recognition in the auditory modality. For some decades, prosopagnosia was the most recognized, and almost uniquely described, disturbance of recognition of familiar faces/people. More than 40 years after Bodamer’s description of prosopagnosia, de Renzi et al.79 proposed a distinction between “apperceptive” and “associative” prosopagnosia. According to this distinction, the apperceptive prosopagnosia is an impairment not only in the recognition of familiar faces but also in the discrimination of unfamiliar faces and in processing non-person-specific information (e.g., age, gender, emotional expression); as such, it is regarded as a higher-level (i.e., cortical) visual disorder. By contrast, associative prosopagnosia is an impairment in the recognition of familiar faces in the absence of problems discriminating them from unfamiliar faces and of other subtle visual disorders; as such, this form of prosopagnosia reflects an associative disturbance or a mnestic disturbance.
From a neuroanatomical perspective, Kanwisher and colleagues’ seminal paper80 explained prosopagnosia in relation to the disturbances in the structure and/or function of the lateral portion of the mid-fusiform gyrus of a processing module specialized for faces, termed the fusiform face area (FFA). More recent studies suggest that prosopagnosia may be due to lesion of a larger network represented in the right hemisphere81,82 that includes, in addition to the FFA, the occipital face area (OFA),83 the ATLs,84 and their interconnections85. It is generally acknowledged that the inferior occipital areas mainly subsume the first stages of face perception, the fusiform face areas shape the holistic face configuration, and the ATLs store individual face templates and/or integrate information concerning the face, voice, and name of a familiar person.
As for voice recognition disorders, Van Lanker and Canter86 first described this problem as “phonagnosia.” Pure phonagnosia has been described in a very small number of patients with bilateral or right temporal lesions, and the methods used in their description limit their interpretation. Hailstone et al.87 offer the only case of “associative phonagnosia” in a patient with behavioral variant of FTD and lesions involving bilaterally the superior temporal gyrus.

The Multimodal Forms of Impaired Recognition of Familiar People

The “dominance” of faces in the study of the process of recognizing familiar people led the neuropsychology community to neglect the description by Ellis et al.88 and Hanley et al.89 of patients with ATL lesions, mainly right-sided, who showed a multimodal defect in the ability to recognize famous people. In these patients, inability to recognize familiar people included recognition by face, voice, and (to a lesser extent) personal name. Even if their recognition disorders were not limited to the visual (face) modality, they were considered as having associative prosopagnosia (e.g.,9092). Several authors9395 have highlighted the fact that individuals with degenerative lesions of the right ATL who are unable to recognize familiar persons from face do not have “associative prosopagnosia” alone because their inability to identify familiar persons extends to recognition of familiar persons by voice and, to a lesser extent, by proper names.
Gainotti96 therefore undertook a review of all reported cases of “associative prosopagnosia” associated with right ATL lesions for the purposes of determining whether their impairments were circumscribed to the visual modality or instead extended to other modalities ordinarily supporting person recognition. This review revealed that most reports limited their study to the visual modality; however, when other modes of people recognition were evaluated, the defect was often multimodal, affecting voice (and, to a lesser extent, name) in addition to face.
The multimodal nature of person recognition disorders shown by patients with right ATL atrophy was recently confirmed by Luzzi et al.97 These authors investigated recognition of famous faces and voices in SD and dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in order to determine whether these conditions differed with respect to the pattern of impairment of famous faces and voices recognition and to test the hypothesis that face and voice recognition disorders prevail in patients with atrophy mainly affecting the RTL. Results showed a differential performance profile in the two diseases: AD patients were significantly impaired in the naming tests but showed preserved recognition, whereas SD patients were profoundly impaired both in naming and in recognition of famous faces and voices. Additionally, among the 12 SD patients in whom positron emission tomography was performed, a strong correlation between FDG uptake and face and voice recognition disorders was found in the right but not in the left ATL.

