Skip to main content
Full access
Articles
Published Online: 15 March 2021

Sleepy Psychotherapists: How Clinicians’ Biological Factors May Affect the Conduct of Psychotherapy

Abstract

Objective:

Numerous therapist variables and cognitive biases can affect the quality of the therapeutic alliance and the conduct and outcomes of psychotherapy. This article aims to examine factors that potentially affect clinician performance, including chronobiological variables of clinicians and patients.

Methods:

The author reviewed literature pertaining to biological influences on human cognitive performance and considered how these factors may apply to the practice of psychotherapy.

Results:

Biological factors potentially affecting the conduct and quality of psychotherapy were identified. These factors include decision fatigue, hunger, sleep deficit, shift work, and several chronobiological issues related to circadian rhythms and episodic ultradian rhythms. In addition, misaligned scheduling of psychotherapy sessions in relation to therapist and patient evening-morning chronotypes may impede the effectiveness of psychotherapy.

Conclusions:

The practice of psychotherapy is cognitively demanding, requiring that clinicians remain constantly alert and in command of their executive functions. Decreases in clinician alertness resulting from homeostatic depletion, chronobiologically misaligned schedules, and illness-associated factors may reduce the quality and benefit of psychotherapy sessions. Mitigation strategies are available. Investigations of these factors are needed.

Highlights

Biological factors, including physiological homeostatic, chronobiological, and illness-related factors, can affect psychotherapists’ cognitive functioning and thus their conduct of psychotherapy.
Scheduling misalignments related to psychotherapist and patient morning-evening chronotype characteristics may affect the quality of psychotherapy sessions.
Mitigation of potentially modifiable factors requires clinician self-appraisal and self-care and institutional attention to optimize clinician alertness and diminish cognitive sluggishness at times of direct patient care.
The practice of psychotherapy demands top cognitive performance by clinicians. Psychotherapists are expected to maintain high degrees of alertness and focused attention, to listen carefully, and to use everything they know about their patients to rapidly reflect on and respond to patients’ verbal and nonverbal communications. Nevertheless, anecdotes abound concerning clinicians who doze off, pinch their legs, or bite the inside of their cheeks to stay awake during some psychotherapy sessions.
As a central tenet of psychodynamic psychotherapy, countertransference results from a mixture of psychological and social influences. Biological factors may play a role as well. Psychotherapists practicing psychodynamic therapy sometimes ascribe clinician sleepiness during psychotherapy to countertransference reactions, specifically clinicians’ responses to patients who use obsessional defenses and who are boring. However, many other factors contributing to drowsiness and cognitive sluggishness during psychotherapy sessions may reside in clinicians themselves. The aim of this article is to call attention to the numerous biological factors that can adversely affect clinicians’ cognitive processes and their conduct of psychotherapy. These factors include depletion of physiologic processes necessary for maintaining homeostasis, chronobiological misalignments, and additional aggravating processes related to illness among clinicians. Interventions that may mitigate these adverse effects and areas for further study are also discussed.
These inquiries were motivated by the following intriguing findings. In a classic study (1) of 1,125 judicial rulings on prisoner parole requests rendered by 65 Israeli judges over a 50-day period, prisoners had a better chance of favorable rulings if they were seen at the beginning of the day rather than later in the day. With the day divided into three sessions (early morning, after a midmorning snack, and after lunch), rulings were more favorable at the beginning than toward the end of each session and were more favorable earlier in the day than later in the day, especially after lunch. The authors postulated that fatigue and hunger affected the judges’ decision making. In the field of general medicine, a study of 33 primary care practices involving nearly 20,000 patients (2) found that patients with appointments later in the day were less likely to receive orders for guideline-recommended cancer screenings. In another study of 1,220 patients matched for numerous clinical variables and types of surgery (3), cardiothoracic surgeons were found to order more blood transfusions during surgeries that were conducted later in the day. To explain these phenomena, the authors (4) invoked mental fatigue, hunger, and decision fatigue as potential contributors.
In addition, a substantial literature (5) addresses the deleterious effects of chronic sleep deprivation and fatigue on clinical performance, patient safety, and clinical outcomes. Increasing attention has also been directed to burnout and clinician sleep deprivation and fatigue in medical settings (6). Concerns about the effects of sleep deprivation on performance spurred the Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education (7) to develop work hour standards for house officers.
On the basis of these leads, literature was reviewed on biological variables related to cognitive performance, including the studies mentioned above as well as several associated with sleep and wakefulness issues, such as circadian and episodic ultradian cycles. The review focused on how these factors might alter therapist performance related to cognition and the conduct and outcomes of psychotherapy.