Controversies About the Nature of Face Recognition Disorders in Patients With Right ATL Lesions

The findings reported in Gainotti’s96 review suggest that describing neuropsychological consequences of structural or functional disturbances of the right ATL requires not only evaluation of famous face recognition but also formal testing of the ability to recognize others by voice. Case reports95,98 have shown that patients with multimodal person recognition disorders are often unaware of their voice recognition disorders. Liu et al.99 studied whether face recognition disorder in ATL must be viewed as an associative variant of prosopagnosia or as part of a multimodal disorder of person recognition by assessing voice perception and short-term recognition of recently heard voices in 10 subjects with impaired face recognition. Deficits indicating a multimodal person recognition disorder were found in two subjects with bilateral ATL lesions, whereas three subjects with right ATL lesions had normal voice perception. They concluded that right ATL lesions can cause a modality-specific form of associative prosopagnosia.
An objection that can be raised to these conclusions stems from the fact that voice recognition disorders have been evaluated in this study with a test of short-term recognition of recently heard voices. However, Gobbini and Haxby,100 as well as Blank et al.,101 have demonstrated that there are two separate networks for recognition of newly learned persons and for famous or personally-familiar persons. A direct comparison of recognition disorders through face and voice of well-known people would have been, therefore, preferable, but a direct comparison between the two modalities is hindered by the fact that voice recognition is more difficult than face recognition102105 and that complex batteries devised to assess people recognition across different modalities are not currently available, because their construction is time consuming and they are culture-specific.
To circumvent these difficulties, Quaranta et al.106 have recently constructed the Famous People Recognition Battery, which tasks subjects with recognizing persons, well-known at the national level, through both their faces and their voices, thereby evaluating familiarity and identification processes. They developed normative data with which to clarify the nature of person recognition disorders observed in patients affected by right ATL lesions, and set the scene for a clearer view of hemispheric asymmetries in familiar people recognition. However, since patients with right ATL atrophy are uncommon, gathering a sufficient sample of such patients is unlikely to be done quickly.

Consistencies and Inconsistencies of Findings from Studies of the Anterior Temporal Lobes

Due to the relative recency of research on the ATL, findings from this literature must be regarded as evolving rather than completed. Indeed, at least one consistency and one inconsistency drawn from the three main areas of research on the ATL may be described. The consistency concerns the fact that both conceptual and familiar people recognition disorders prevail in the verbal modality when the atrophy mainly affects the left ATL and in the pictorial modality when it mostly affects the right ATL. The apparent inconsistency concerns the presence of category-specific semantic disorders in many HSE patients and the presence of a general semantic impairment in SD patients (e.g.,65,107,108), in spite of the fact that both diseases affect the ATLs.

Consistency Between the Format of Conceptual and Familiar People Representations in the Right and Left ATL

Snowden et al.109,110 studied person-specific semantic information obtainable from visual (face) and verbal (name) stimuli in patients with degenerative lesions of the right and left ATL, administering to the same subjects also the picture and the word version of the semantic memory “Pyramids and Palm Trees” (PPT) test.111 They showed that SD patients with predominantly left temporal lobe atrophy identified faces better than names and performed better on the picture than on the word version of the PPT test; by contrast, patients with right temporal lobe atrophy showed the opposite pattern of performance. Similar data were recently reported by Luzzi et al.97
These findings support the thesis that conceptual and personal representations are linked to the verbal modality in the left ATL and to nonverbal modalities in the right ATL.50,51 It remains, however, very difficult to say whether the differences between left and right ATLs are due to the verbal versus nonverbal formats of semantic representations they support60,61 or instead result from the greater connectivity of the left ATL with language areas and of the right ATL with perceptual areas.65

Inconsistency Between HSE and SD Patients With Respect to Category-Specific Semantic Disorders

As for the inconsistency between presence of category-specific semantic disorders in HSE but not in SD patients, Lambon Ralph et al.107 reported a direct comparison of semantic deficits in two groups of patients suffering from HSE and SD. They confirmed that a selective impairment of biological categories is rarely observed in SD, though it is commonly found in HSE patients. On the other hand, Noppeney et al.108 compared the structural damage in four herpes simplex virus encephalitis patients shoving a semantic deficit that particularly affected the biological categories and six SD patients with semantic impairment across all categories tested. According to Noppeney et al.,108 this apparent inconsistency could be due to the fact that in patients with HSE the gray matter loss prevailed in the medial parts of the ATLs, whereas in SD patients the abnormalities extended more laterally and posteriorly in either the left, right, or both temporal lobes. Lambon Ralph et al.65 have recently explained the neuroanatomical correlates of category-specific semantic disorders in terms of their connectivity-constrained hub-and-spokes model. For example, the fronto-parietal areas could store the representations of man-made objects because they are directly connected to the medial ventral occipito-temporal regions that according to some authors (e.g.,4547) exhibit greater activation for artifacts.

Conclusions and Clinical Implications

Prior to the discovery of the category-specific semantic disorders for biological entities due to HSE-related ATL lesions and the semantic disorders associated with bilateral ATL atrophy in SD, the functions of the ATLs were completely unknown, and knowledge of the clinical disorders resulting from ATL damage was very limited. The only source of information about the functions of the ATLs and the symptomatology resulting from their damage was the Klüver-Bucy syndrome in monkeys, which consisted of tameness and diminished fear, hyperorality, hypersexuality, blunted affect, visual agnosia, and social withdrawal112,113 and which was reproduced in man by bilateral removal of the temporal lobes.114
Data reported in the present survey demonstrate that the ATLs critically contribute to some of the most important human cognitive functions, including representation of general conceptual knowledge or of specific biological categories and the recognition of familiar people, and tend to restrict to the right ATL the emotional and social functions stressed by the Klüver-Bucy syndrome. Among patients with right and left ATL atrophy, the Klüver-Bucy-like symptoms differ: Those with right ATL atrophy often present some of the striking social and emotional deficits of the Klüver-Bucy syndrome, whereas these symptoms are typically absent in patients with left ATL atrophy. Several groups57,115.116 describe semantic disorders with relatively preserved socio-emotional function in association with left ATL involvement and severe behavioral disturbances with relatively preserved semantic skills in association with right ATL involvement. A recent study117 also differentiated right-lateralized SD and behavioral-variant FTD (bvFTD) with respect to semantic and behavioral profiles: Patients with right ATL atrophy demonstrated face recognition disorders and an increased rigidity with obsessive personality/behavioral changes, whereas patients with bvFTD demonstrated pronounced deficits in attention/working memory, increased apathy, and greater executive dysfunction.
The coexistence of social and emotional defects with familiar people recognition disorders in patients with right ATL atrophy and the presence within the ATLs of a general system of semantic representations has suggested to some authors118120 that part of the ATLs may support a special form of knowledge (i.e., social cognition) and that the corresponding structures may be engaged when tasks demand access to social conceptual knowledge. This model has been critically reviewed by Gainotti,61 who demonstrated that behavioral data gathered in patients with right and left ATL lesions and results of neuroimaging investigations using tasks drawn from the “Theory of Mind” or involving moral judgments and emotional or social tasks do not support the social cognition hypothesis, but instead simply suggest a superiority of the right ATL for emotional and social functions.
Clinically, identifying patients with functionally-relevant right ATL atrophy is difficult, and the social and behavioral disorders of patients with right ATL involvement often overshadow semantic disturbances. Administering a “famous faces” test and/or semantic category tests to such patients may be very useful: A finding of deficits on these or similar tests may suggest a neurodegenerative disease affecting the ATLs rather than the frontal lobes117 and clarify involvement of the right versus left ATL.121 Unfortunately, simple and reliable international tasks of this kind are not easily available, for the reason that their construction is time consuming and that faces (and, even more, voices) of famous people are recognized only in a well-defined linguistic and cultural context. The recent construction and standardization in Italy of a “Famous People Recognition Battery,”106 in which subjects are requested to recognize through their faces and voices the same persons (well-known at the national level), could be a first step in this direction. Following on the development of such tests and elucidation of the underlying neurobiology of ATL-related neuropsychiatric disturbances may improve diagnosis and facilitate advances in the care and lives of persons with these conditions.