Methods

A PubMed literature search was conducted for studies that addressed biological factors having an impact on the cognitive functioning of psychotherapists and the conduct of psychotherapy, specifically those addressing homeostatic, chronobiological, and illness-related factors. Literature more broadly concerning the impact of these factors on human cognitive performance was also selectively examined.

Results

No studies specifically related to the effects of biological factors on the performance of psychotherapy were found in PubMed. However, several articles indicated that various biological factors, including homeostatic depletion, chronobiological influences, and illness-related issues and their treatment can influence the cognitive performance of various populations. Consideration was given to how these factors might also have an impact on clinicians’ performance in the conduct of psychotherapy.

Cognitive Functions

Research in the area of cognitive functioning has considered differing aspects of cognitive performance, which in one scholarly review (8), were separated into attention, memory, and executive functioning. These categories, in turn, can be subdivided, both conceptually and on the basis of experimental methods and scales designed to measure each component separately. Accordingly, studies of attention have addressed alertness and arousal, selective attention flexibility, divided attention, sustained attention or vigilance, and sleepiness and fatigue. Studies of memory have addressed working memory, episodic memory, semantic memory, procedural memory, and the perceptual representation system. Studies of executive functioning (8) have addressed inhibition, set shifting, rule deduction and categorization, and planning. Although laboratory studies (9) have operationally defined, measured, and attempted to unpack the influence of each of these variables, in our daily experience the variables closely intertwine with one another to produce varying levels of competent or sluggish cognitive functioning.

Biological Factors

A range of biological factors work in concert to augment or diminish the quality of clinicians’ cognitive performance during psychotherapy. These include factors that can be grouped as homeostatic (depletion models), chronobiological (circadian and episodic ultradian models), and illnesses and their treatments.

Homeostatic factors.

Homeostatic factors are best represented by sleep deprivation and hunger. First, chronic sleep deprivation caused by personal habits of poor sleep hygiene and undersleeping, disturbed sleep (e.g., insomnias, sleep interruptions because of exogenous factors ranging from baby care to urinary frequency), shift work (e.g., nighttime calls interrupting sleep), or other causes of insomnia can all contribute to progressive sleep deficits. These deficits increase the need for sleep, which is more likely to occur in settings of low sensory stimulation, such as dimly lit offices and during soft-toned conversations. Discrepancies between individual preferences for sleep-wake periods and work demands have been called “social jetlag” (10), as occurs in individuals who chronically go to sleep late during the work week and attempt to compensate for their sleep deficits on the weekends. Notably, in a study of health care workers performing shift work (11), those who recognized they were fatigued and whose cognitive testing showed objective decrements did not view their performance as any worse than before. For caffeine-dependent individuals, caffeine deficiency may contribute to daytime sluggishness (12). Furthermore, hunger, especially glucose deprivation, may contribute to diminished cognitive performance. Intense cognitive work is glucose dependent, and several studies (13, 14) have suggested that cognitive sharpness is likely to fall in direct relation to cognitive effort expended and time since the last meal or snack. At the same time, daytime sleepiness, especially in the early afternoon, can follow large meals (15). Suggested mechanisms of this effect, which remain speculative, are based on observations that high fat intake elevates blood cholecystokinin levels, which increase lassitude, and that foods and drinks with high glycemic indices also promote sleepiness (15).

Chronobiological factors.