References

1.
Cantu RC, Ames A III: Distribution of vascular lesions caused by cerebral ischemia: relation to survival. Neurology 1969; 19:128–132
2.
Fetter RB, Shin Y, Freeman JL, et al: Case mix definition by diagnosis-related groups. Med Care 1980; 18(Suppl):1–53
3.
Armstrong E: Communication culture in acute speech pathology settings: currents issues. Adv Speech Lang Pathol 2003; 5:137–143
4.
Enderby P, Petheram B: Has aphasia therapy been swallowed up? Clin Rehabil 2002; 16:604–608
5.
Lalor E, Cranfield E: Aphasia: a description of the incidence and management in the acute hospital setting. Asia Pac J Speech Lang Hear 2004; 9:129–136
6.
National Stroke Foundation: Clinical Guidelines for Stroke Management. Melbourne, Australia, 2010
7.
Harper S: Ageing Societies: Myths, Challenges and Opportunities. Hoboken, NJ, Wiley, 2006
8.
Kreisler A, Godefroy O, Delmaire C, et al: The anatomy of aphasia revisited. Neurology 2000; 54:1117–1123
9.
Borden NM: 3D Angiographic Atlas of Neurovascular Anatomy and Pathology. Cambridge, Mass, Cambridge University Press, 2006
10.
Kiernan JA: Anatomy of the temporal lobe. Epilepsy Res Treat 2012; 2012:176157.
11.
Lichtheim L: On aphasia. Brain 1885; 7:433–484
12.
Warrington EK, McCarthy R: Category specific access dysphasia. Brain 1983; 106:859–878
13.
Warrington EK, McCarthy RA: Categories of knowledge: Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain 1987; 110:1273–1296
14.
Warrington EK, Shallice T: Semantic access dyslexia. Brain 1979; 102:43–63
15.
Pietrini V, Nertempi P, Vaglia A, et al: Recovery from herpes simplex encephalitis: selective impairment of specific semantic categories with neuroradiological correlation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988; 51:1284–1293
16.
Sartori G, Job R: The oyster with four legs: a neuropsychological study on the interaction of visual and semantic information. Cogn Neuropsychol 1988; 5:105–132
17.
Silveri MC, Gainotti G: Interaction between vision and language in category-specific semantic impairment. Cogn Neuropsychol 1988; 5:677–709
18.
Hillis AE, Rapp B, Romani C, et al: Selective impairment of semantics in lexical processing. Cogn Neuropsychol 1990; 7:191–243
19.
Sacchett C, Humphreys GW: Calling a squirrel a squirrel but a canoe a wigwam: a category-specific deficit for artefactual objects and body parts. Cogn Neuropsychol 1992; 9:73–86
20.
Gainotti G: What the locus of brain lesion tells us about the nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders: a review. Cortex 2000; 36:539–559
21.
Damasio AR: Category-related recognition defects as a clue to the neural substrates of knowledge. Trends Neurosci 1990; 13:95–98
22.
Cree GS, McRae K: Analyzing the factors underlying the structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunk, cherry, chisel, cheese, and cello (and many other such concrete nouns). J Exp Psychol Gen 2003; 132:163–201
23.
Vinson DP, Vigliocco G, Cappa S, et al: The breakdown of semantic knowledge: insights from a statistical model of meaning representation. Brain Lang 2003; 86:347–365
24.
Vigliocco G, Vinson DP, Lewis W, et al: Representing the meanings of object and action words: the featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cognit Psychol 2004; 48:422–488
25.
Ventura P, Morais J, Brito-Mendes C, et al: The mental representation of living and nonliving things: differential weighting and interactivity of sensorial and non-sensorial features. Memory 2005; 13:124–147
26.
Tranel D, Logan CG, Frank RJ, et al: Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval of conceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entities: operationalization and analysis of factors. Neuropsychologia 1997; 35:1329–1339
27.
Gainotti G, Ciaraffa F, Silveri MC, et al: Mental representation of normal subjects about the sources of knowledge in different semantic categories and unique entities. Neuropsychology 2009; 23:803–812
28.
Gainotti G, Spinelli P, Scaricamazza E, et al: The evaluation of sources of knowledge underlying different semantic categories. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:40–
29.
Hoffman P, Lambon Ralph MA: Shapes, scents and sounds: quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51:14–25
30.
Binder JR, Conant LL, Humphries CJ, et al: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation. Cogn Neuropsychol 2016; 33:130–174
31.
Boronat CB, Buxbaum LJ, Coslett HB, et al: Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2005; 23:361–373
32.
Gainotti G: The influence of gender and lesion location on naming disorders for animals, plants and artefacts. Neuropsychologia 2005; 43:1633–1644
33.
Gainotti G: The influence of anatomical locus of lesion and of gender-related familiarity factors in category-specific semantic disorders for animals, fruits and vegetables: A review of single-case studies. Cortex 2010; 46:1072–1087
34.
Capitani E, Laiacona M, Pagani R, et al: Posterior cerebral artery infarcts and semantic category dissociations: A study of 28 patients. Brain 2009; 132:965–981
35.
Merck C, Jonin PY, Vichard H, et al: Relative category-specific preservation in semantic dementia? Evidence from 35 cases. Brain Lang 2013; 124:257–267
36.
Lewis DE, Pearson J, Khuu SK: The color “fruit”: object memories defined by color. PLoS One 2013; 8:e64960
37.
Gerlach C, Gainotti G: Gender differences in category-specificity do not reflect innate dispositions. Cortex 2016; 85:46–53
38.
Gainotti G: Inborn and experience-dependent models of categorical brain organization: A position paper. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:2
39.
Caramazza A: The interpretation of semantic category-specific deficits: what do they reveal about the organization of conceptual knowledge in the brain? Neurocase 1998; 4:265–272
40.
Caramazza A, Shelton JR: Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: the animate-inanimate distinction. J Cogn Neurosci 1998; 10:1–34
41.
Caramazza A, Mahon BZ: The organisation of conceptual knowledge in the brain: the future’s past and some future directions. Cogn Neuropsychol 2006; 23:13–38
42.
Mahon BZ, Caramazza A: Concepts and categories: a cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annu Rev Psychol 2009; 60:27–51
43.
Büchel C, Price C, Frackowiak RS, et al: Different activation patterns in the visual cortex of late and congenitally blind subjects. Brain 1998; 121:409–419
44.
Mahon BZ, Anzellotti S, Schwarzbach J, et al: Category-specific organization in the human brain does not require visual experience. Neuron 2009; 63:397–405
45.
Chao LL, Haxby JV, Martin A: Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:913–919
46.
Noppeney U, Price CJ, Penny WD, et al: Two distinct neural mechanisms for category-selective responses. Cereb Cortex 2006; 16:437–445
47.
Mahon BZ, Milleville SC, Negri GA, et al: Action-related properties shape object representations in the ventral stream. Neuron 2007; 55:507–520
48.
James TW, Gauthier I: Auditory and action semantic features activate sensory-specific perceptual brain regions. Curr Biol 2003; 13:1792–1796
49.
Kiefer M, Sim EJ, Liebich S, et al: Experience-dependent plasticity of conceptual representations in human sensory-motor areas. J Cogn Neurosci 2007; 19:525–542
50.
Hoenig K, Müller C, Herrnberger B, et al: Neuroplasticity of semantic representations for musical instruments in professional musicians. Neuroimage 2011; 56:1714–1725
51.
Bellebaum C, Tettamanti M, Marchetta E, et al: Neural representations of unfamiliar objects are modulated by sensorimotor experience. Cortex 2013; 49:1110–1125
52.
Yee E, Chrysikou EG, Hoffman E, et al: Manual experience shapes object representations. Psychol Sci 2013; 24:909–919
53.
Anzellotti S, Mahon BZ, Schwarzbach J, et al: Differential activity for animals and manipulable objects in the anterior temporal lobes. J Cogn Neurosci 2011; 23:2059–2067
54.