Chronobiological factors include circadian and episodic ultradian rhythms. Circadian (approximately 24-hour rhythms) occur in numerous physiological functions, including a range of hormonal and cellular rhythms, such as body temperature and alertness. Notably, better and worse cognitive performance has been shown to generally correspond to higher and lower core body temperatures, respectively, although the relationship is complex (16, 17).
Episodic ultradian rhythms, which ordinarily last 90–120 minutes but can take place over up to 6 hours, are readily observed on electroencephalographic measures (18). These include the classic 90- to 120-minute basic rest-activity cycles (BRACs) initially described decades ago by Kleitman (19). Daytime BRACs appear to be continuations of the rapid eye movement cycle periodicity during sleep, occurring at roughly 90-minute intervals. Daytime ultradian cycles occurring roughly every 90–120 minutes have been described as influencing daytime vigilance (20) and waking fantasies (21). Dopaminergic networks are thought to mediate ultradian oscillators, and it has been suggested that these rhythms play a role in optimizing and restoring energy throughout the day (22). Several ultradian oscillators may operate concomitantly (23). For example, evidence shows that verbal and spatial performance are affected by 90-minute daytime cycles that are approximately 180 out of phase with one another, suggesting that ultradian rhythms can affect cognitive style and that separate ultradian oscillators may be operating in each cerebral hemisphere (24).
Fluctuations in performance of two or more individuals working together, as in psychotherapy, are also influenced by interindividual differences in chronotype, reflecting circadian preference, and by the synchronicity between individuals’ peak periods of arousal and the time of the day at which optimum performance is required. Accordingly, some individuals are at their peak during early mornings (“larks”) and prefer early bedtimes, others are sluggish in the morning but wake up during evening hours and prefer late bedtimes (“owls”), and many other individuals are somewhere in the middle (25, 26).
Whereas roughly 50% of the variance in chronotype preference appears to be genetic (27), these patterns are also influenced by environmental forces (e.g., work and school schedules) and may shift with age. Early morning preference occurs more commonly among elderly populations (up to 75%), whereas early morning preference occurs among only 7% of younger individuals (28). Furthermore, over the course of the day, cognitive performance deteriorates among elderly persons but often improves among young adults (29).
Overall, these time-of-day modulations affect performance on a wide range of cognitive tasks measuring attentional capacities, executive functioning, and memory (8, 9). These performance fluctuations are also contingent on the chronotype, which reflects interindividual differences in circadian preference, and particularly depend on the synchronicity between individuals’ peak periods of circadian arousal and the time of the day at which testing occurs. When juxtaposed, the effects of homeostatic and chronobiological factors are sometimes counterbalanced but at times their effects may be superimposed. For example, a confluence of circadian and episodic ultradian troughs in conjunction with homeostatic depletion can assure periods of low mental alertness and cognitive sluggishness.

Illnesses and their Treatments.

Illnesses and their treatments that affect clinicians and potentially impair their cognitive sharpness are plentiful. These include numerous chronic medical illnesses contributing to fatigue (30) or pain (31); depression; early neurocognitive decline; excessive alcohol or substance use; and adverse cognitive effects of medications, such as analgesics, muscle relaxants, psychotropics, anticholinergics, steroids and other hormones, and immune suppressors. Decreased visual and auditory acuity can also contribute to problems in attending and ascertaining shifts during psychotherapy sessions.