Warrington EK: The selective impairment of semantic memory. Q J Exp Psychol 1975; 27:635–657
55.
Snowden J, Goulding PJ, Neary D: Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behav Neurol 1989; 2:167–182
56.
Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, et al: Semantic dementia: progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 1992; 115:1783–1806
57.
Edwards-Lee T, Miller BL, Benson DF, et al: The temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 1997; 120:1027–1040
58.
Rogers TT, Lambon Ralph MA, Garrard P, et al: Structure and deterioration of semantic memory: a neuropsychological and computational investigation. Psychol Rev 2004; 111:205–235
59.
Patterson K, Nestor PJ, Rogers TT: Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007; 8:976–987
60.
Gainotti G: The format of conceptual representations disrupted in semantic dementia: a position paper. Cortex 2012; 48:521–529
61.
Gainotti G: Is the difference between right and left ATLs due to the distinction between general and social cognition or between verbal and non-verbal representations? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 51:296–312
62.
Binney RJ, Parker GJ, Lambon Ralph MA: Convergent connectivity and graded specialization in the rostral human temporal lobe as revealed by diffusion-weighted imaging probabilistic tractography. J Cogn Neurosci 2012; 24:1998–2014
63.
Schapiro AC, McClelland JL, Welbourne SR, et al: Why bilateral damage is worse than unilateral damage to the brain. J Cogn Neurosci 2013; 25:2107–2123
64.
Rice GE, Lambon Ralph MA, Hoffman P: The roles of left versus right anterior temporal lobes in conceptual knowledge: an ALE meta-analysis of 97 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25:4374–4391
65.
Lambon Ralph MA, Jefferies E, Patterson K, et al: The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 2017; 18:42–55
66.
Hoffman P, Binney RJ, Lambon Ralph MA: Differing contributions of inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex to concrete and abstract conceptual knowledge. Cortex 2015; 63:250–266
67.
Binney RJ, Hoffman P, Lambon Ralph MA: Mapping the multiple graded contributions of the anterior temporal lobe representational hub to abstract and social concepts: evidence from distortion-corrected fMRI. Cereb Cortex 2016; 26:4227–4241
68.
Pobric G, Lambon Ralph MA, Zahn R: Hemispheric specialization within the superior anterior temporal cortex for social and nonsocial concepts. J Cogn Neurosci 2016; 28:351–360
69.
Spinnler H, Vignolo LA: Impaired recognition of meaningful sounds. Cortex 1966; 2:337–348
70.
Gainotti G, Lemmo MS: Comprehension of symbolic gestures in aphasia. Brain Lang 1976; 3:451–460
71.
Gainotti G, Silveri MC, Villa G, et al: Drawing objects from memory in aphasia. Brain 1983; 106:613–622
72.
Jefferies E, Lambon Ralph MA: Semantic impairment in stroke aphasia versus semantic dementia: a case-series comparison. Brain 2006; 129:2132–2147
73.
Jefferies E, Baker SS, Doran M, et al: Refractory effects in stroke aphasia: a consequence of poor semantic control. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:1065–1079
74.
Corbett F, Jefferies E, Ehsan S, et al: Different impairments of semantic cognition in semantic dementia and semantic aphasia: evidence from the non-verbal domain. Brain 2009; 132:2593–2608
75.
Hoffman P, Jefferies E, Lambon Ralph MA: Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex plays an executive regulation role in comprehension of abstract words: convergent neuropsychological and repetitive TMS evidence. J Neurosci 2010; 30:15450–15456
76.
Whitney C, Kirk M, O’Sullivan J, et al: Executive semantic processing is underpinned by a large-scale neural network: revealing the contribution of left prefrontal, posterior temporal, and parietal cortex to controlled retrieval and selection using TMS. J Cogn Neurosci 2012; 24:133–147
77.
Noonan KA, Jefferies E, Visser M, et al: Going beyond inferior prefrontal involvement in semantic control: evidence for the additional contribution of dorsal angular gyrus and posterior middle temporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 2013; 25:1824–1850
78.
Bodamer J: Die prosopoagnosie. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 1947; 179:6–53
79.
De Renzi E, Faglioni P, Grossi D, et al: Apperceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia. Cortex 1991; 27:213–221
80.
Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM: The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 1997; 17:4302–4311
81.
Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI: The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn Sci 2000; 4:223–233
82.
Rossion B, Hanseeuw B, Dricot L: Defining face perception areas in the human brain: a large-scale factorial fMRI face localizer analysis. Brain Cogn 2012; 79:138–157
83.
Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Moylan J, et al: The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J Cogn Neurosci 2000; 12:495–504
84.
Barton JJ: Structure and function in acquired prosopagnosia: lessons from a series of 10 patients with brain damage. J Neuropsychol 2008; 2:197–225
85.
Fox CJ, Iaria G, Barton JJ: Disconnection in prosopagnosia and face processing. Cortex 2008; 44:996–1009
86.
Van Lancker DR, Canter GJ: Impairment of voice and face recognition in patients with hemispheric damage. Brain Cogn 1982; 1:185–195
87.
Hailstone JC, Crutch SJ, Vestergaard MD, et al: Progressive associative phonagnosia: a neuropsychological analysis. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:1104–1114
88.
Ellis AW, Young AW, Critchley EMR: Loss of memory for people following temporal lobe damage. Brain 1989; 112:1469–1483
89.
Hanley JR, Young AW, Pearson NA: Defective recognition of familiar people. Cogn Neuropsychol 1989; 6:179–210
90.
Nakachi R, Muramatsu T, Kato M, et al: Progressive prosopagnosia at the very early stage of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Psychogeriatrics 2007; 7:155–162
91.
Pancaroglu R, Busigny T, Johnston S, et al: The right anterior temporal lobe variant of prosopagnosia. J Vis 2011; 1:573–573
92.
Davies-Thompson J, Pancaroglu R, Barton J: Acquired prosopagnosia: Structural basis and processing impairments. Front Biosci (Elite Ed) 2014; 6:159–174
93.
Evans JJ, Heggs AJ, Antoun N, et al: Progressive prosopagnosia associated with selective right temporal lobe atrophy. A new syndrome? Brain 1995; 118:1–13
94.
Gentileschi V, Sperber S, Spinnler H: Crossmodal agnosia for familiar people as a consequence of right infero polar temporal atrophy. Cogn Neuropsychol 2001; 18:439–463
95.
Gainotti G, Barbier A, Marra C: Slowly progressive defect in recognition of familiar people in a patient with right anterior temporal atrophy. Brain 2003; 126:792–803
96.
Gainotti G: Is the right anterior temporal variant of prosopagnosia a form of “associative prosopagnosia” or a form of “multimodal person recognition disorder”? Neuropsychol Rev 2013; 23:99–110
97.
Luzzi S, Baldinelli S, Ranaldi V, et al: Famous faces and voices: differential profiles in early right and left semantic dementia and in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2017; 94:118–128
98.
Boudouresques J, Poncet M, Cherif AA et al: Agnosia for faces: evidence of functional disorganization of a certain type of recognition of objects in the physical world. B Acad Nat Med Paris 1979; 163:695–702
99.
Liu RR, Pancaroglu R, Hills CS, et al: Voice recognition in face-blind patients. Cereb Cortex 2016; 26:1473–1487
100.
Gobbini MI, Haxby JV: Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:32–41
101.
Blank H, Wieland N, von Kriegstein K: Person recognition and the brain: merging evidence from patients and healthy individuals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 47:717–734
102.
Hanley JR, Smith T, Hadfield J: I recognise you but I can’t place you: An investigation of familiar-only experiences during tests of voice and face recognition. Q J Exp Psychol 1998; 51A:179–195