Discussion and Conclusions

Implications for the Conduct of Psychotherapy

Although biological factors influencing clinician performance, including the homeostatic, chronobiological, and illness-related factors discussed above, are of great concern to health care clinician performance in general, they have not previously been considered specifically with respect to the performance of psychotherapy. Are sleepy psychotherapists sloppy psychotherapists? Do patients get their psychotherapists’ best efforts? Are all a clinician’s psychotherapy hours worth the same amount? Should clinicians attempt to conduct psychotherapy after a sleepless night of being on call? Although no specific studies address these questions, the types of cognitive functions enumerated earlier in this article offer reasonable entry points to consider how diminished cognitive functioning may adversely affect psychotherapy.
Impairments affecting attention (e.g., alertness, selective attention flexibility, and divided attention) may decrease the chances that psychotherapists hear or see something important being communicated (or omitted) verbally or nonverbally. Inattentive psychotherapists may miss important but subtle shifts in topic or emphasis and are less likely to be attuned to patients.
Impairments affecting memory, including working memory, episodic memory, semantic memory, procedural memory, and the perceptual representation system, may decrease psychotherapists’ recall of important aspects of patients’ histories that are necessary to fully understand patients’ current issues. Important connections might more easily slip their minds.
Impairments in executive functioning affecting inhibition, set shifting, rule deduction and categorization, and planning may decrease psychotherapists’ fidelity to treatment models. Psychotherapists with subtly diminished executive functioning may show increased impulsivity in thought, disinhibition in speech, and greater reliance on fast-thinking heuristic shortcuts, including cognitive biases, rather than on deliberate, slow thoughtfulness. This view is consistent with data suggesting that prefrontal cortical executive functions inhibit and evaluate incorrect hunches and intuitions, all of which commonly occur among clinicians and may adversely affect psychotherapy.
Although a full discussion is beyond the scope of this article, psychotherapists’ mood states, which are likely to have an impact on the conduct of psychotherapy, are also affected by biological factors, including chronobiological factors. States of happiness, sadness, motivation, irritability, anxiety, and pessimism have been described as shifting in circadian patterns and linked to circadian rhythms (3234). Concurrently, irritability and other mood states are clearly affected by hunger, sleepiness, pain, sensitivity to sounds, and other physiological stimuli (35). In addition, biological contributions to psychotherapist temperament (36) and attachment style (37) also have an impact on countertransference reactions.
Additional considerations stem from the fact that patients, as well as psychotherapists, are subject to these factors and that their respective biologies interact. How homeostatic, chronobiologic, and illness-associated factors of psychotherapists and patients align and misalign to influence how psychotherapies are processed is unstudied. Consider how psychotherapists who are bright and eager early in the morning might work with patients who are mentally sluggish—“still asleep”—at those times. Compare these psychotherapy sessions to those in which psychotherapists who are sluggish in the midafternoon might work with patients who are by then fully present and expectant. How might these factors affect alliance and attunement? Studies of functional near-infrared spectroscopy-based hyperscanning using paired fitted skull caps have already demonstrated how teachers and students, basketball players, and other dyads and small groups synchronize neurally around mutual tasks (3840). Similar approaches might be applied to psychotherapy studies as well.

Mitigating Strategies

Several approaches are available to mitigate the above biological factors that may contribute to impeded performance by clinicians. Most obvious is reflection on the part of clinicians to honestly self-assess patterns of shifting alertness during their scheduled psychotherapy hours. Such appraisals require examination of one’s own health, including illnesses and treatments; eating, sleeping, and substance use patterns; personal biorhythms; self-care; and health habits. When in doubt, simple self-assessment tools, such as the Epworth Sleepiness Scale for daytime sleepiness (4143), may reveal otherwise unrecognized difficulties, especially if conducted at multiple times throughout the day. Web-based apps integrated into smartphones and smartwatches are available (44). When problems are identified, clinicians should engage in self-care, obtain supervision to examine how their own psychological contributions interact with biological ones, and seek professional consultation for their own health issues as indicated.
Regarding self-care, psychotherapists may become more aware of the need to keep physically and psychologically fit. They may be able to identify certain hours during the day that should not be scheduled for psychotherapy (or for other demanding work, for that matter), because they are unlikely to be mentally sharp at those times. No doubt, clinicians themselves would not want to be seen by a psychotherapist who felt mentally sluggish, nor would they be willing to accept services provided by mentally sluggish surgeons or other physicians. In addition to attending to their own health needs, psychotherapists may want to respect their personal biorhythms by scheduling brief nap times or “siesta hours” for refreshment (45), assuring availability of snacks for when they are prone to hunger or blood sugar nadirs, taking walks outside the building to recharge, or engaging in other activities that restore mental energy (46).
Finally, to foster clinician well-being and better patient care, health care systems should consider how they might attend to individual clinician’s variable alertness and fatigue rhythms and accommodate schedules accordingly. Some difficulties might be identified via patient satisfaction surveys and patient or peer complaints, by comparing practice patterns across individuals and time of day in electronic records, or via innovative quality improvement initiatives that use self-report (47), smartwatches, and other forms of monitoring (43, 44, 48). Institutions scrutinizing potential performance problems must assure that clinicians are treated with respect and dignity (49). Everyone benefits when clinicians work when they are fresh and refreshed and avoid direct patient care when they are likely to be sluggish.

Implications for Research

These considerations raise numerous questions for further study. To what extent can differences in the quality of psychotherapy (fidelity, patient satisfaction, therapeutic alliance, and outcomes) be associated with clinicians’ cognitive functioning? To what extent can differences in clinicians’ cognitive functioning be linked to the various biological factors enumerated above? What are the best approaches for studying these relationships and teasing apart specific contributing factors? How can individual clinicians and health care systems optimally assure clinician well-being and mitigate possible adverse effects of these factors on psychotherapy outcomes?
Whereas the questions raised in this article invite systematic investigation, numerous anecdotal observations already suggest that biological effects can detract from the quality of psychotherapy. They also suggest that clinicians and health care systems should do what they can to mitigate modifiable adverse influences.