103.
Damjanovic L, Hanley JR: Recalling episodic and semantic information about famous faces and voices. Mem Cognit 2007; 35:1205–1210
104.
Brédart S, Barsics C, Hanley JR: Recalling semantic information about personally known faces and voices. Eur J Cogn Psychol 2009; 7:1013–1021
105.
Hanley JR, Damjanovic L: It is more difficult to retrieve a familiar person’s name and occupation from their voice than from their blurred face. Memory 2009; 17:830–839
106.
Quaranta D, Piccininni C, Carlesimo GA, et al: Recognition disorders for famous faces and voices: a review of the literature and normative data of a new test battery. Neurol Sci 2016; 37:345–352
107.
Lambon Ralph MA, Lowe C, Rogers TT: Neural basis of category-specific semantic deficits for living things: evidence from semantic dementia, HSVE and a neural network model. Brain 2007; 130:1127–1137
108.
Noppeney U, Patterson K, Tyler LK, et al: Temporal lobe lesions and semantic impairment: a comparison of herpes simplex virus encephalitis and semantic dementia. Brain 2007; 130:1138–1147
109.
Snowden JS, Thompson JC, Neary D: Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia. Brain 2004; 127:860–872
110.
Snowden JS, Thompson JC, Neary D: Famous people knowledge and the right and left temporal lobes. Behav Neurol 2012; 25:35–44
111.
Howard D, Patterson K: Pyramids and Palm Trees: Access From Pictures and Words. Bury St Edmunds, United Kingdom, Thames Valley Test Company, 1992
112.
Kluver H, Bucy P: Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1939; 41:979–1000
113.
Kling AS, Tachiki K, Lloyd R: Neurochemical correlates of the Klüver-Bucy syndrome by in vivo microdialysis in monkey. Behav Brain Res 1993; 56:161–170
114.
Terzian H, Ore GD: Syndrome of Klüver and Bucy: Reproduced in man by bilateral removal of the temporal lobes. Neurology 1955; 5:373–380
115.
Thompson SA, Patterson K, Hodges JR: Left/right asymmetry of atrophy in semantic dementia: behavioral-cognitive implications. Neurology 2003; 61:1196–1203
116.
Chan D, Anderson V, Pijnenburg Y, et al: The clinical profile of right temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 2009; 132:1287–1298
117.
Kamminga J, Kumfor F, Burrell JR, et al: Differentiating between right-lateralised semantic dementia and behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: an examination of clinical characteristics and emotion processing. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2015; 86:1082–1088
118.
Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y: The enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 2007; 130:1718–1731
119.
Ross LA, Olson IR: Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes. Neuroimage 2010; 49:3452–3462
120.
Olson IR, McCoy D, Klobusicky E, et al: Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes: A review and theoretical framework. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 8:123–133
121.
Josephs KA, Whitwell JL, Vemuri P, et al: The anatomic correlate of prosopagnosia in semantic dementia. Neurology 2008; 71:1628–1633

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 22 - 30
PubMed: 28774225

History

Received: 2 January 2017
Revision received: 25 January 2017
Revision received: 20 April 2017
Accepted: 21 April 2017
Published online: 4 August 2017
Published in print: Winter 2018

Keywords

  1. Cognitive Disorders
  2. Hemispheric Asymmetries and Lateralization
  3. Memory
  4. Neuroanatomy
  5. Neuropsychology

Authors

Details

Guido Gainotti, M.D. gainotti@ rm.unicatt.it
From the Institute of Neurology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Rome, Italy; and the IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Department of Clinical and Behavioral Neurology, Rome, Italy.

Notes

Send correspondence to Prof. Gainotti; e-mail: [email protected]

Funding Information

Prof. Gainotti reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share