References

1.
Danziger S, Levav J, Avnaim-Pesso L: Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108:6889–6892
2.
Hsiang EY, Mehta SJ, Small DS, et al: Association of primary care clinic appointment time with clinician ordering and patient completion of breast and colorectal cancer screening. JAMA Netw Open 2019; 2:e193403
3.
Addis DR, Moore BA, Garner CR, et al: Case start time affects intraoperative transfusion rates in adult cardiac surgery: a single-center retrospective analysis. J Cardiothorac Vasc Anesth 2020; 34:632–639
4.
Vohs KD, Baumeister RF, Schmeichel BJ, et al: Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. J Pers Soc Psychol 2008; 94:883–898
5.
Fatigue, Sleep Deprivation, and Patient Safety. Rockville, MD, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2019. https://psnet.ahrq.gov/primer/fatigue-sleep-deprivation-and-patient-safety?page=1. Accessed Sept 11, 2020
6.
Kancherla BS, Upender R, Collen JF, et al: Sleep, fatigue and burnout among physicians: an American Academy of Sleep Medicine position statement. J Clin Sleep Med 2020; 16:803–805
7.
Burchiel KJ, Zetterman RK, Ludmerer KM, et al: The 2017 ACGME common work hour standards: promoting physician learning and professional development in a safe, humane environment. J Grad Med Educ 2017; 9:692–696
8.
Schmidt C, Collette F, Cajochen C, et al: A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn Neuropsychol 2007; 24:755–789
9.
Goel N, Basner M, Rao H, et al: Circadian rhythms, sleep deprivation, and human performance. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci 2013; 119:155–190
10.
Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, et al: Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 2006; 23:497–509
11.
Ganesan S, Magee M, Stone JE, et al: The impact of shift work on sleep, alertness and performance in healthcare workers. Sci Rep 2019; 9:4635
12.
Dagan Y, Doljansky JT: Cognitive performance during sustained wakefulness: a low dose of caffeine is equally effective as modafinil in alleviating the nocturnal decline. Chronobiol Int 2006; 23:973–983
13.
Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF: The physiology of willpower: linking blood glucose to self-control. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2007; 11:303–327
14.
Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, et al: Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2010; 136:495–525
15.
Wells AS, Read NW, Idzikowski C, et al: Effects of meals on objective and subjective measures of daytime sleepiness. J Appl Physiol 1998; 84:507–515
16.
Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF III, et al: Circadian rhythms in human performance and mood under constant conditions. J Sleep Res 1997; 6:9–18
17.
Kleitman N, Jackson DP: Body temperature and performance under different routines. J Appl Physiol 1950; 3:309–328
18.
Kaiser D: Infralow frequencies and ultradian rhythms. Semin Pediatr Neurol 2013; 20:242–245
19.
Kleitman N: Basic rest-activity cycle—22 years later. Sleep 1982; 5:311–317
20.
Okawa M, Matousek M, Petersén I: Spontaneous vigilance fluctuations in the daytime. Psychophysiology 1984; 21:207–11.
21.
Kripke DF, Sonnenschein D: A biologic rhythm in waking fantasy; in The Stream of Consciousness. Edited by Pope KS, Singer JL. New York, Plenum Press, 1978
22.
Goh GH, Maloney SK, Mark PJ, et al: Episodic ultradian events—ultradian rhythms. Biology 2019; 8:15
23.
Lavie P, Kripke DF: Ultradian circa 1 1/2 hour rhythms: a multioscillatory system. Life Sci 1981; 29:2445–2450.
24.
Klein R, Armitage R: Rhythms in human performance: 1 1/2-hour oscillations in cognitive style. Science 1979; 204:1326–1328
25.
Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M: Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 2003; 18:80–90
26.
Taillard J, Philip P, Coste O, et al: The circadian and homeostatic modulation of sleep pressure during wakefulness differs between morning and evening chronotypes. J Sleep Res 2003; 12:275–282
27.
Barclay NL, Gregory AM: Quantitative genetic research on sleep: a review of normal sleep, sleep disturbances and associated emotional, behavioural, and health-related difficulties. Sleep Med Rev 2013; 17:29–40
28.
Yoon C, May CP, Hasher L: Aging, circadian arousal patterns, and cognition; in Cognitive Aging: A Primer. Edited by Park D, Schwartz N. London, Psychology Press, 1999
29.
Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF III, et al: Subjective alertness rhythms in elderly people. J Biol Rhythms 1996; 11:268–276
30.
Jaime-Lara RB, Koons BC, Matura LA, et al: A qualitative metasynthesis of the experience of fatigue across five chronic conditions. J Pain Symptom Manage 2020; 59:1320–1343
31.
Moriarty O, McGuire BE, Finn DP: The effect of pain on cognitive function: a review of clinical and preclinical research. Prog Neurobiol 2011; 93:385–404
32.
Boivin DB, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ, et al: Complex interaction of the sleep-wake cycle and circadian phase modulates mood in healthy subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:145–152
33.
Pilz LK, Carissimi A, Francisco AP, et al: Prospective assessment of daily patterns of mood-related symptoms. Front Psychiatry 2018; 9:370
34.
Wefelmeyer T, Kuhs H: Diurnal mood variation in melancholic patients and healthy controls. Psychopathology 1996; 29:184–192
35.
Toohey MJ, DiGiuseppe R: Defining and measuring irritability: construct clarification and differentiation. Clin Psychol Rev 2017; 53:93–108
36.
Cloninger CR, Cloninger KM, Zwir I, et al: The complex genetics and biology of human temperament: a review of traditional concepts in relation to new molecular findings. Transl Psychiatry 2019; 9:290
37.
Perlini C, Bellani M, Rossetti MG, et al: Disentangle the neural correlates of attachment style in healthy individuals. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2019; 28:371–375
38.
Liu J, Zhang R, Geng B, et al: Interplay between prior knowledge and communication mode on teaching effectiveness: interpersonal neural synchronization as a neural marker. Neuroimage 2019; 193:93–102
39.
Li L, Wang H, Luo H, et al: Interpersonal neural synchronization during cooperative behavior of basketball players: a fNIRS-based hyperscanning study. Front Hum Neurosci 2020; 14:169
40.
Nummenmaa L, Lahnakoski JM, Glerean E: Sharing the social world via intersubject neural synchronisation. Curr Opin Psychol 2018; 24:7–14
41.
Jonas DE, Amick HR, Feltner C, et al: Screening for obstructive sleep apnea in adults: evidence report and systematic review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. JAMA 2017; 317:415–433
42.
Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, et al: Quantification of sleepiness: a new approach. Psychophysiology 1973; 10:431–436
43.
Miller CB, Kyle SD, Marshall NS, et al: Ecological momentary assessment of daytime symptoms during sleep restriction therapy for insomnia. J Sleep Res 2013; 22:266–272
44.
Aji M, Glozier N, Bartlett D, et al: A feasibility study of a mobile app to treat insomnia. Transl Behav Med 2020; ibaa019
45.
Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Carrier J, et al: Effects of afternoon “siesta” naps on sleep, alertness, performance, and circadian rhythms in the elderly. Sleep 2001; 24:680–687
46.
Tyler JM, Burns KC: After depletion: the replenishment of the self’s regulatory resources. Self Ident 2008; 7:305–321
47.
Horsley T, Lockyer J, Cogo E, et al: National programmes for validating physician competence and fitness for practice: a scoping review. BMJ Open 2016; 6:e010368
48.
Bioulac S, Taillard J, Philip P, et al: Excessive daytime sleepiness measurements in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11:3
49.
Soonsawat A, Tanaka G, Lammando MA, et al: Cognitively impaired physicians: how do we detect them? How do we assist them? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2018; 26:631–640

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychotherapy
Go to American Journal of Psychotherapy
American Journal of Psychotherapy
Pages: 30 - 35

History

Received: 3 July 2020
Revision received: 8 August 2020
Revision received: 12 September 2020
Accepted: 5 October 2020
Published in print: March 01, 2021
Published online: 15 March 2021

Keywords

  1. psychotherapy process
  2. psychotherapy outcomes
  3. homeostatic factors
  4. circadian rhythms
  5. episodic ultradian rhythms
  6. illness in clinicians

Authors

Details

Joel Yager, M.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Yager ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The author reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - APT - American Journal of Psychotherapy

PPV Articles - APT - American Journal of Psychotherapy

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share